首页 > 最新文献

Behavior Research Methods最新文献

英文 中文
SEMA3: A free smartphone platform for daily life surveys. SEMA3:用于日常生活调查的免费智能手机平台。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02445-w
Sarah T O'Brien, Nerisa Dozo, Jordan D X Hinton, Ella K Moeck, Rio Susanto, Glenn T Jayaputera, Richard O Sinnott, Duy Vu, Mario Alvarez-Jimenez, John Gleeson, Peter Koval

Traditionally, behavioral, social, and health science researchers have relied on global/retrospective survey methods administered cross-sectionally (i.e., on a single occasion) or longitudinally (i.e., on several occasions separated by weeks, months, or years). More recently, social and health scientists have added daily life survey methods (also known as intensive longitudinal methods or ambulatory assessment) to their toolkit. These methods (e.g., daily diaries, experience sampling, ecological momentary assessment) involve dense repeated assessments in everyday settings. To facilitate research using daily life survey methods, we present SEMA3 ( http://www.SEMA3.com ), a platform for designing and administering intensive longitudinal daily life surveys via Android and iOS smartphones. SEMA3 fills an important gap by providing researchers with a free, intuitive, and flexible platform with basic and advanced functionality. In this article, we describe SEMA3's development history and system architecture, provide an overview of how to design a study using SEMA3 and outline its key features, and discuss the platform's limitations and propose directions for future development of SEMA3.

传统上,行为、社会和健康科学研究人员依赖于横截面(即在单一场合)或纵向(即在相隔数周、数月或数年的多个场合)的全球/回顾性调查方法。最近,社会和健康科学家的工具包中又增加了日常生活调查方法(也称为密集纵向方法或流动评估)。这些方法(如日常日记、经验抽样、生态瞬间评估)涉及在日常环境中进行密集的重复评估。为了促进使用日常生活调查方法的研究,我们推出了 SEMA3 ( http://www.SEMA3.com ),这是一个通过安卓和 iOS 智能手机设计和实施密集纵向日常生活调查的平台。SEMA3 为研究人员提供了一个免费、直观、灵活且具有基本和高级功能的平台,填补了一项重要空白。在本文中,我们将介绍 SEMA3 的开发历史和系统架构,概述如何使用 SEMA3 设计研究并概述其主要功能,讨论该平台的局限性并提出 SEMA3 的未来发展方向。
{"title":"SEMA<sup>3</sup>: A free smartphone platform for daily life surveys.","authors":"Sarah T O'Brien, Nerisa Dozo, Jordan D X Hinton, Ella K Moeck, Rio Susanto, Glenn T Jayaputera, Richard O Sinnott, Duy Vu, Mario Alvarez-Jimenez, John Gleeson, Peter Koval","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02445-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02445-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Traditionally, behavioral, social, and health science researchers have relied on global/retrospective survey methods administered cross-sectionally (i.e., on a single occasion) or longitudinally (i.e., on several occasions separated by weeks, months, or years). More recently, social and health scientists have added daily life survey methods (also known as intensive longitudinal methods or ambulatory assessment) to their toolkit. These methods (e.g., daily diaries, experience sampling, ecological momentary assessment) involve dense repeated assessments in everyday settings. To facilitate research using daily life survey methods, we present SEMA<sup>3</sup> ( http://www.SEMA3.com ), a platform for designing and administering intensive longitudinal daily life surveys via Android and iOS smartphones. SEMA<sup>3</sup> fills an important gap by providing researchers with a free, intuitive, and flexible platform with basic and advanced functionality. In this article, we describe SEMA<sup>3</sup>'s development history and system architecture, provide an overview of how to design a study using SEMA<sup>3</sup> and outline its key features, and discuss the platform's limitations and propose directions for future development of SEMA<sup>3</sup>.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362263/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141445376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A jsPsych touchscreen extension for behavioral research on touch-enabled interfaces. 用于触摸界面行为研究的 jsPsych 触摸屏扩展。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02454-9
Younes Strittmatter, Markus W H Spitzer, Nadja Ging-Jehli, Sebastian Musslick

Online experiments are increasingly gaining traction in the behavioral sciences. Despite this, behavioral researchers have largely continued to use keyboards as the primary input devices for such online studies, overlooking the ubiquity of touchscreens in everyday use. This paper presents an open-source touchscreen extension for jsPsych, a JavaScript framework designed for conducting online experiments. We additionally evaluated the touchscreen extension assessing whether typical behavioral findings from two distinct perceptual decision-making tasks - the random-dot kinematogram and the Stroop task - can similarly be observed when administered via touchscreen devices compared to keyboard devices. Our findings indicate similar performance metrics for each paradigm between the touchscreen and keyboard versions of the experiments. Specifically, we observe similar psychometric curves in the random-dot kinematogram across the touchscreen and keyboard versions. Similarly, in the Stroop task, we detect significant task, congruency, and sequential congruency effects in both experiment versions. We conclude that our open-source touchscreen extension serves as a promising tool for data collection in online behavioral experiments on forced-choice tasks.

在线实验在行为科学领域日益受到重视。尽管如此,行为研究人员在很大程度上仍将键盘作为此类在线研究的主要输入设备,而忽视了日常使用中无处不在的触摸屏。本文介绍了 jsPsych 的开源触摸屏扩展,这是一个专为开展在线实验而设计的 JavaScript 框架。此外,我们还对触摸屏扩展进行了评估,评估通过触摸屏设备与键盘设备相比,是否能从随机点运动图和 Stroop 任务这两个不同的感知决策任务中观察到类似的典型行为结果。我们的研究结果表明,在触摸屏和键盘版本的实验中,每个范例的性能指标相似。具体来说,我们观察到触摸屏和键盘版随机点运动图的心理测量曲线相似。同样,在 Stroop 任务中,我们在两个实验版本中都检测到了显著的任务效应、一致性效应和连续一致性效应。最后,我们得出结论:我们的开源触摸屏扩展是一种很有前途的工具,可用于强迫选择任务在线行为实验的数据收集。
{"title":"A jsPsych touchscreen extension for behavioral research on touch-enabled interfaces.","authors":"Younes Strittmatter, Markus W H Spitzer, Nadja Ging-Jehli, Sebastian Musslick","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02454-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02454-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Online experiments are increasingly gaining traction in the behavioral sciences. Despite this, behavioral researchers have largely continued to use keyboards as the primary input devices for such online studies, overlooking the ubiquity of touchscreens in everyday use. This paper presents an open-source touchscreen extension for jsPsych, a JavaScript framework designed for conducting online experiments. We additionally evaluated the touchscreen extension assessing whether typical behavioral findings from two distinct perceptual decision-making tasks - the random-dot kinematogram and the Stroop task - can similarly be observed when administered via touchscreen devices compared to keyboard devices. Our findings indicate similar performance metrics for each paradigm between the touchscreen and keyboard versions of the experiments. Specifically, we observe similar psychometric curves in the random-dot kinematogram across the touchscreen and keyboard versions. Similarly, in the Stroop task, we detect significant task, congruency, and sequential congruency effects in both experiment versions. We conclude that our open-source touchscreen extension serves as a promising tool for data collection in online behavioral experiments on forced-choice tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141589532","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Ambiguous Cue Task: Measurement reliability of an experimental paradigm for the assessment of interpretation bias and associations with mental health. 模糊线索任务:用于评估解释偏差及与心理健康关系的实验范式的测量可靠性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02451-y
Diana J N Armbruster-Genç, Rebecca A Rammensee, Stefanie M Jungmann, Philine Drake, Michèle Wessa, Ulrike Basten

Interpretation biases in the processing of ambiguous affective information are assumed to play an important role in the onset and maintenance of emotional disorders. Reports of low reliability for experimental measures of cognitive biases have called into question previous findings on the association of these measures with markers of mental health and demonstrated the need to systematically evaluate measurement reliability for measures of cognitive biases. We evaluated reliability and correlations with self-report measures of mental health for interpretation bias scores derived from the Ambiguous Cue Task (ACT), an experimental paradigm for the assessment of approach-avoidance behavior towards ambiguous affective stimuli. For a non-clinical sample, the measurement of an interpretation bias with the ACT showed high internal consistency (rSB = .91 - .96, N = 354) and acceptable 2-week test-retest correlations (rPearson = .61 - .65, n = 109). Correlations between the ACT interpretation bias scores and mental health-related self-report measures of personality and well-being were generally small (r ≤ |.11|) and statistically not significant when correcting for multiple comparisons. These findings suggest that in non-clinical populations, individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous affective information as assessed with the ACT do not show a clear association with self-report markers of mental health. However, in allowing for a highly reliable measurement of interpretation bias, the ACT provides a valuable tool for studies considering potentially small effect sizes in non-clinical populations by studying bigger samples as well as for work on clinical populations, for which potentially greater effects can be expected.

在处理模棱两可的情感信息时出现的解释偏差被认为在情感障碍的发生和维持中起着重要作用。有报告称,认知偏差实验测量的可靠性较低,这使人们对之前关于这些测量与心理健康指标之间联系的研究结果产生了质疑,并表明有必要对认知偏差测量的测量可靠性进行系统评估。我们评估了 "模糊线索任务"(ACT)得出的解释偏差分数与心理健康自我报告测量的可靠性和相关性。在非临床样本中,ACT 对解释偏差的测量显示出较高的内部一致性(rSB = .91 - .96, n = 354)和可接受的两周测试-再测相关性(rPearson = .61 - .65, n = 109)。ACT 解释偏差得分与心理健康相关的人格和幸福感自我报告测量之间的相关性一般较小(r ≤ |.11|),并且在进行多重比较校正后,相关性在统计学上并不显著。这些研究结果表明,在非临床人群中,ACT 评估的模糊情感信息解释的个体差异与心理健康自我报告指标并没有明显的关联。然而,由于 ACT 可以对解释偏差进行高度可靠的测量,因此它为通过研究更大样本来考虑非临床人群中可能较小的效应大小的研究以及临床人群的研究提供了一种有价值的工具,因为临床人群的效应可能会更大。
{"title":"The Ambiguous Cue Task: Measurement reliability of an experimental paradigm for the assessment of interpretation bias and associations with mental health.","authors":"Diana J N Armbruster-Genç, Rebecca A Rammensee, Stefanie M Jungmann, Philine Drake, Michèle Wessa, Ulrike Basten","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02451-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02451-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interpretation biases in the processing of ambiguous affective information are assumed to play an important role in the onset and maintenance of emotional disorders. Reports of low reliability for experimental measures of cognitive biases have called into question previous findings on the association of these measures with markers of mental health and demonstrated the need to systematically evaluate measurement reliability for measures of cognitive biases. We evaluated reliability and correlations with self-report measures of mental health for interpretation bias scores derived from the Ambiguous Cue Task (ACT), an experimental paradigm for the assessment of approach-avoidance behavior towards ambiguous affective stimuli. For a non-clinical sample, the measurement of an interpretation bias with the ACT showed high internal consistency (r<sub>SB</sub> = .91 - .96, N = 354) and acceptable 2-week test-retest correlations (r<sub>Pearson</sub> = .61 - .65, n = 109). Correlations between the ACT interpretation bias scores and mental health-related self-report measures of personality and well-being were generally small (r ≤ |.11|) and statistically not significant when correcting for multiple comparisons. These findings suggest that in non-clinical populations, individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous affective information as assessed with the ACT do not show a clear association with self-report markers of mental health. However, in allowing for a highly reliable measurement of interpretation bias, the ACT provides a valuable tool for studies considering potentially small effect sizes in non-clinical populations by studying bigger samples as well as for work on clinical populations, for which potentially greater effects can be expected.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362423/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141589533","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Many nonnormalities, one simulation: Do different data generation algorithms affect study results? 许多非正态性,一次模拟:不同的数据生成算法会影响研究结果吗?
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02364-w
Amanda J Fairchild, Yunhang Yin, Amanda N Baraldi, Oscar L Olvera Astivia, Dexin Shi

Monte Carlo simulation studies are among the primary scientific outputs contributed by methodologists, guiding application of various statistical tools in practice. Although methodological researchers routinely extend simulation study findings through follow-up work, few studies are ever replicated. Simulation studies are susceptible to factors that can contribute to replicability failures, however. This paper sought to conduct a meta-scientific study by replicating one highly cited simulation study (Curran et al., Psychological Methods, 1, 16-29, 1996) that investigated the robustness of normal theory maximum likelihood (ML)-based chi-square fit statistics under multivariate nonnormality. We further examined the generalizability of the original study findings across different nonnormal data generation algorithms. Our replication results were generally consistent with original findings, but we discerned several differences. Our generalizability results were more mixed. Only two results observed under the original data generation algorithm held completely across other algorithms examined. One of the most striking findings we observed was that results associated with the independent generator (IG) data generation algorithm vastly differed from other procedures examined and suggested that ML was robust to nonnormality for the particular factor model used in the simulation. Findings point to the reality that extant methodological recommendations may not be universally valid in contexts where multiple data generation algorithms exist for a given data characteristic. We recommend that researchers consider multiple approaches to generating a specific data or model characteristic (when more than one is available) to optimize the generalizability of simulation results.

蒙特卡罗模拟研究是方法论研究人员的主要科学成果之一,指导着各种统计工具在实践中的应用。尽管方法论研究人员经常通过后续工作扩展模拟研究结果,但很少有研究是重复的。然而,模拟研究很容易受到一些因素的影响,导致复制失败。本文试图通过复制一项被高度引用的模拟研究(Curran 等人,《心理学方法》,1, 16-29, 1996 年)来开展一项元科学研究,该研究调查了基于正态理论最大似然法 (ML) 的卡方拟合统计量在多元非正态性下的稳健性。我们进一步检验了原始研究结果在不同的非正态数据生成算法中的通用性。我们的复制结果与原始研究结果基本一致,但也发现了一些不同之处。我们的可推广性结果喜忧参半。只有两个在原始数据生成算法下观察到的结果在其他算法下完全成立。我们观察到的最惊人的发现之一是,与独立生成器(IG)数据生成算法相关的结果与所研究的其他程序大不相同,这表明 ML 对模拟中使用的特定因子模型的非正态性具有稳健性。研究结果表明,在特定数据特征存在多种数据生成算法的情况下,现有的方法建议可能并不普遍有效。我们建议研究人员考虑采用多种方法生成特定的数据或模型特征(当有多种方法可用时),以优化模拟结果的可推广性。
{"title":"Many nonnormalities, one simulation: Do different data generation algorithms affect study results?","authors":"Amanda J Fairchild, Yunhang Yin, Amanda N Baraldi, Oscar L Olvera Astivia, Dexin Shi","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02364-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02364-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Monte Carlo simulation studies are among the primary scientific outputs contributed by methodologists, guiding application of various statistical tools in practice. Although methodological researchers routinely extend simulation study findings through follow-up work, few studies are ever replicated. Simulation studies are susceptible to factors that can contribute to replicability failures, however. This paper sought to conduct a meta-scientific study by replicating one highly cited simulation study (Curran et al., Psychological Methods, 1, 16-29, 1996) that investigated the robustness of normal theory maximum likelihood (ML)-based chi-square fit statistics under multivariate nonnormality. We further examined the generalizability of the original study findings across different nonnormal data generation algorithms. Our replication results were generally consistent with original findings, but we discerned several differences. Our generalizability results were more mixed. Only two results observed under the original data generation algorithm held completely across other algorithms examined. One of the most striking findings we observed was that results associated with the independent generator (IG) data generation algorithm vastly differed from other procedures examined and suggested that ML was robust to nonnormality for the particular factor model used in the simulation. Findings point to the reality that extant methodological recommendations may not be universally valid in contexts where multiple data generation algorithms exist for a given data characteristic. We recommend that researchers consider multiple approaches to generating a specific data or model characteristic (when more than one is available) to optimize the generalizability of simulation results.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139929818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dealing with missing data in multi-informant studies: A comparison of approaches. 处理多信息研究中的缺失数据:方法比较。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02367-7
Po-Yi Chen, Fan Jia, Wei Wu, Min-Heng Wang, Tzi-Yang Chao

Multi-informant studies are popular in social and behavioral science. However, their data analyses are challenging because data from different informants carry both shared and unique information and are often incomplete. Using Monte Carlo Simulation, the current study compares three approaches that can be used to analyze incomplete multi-informant data when there is a distinction between reference and nonreference informants. These approaches include a two-method measurement model for planned missing data (2MM-PMD), treating nonreference informants' reports as auxiliary variables with the full-information maximum likelihood method or multiple imputation, and listwise deletion. The result suggests that 2MM-PMD, when correctly specified and data are missing at random, has the best overall performance among the examined approaches regarding point estimates, type I error rates, and statistical power. In addition, it is also more robust to data that are not missing at random.

多信息提供者研究在社会和行为科学领域很受欢迎。然而,由于来自不同信息提供者的数据既有共享信息,也有独特信息,而且往往不完整,因此其数据分析具有挑战性。本研究使用蒙特卡洛模拟(Monte Carlo Simulation),比较了在参考线人和非参考线人之间存在区别时,可用于分析不完整的多线人数据的三种方法。这些方法包括计划缺失数据的双方法测量模型(2MM-PMD)、用全信息最大似然法或多重估算法将非参考信息提供者的报告视为辅助变量,以及列表删除法。结果表明,2MM-PMD 在指定正确且数据随机缺失的情况下,在点估计、I 型误差率和统计能力方面的总体表现是所研究方法中最好的。此外,它对非随机缺失数据也更稳健。
{"title":"Dealing with missing data in multi-informant studies: A comparison of approaches.","authors":"Po-Yi Chen, Fan Jia, Wei Wu, Min-Heng Wang, Tzi-Yang Chao","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02367-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02367-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multi-informant studies are popular in social and behavioral science. However, their data analyses are challenging because data from different informants carry both shared and unique information and are often incomplete. Using Monte Carlo Simulation, the current study compares three approaches that can be used to analyze incomplete multi-informant data when there is a distinction between reference and nonreference informants. These approaches include a two-method measurement model for planned missing data (2MM-PMD), treating nonreference informants' reports as auxiliary variables with the full-information maximum likelihood method or multiple imputation, and listwise deletion. The result suggests that 2MM-PMD, when correctly specified and data are missing at random, has the best overall performance among the examined approaches regarding point estimates, type I error rates, and statistical power. In addition, it is also more robust to data that are not missing at random.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139989187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The discrepancy in timing between synchronous signals and visual stimulation should not be underestimated. 同步信号和视觉刺激在时间上的差异不容小觑。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02382-8
Biao Chen, Junjie Bu, Xu Jiang, Ping Wang, Yan Xie, Zhuoyun Wang, Zhen Liang, Shengzhao Zhang

Response latency is a critical parameter in studying human behavior, representing the time interval between the onset of stimulus and the response. However, different time between devices can introduce errors. Serial port synchronization signal can mitigate this, but limited information is available regarding their accuracy. Optical signals offer another option, but the difference in the positioning of optical signals and visual stimuli can introduce errors, and there have been limited reports of error reduction. This study aims to investigate methods for reducing the time errors. We used the Psychtoolbox to generate visual stimuli and serial port synchronization signals to explore their accuracy. Subsequently, we proposed a calibration formula to minimize the error between optical signals and visual stimuli. The findings are as follows: Firstly, the serial port synchronization signal presenting precedes visual stimulation, with a smaller lead time observed at higher refresh rates. Secondly, the lead time increases as the stimulus position deviates to the right and downwards. In Linux and IOPort(), serial port synchronization signals exhibited greater accuracy. Considering the poor accuracy and the multiple influencing factors associated with serial port synchronization signals, it is recommended to use optical signals to complete time synchronization. The results indicate that under the darkening process, the time error is - 0.23 ~ 0.08 ms (mean). This calibration formula can help measure the response latency accurately. This study provides valuable insights for optimizing experimental design and improving the accuracy of response latency. Although it only involves visual stimuli, the methods and results of this study can still serve as a reference.

反应潜伏期是研究人类行为的一个关键参数,它代表了从刺激开始到做出反应之间的时间间隔。然而,设备之间的时间不同会带来误差。串行端口同步信号可以缓解这一问题,但有关其准确性的信息有限。光学信号提供了另一种选择,但光学信号和视觉刺激在定位上的差异可能会带来误差,而且有关减少误差的报道也很有限。本研究旨在探讨减少时间误差的方法。我们使用 Psychtoolbox 生成视觉刺激和串行端口同步信号,以探索其准确性。随后,我们提出了一个校准公式,以尽量减小光学信号和视觉刺激之间的误差。研究结果如下:首先,串行端口同步信号先于视觉刺激出现,刷新率越高,前导时间越短。其次,随着刺激位置向右和向下偏移,前导时间也会增加。在 Linux 和 IOPort() 中,串行端口同步信号表现出更高的精确度。考虑到串行端口同步信号的准确性较差且影响因素较多,建议使用光信号来完成时间同步。结果表明,在暗化过程中,时间误差为 - 0.23 ~ 0.08 ms(平均值)。这一校准公式有助于精确测量响应延迟。这项研究为优化实验设计和提高反应潜伏期的准确性提供了宝贵的启示。虽然只涉及视觉刺激,但本研究的方法和结果仍可作为参考。
{"title":"The discrepancy in timing between synchronous signals and visual stimulation should not be underestimated.","authors":"Biao Chen, Junjie Bu, Xu Jiang, Ping Wang, Yan Xie, Zhuoyun Wang, Zhen Liang, Shengzhao Zhang","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02382-8","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02382-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Response latency is a critical parameter in studying human behavior, representing the time interval between the onset of stimulus and the response. However, different time between devices can introduce errors. Serial port synchronization signal can mitigate this, but limited information is available regarding their accuracy. Optical signals offer another option, but the difference in the positioning of optical signals and visual stimuli can introduce errors, and there have been limited reports of error reduction. This study aims to investigate methods for reducing the time errors. We used the Psychtoolbox to generate visual stimuli and serial port synchronization signals to explore their accuracy. Subsequently, we proposed a calibration formula to minimize the error between optical signals and visual stimuli. The findings are as follows: Firstly, the serial port synchronization signal presenting precedes visual stimulation, with a smaller lead time observed at higher refresh rates. Secondly, the lead time increases as the stimulus position deviates to the right and downwards. In Linux and IOPort(), serial port synchronization signals exhibited greater accuracy. Considering the poor accuracy and the multiple influencing factors associated with serial port synchronization signals, it is recommended to use optical signals to complete time synchronization. The results indicate that under the darkening process, the time error is - 0.23 ~ 0.08 ms (mean). This calibration formula can help measure the response latency accurately. This study provides valuable insights for optimizing experimental design and improving the accuracy of response latency. Although it only involves visual stimuli, the methods and results of this study can still serve as a reference.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140130648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A new sample-size planning approach for person-specific VAR(1) studies: Predictive accuracy analysis. 针对特定人群的 VAR(1) 研究的新样本大小规划方法:预测准确性分析。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02413-4
Jordan Revol, Ginette Lafit, Eva Ceulemans

Researchers increasingly study short-term dynamic processes that evolve within single individuals using N = 1 studies. The processes of interest are typically captured by fitting a VAR(1) model to the resulting data. A crucial question is how to perform sample-size planning and thus decide on the number of measurement occasions that are needed. The most popular approach is to perform a power analysis, which focuses on detecting the effects of interest. We argue that performing sample-size planning based on out-of-sample predictive accuracy yields additional important information regarding potential overfitting of the model. Predictive accuracy quantifies how well the estimated VAR(1) model will allow predicting unseen data from the same individual. We propose a new simulation-based sample-size planning method called predictive accuracy analysis (PAA), and an associated Shiny app. This approach makes use of a novel predictive accuracy metric that accounts for the multivariate nature of the prediction problem. We showcase how the values of the different VAR(1) model parameters impact power and predictive accuracy-based sample-size recommendations using simulated data sets and real data applications. The range of recommended sample sizes is smaller for predictive accuracy analysis than for power analysis.

研究人员越来越多地使用 N = 1 研究方法来研究单个个体内部演变的短期动态过程。通常是通过对所得数据拟合 VAR(1) 模型来捕捉感兴趣的过程。一个关键问题是如何进行样本大小规划,从而决定所需的测量次数。最常用的方法是进行幂次分析,重点是检测感兴趣的效应。我们认为,根据样本外预测准确性来进行样本大小规划,可以获得更多有关模型潜在过拟合的重要信息。预测准确度量化了估计的 VAR(1) 模型在预测同一个体的未见数据方面的效果。我们提出了一种新的基于模拟的样本大小规划方法,称为预测准确性分析(PAA),以及相关的 Shiny 应用程序。这种方法使用了一种新的预测准确度指标,该指标考虑到了预测问题的多变量性质。我们利用模拟数据集和真实数据应用,展示了不同 VAR(1) 模型参数值如何影响功率和基于预测准确度的样本大小建议。与功率分析相比,预测准确度分析推荐的样本大小范围较小。
{"title":"A new sample-size planning approach for person-specific VAR(1) studies: Predictive accuracy analysis.","authors":"Jordan Revol, Ginette Lafit, Eva Ceulemans","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02413-4","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02413-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers increasingly study short-term dynamic processes that evolve within single individuals using N = 1 studies. The processes of interest are typically captured by fitting a VAR(1) model to the resulting data. A crucial question is how to perform sample-size planning and thus decide on the number of measurement occasions that are needed. The most popular approach is to perform a power analysis, which focuses on detecting the effects of interest. We argue that performing sample-size planning based on out-of-sample predictive accuracy yields additional important information regarding potential overfitting of the model. Predictive accuracy quantifies how well the estimated VAR(1) model will allow predicting unseen data from the same individual. We propose a new simulation-based sample-size planning method called predictive accuracy analysis (PAA), and an associated Shiny app. This approach makes use of a novel predictive accuracy metric that accounts for the multivariate nature of the prediction problem. We showcase how the values of the different VAR(1) model parameters impact power and predictive accuracy-based sample-size recommendations using simulated data sets and real data applications. The range of recommended sample sizes is smaller for predictive accuracy analysis than for power analysis.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140875721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistency within change: Evaluating the psychometric properties of a widely used predictive-inference task. 变化中的一致性:评估一项广泛使用的预测推理任务的心理测量特性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02427-y
Alisa M Loosen, Tricia X F Seow, Tobias U Hauser

Rapid adaptation to sudden changes in the environment is a hallmark of flexible human behaviour. Many computational, neuroimaging, and even clinical investigations studying this cognitive process have relied on a behavioural paradigm known as the predictive-inference task. However, the psychometric quality of this task has never been examined, leaving unanswered whether it is indeed suited to capture behavioural variation on a within- and between-subject level. Using a large-scale test-retest design (T1: N = 330; T2: N = 219), we assessed the internal (internal consistency) and temporal (test-retest reliability) stability of the task's most used measures. We show that the main measures capturing flexible belief and behavioural adaptation yield good internal consistency and overall satisfying test-retest reliability. However, some more complex markers of flexible behaviour show lower psychometric quality. Our findings have implications for the large corpus of previous studies using this task and provide clear guidance as to which measures should and should not be used in future studies.

快速适应环境的突然变化是人类灵活行为的标志。许多研究这一认知过程的计算、神经影像甚至临床研究都依赖于一种行为范式,即预测推理任务。然而,这项任务的心理测量学质量却从未被考察过,因此它是否真的适合捕捉受试者内部和受试者之间的行为变异也就成了未知数。通过大规模的重复测试设计(T1:N = 330;T2:N = 219),我们评估了该任务最常用测量指标的内部(内部一致性)和时间(重复测试可靠性)稳定性。我们发现,捕捉灵活信念和行为适应性的主要测量指标具有良好的内部一致性和总体令人满意的重测可靠性。然而,一些更复杂的灵活行为指标显示出较低的心理测量质量。我们的研究结果对以往使用该任务进行的大量研究产生了影响,并为未来研究中应该和不应该使用哪些测量方法提供了明确的指导。
{"title":"Consistency within change: Evaluating the psychometric properties of a widely used predictive-inference task.","authors":"Alisa M Loosen, Tricia X F Seow, Tobias U Hauser","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02427-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02427-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rapid adaptation to sudden changes in the environment is a hallmark of flexible human behaviour. Many computational, neuroimaging, and even clinical investigations studying this cognitive process have relied on a behavioural paradigm known as the predictive-inference task. However, the psychometric quality of this task has never been examined, leaving unanswered whether it is indeed suited to capture behavioural variation on a within- and between-subject level. Using a large-scale test-retest design (T1: N = 330; T2: N = 219), we assessed the internal (internal consistency) and temporal (test-retest reliability) stability of the task's most used measures. We show that the main measures capturing flexible belief and behavioural adaptation yield good internal consistency and overall satisfying test-retest reliability. However, some more complex markers of flexible behaviour show lower psychometric quality. Our findings have implications for the large corpus of previous studies using this task and provide clear guidance as to which measures should and should not be used in future studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362202/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141282840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Geofencing in location-based behavioral research: Methodology, challenges, and implementation. 基于位置的行为研究中的地理围栏:方法、挑战和实施。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-25 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02213-2
Yury Shevchenko, Ulf-Dietrich Reips

This manuscript presents a novel geofencing method in behavioral research. Geofencing, built upon geolocation technology, constitutes virtual fences around specific locations. Every time a participant crosses the virtual border around the geofenced area, an event can be triggered on a smartphone, e.g., the participant may be asked to complete a survey. The geofencing method can alleviate the problems of constant location tracking, such as recording sensitive geolocation information and battery drain. In scenarios where locations for geofencing are determined by participants (e.g., home, workplace), no location data need to be transferred to the researcher, so this method can ensure privacy and anonymity. Given the widespread use of smartphones and mobile Internet, geofencing has become a feasible tool in studying human behavior and cognition outside of the laboratory. The method can help advance theoretical and applied psychological science at a new frontier of context-aware research. At the same time, there is a lack of guidance on how and when geofencing can be applied in research. This manuscript aims to fill the gap and ease the adoption of the geofencing method. We describe the current challenges and implementations in geofencing and present three empirical studies in which we evaluated the geofencing method using the Samply application, a tool for mobile experience sampling research. The studies show that sensitivity and precision of geofencing were affected by the type of event, location radius, environment, operating system, and user behavior. Potential implications and recommendations for behavioral research are discussed.

本手稿介绍了一种用于行为研究的新型地理围栏方法。地理围栏建立在地理定位技术的基础上,在特定地点周围形成虚拟围栏。每当参与者越过地理围栏周围的虚拟边界,智能手机上就会触发一个事件,例如,参与者可能会被要求完成一项调查。地理围栏方法可以缓解持续位置跟踪带来的问题,如记录敏感的地理位置信息和电池消耗。在地理围栏的位置由参与者确定的情况下(如家庭、工作场所),无需向研究人员传输位置数据,因此这种方法可以确保隐私和匿名性。鉴于智能手机和移动互联网的广泛使用,地理围栏已成为在实验室外研究人类行为和认知的可行工具。这种方法有助于在情境感知研究的新领域推动理论和应用心理科学的发展。与此同时,在如何以及何时将地理围栏应用于研究方面还缺乏指导。本手稿旨在填补这一空白,促进地理围栏方法的应用。我们描述了地理围栏目前面临的挑战和实施情况,并介绍了三项实证研究,在这些研究中,我们使用移动体验采样研究工具 Samply 应用程序对地理围栏方法进行了评估。研究表明,地理围栏的灵敏度和精确度受事件类型、位置半径、环境、操作系统和用户行为的影响。研究还讨论了对行为研究的潜在影响和建议。
{"title":"Geofencing in location-based behavioral research: Methodology, challenges, and implementation.","authors":"Yury Shevchenko, Ulf-Dietrich Reips","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02213-2","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02213-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This manuscript presents a novel geofencing method in behavioral research. Geofencing, built upon geolocation technology, constitutes virtual fences around specific locations. Every time a participant crosses the virtual border around the geofenced area, an event can be triggered on a smartphone, e.g., the participant may be asked to complete a survey. The geofencing method can alleviate the problems of constant location tracking, such as recording sensitive geolocation information and battery drain. In scenarios where locations for geofencing are determined by participants (e.g., home, workplace), no location data need to be transferred to the researcher, so this method can ensure privacy and anonymity. Given the widespread use of smartphones and mobile Internet, geofencing has become a feasible tool in studying human behavior and cognition outside of the laboratory. The method can help advance theoretical and applied psychological science at a new frontier of context-aware research. At the same time, there is a lack of guidance on how and when geofencing can be applied in research. This manuscript aims to fill the gap and ease the adoption of the geofencing method. We describe the current challenges and implementations in geofencing and present three empirical studies in which we evaluated the geofencing method using the Samply application, a tool for mobile experience sampling research. The studies show that sensitivity and precision of geofencing were affected by the type of event, location radius, environment, operating system, and user behavior. Potential implications and recommendations for behavioral research are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362315/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10428016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Validating the IDRIS and IDRIA: Two infrequency/frequency scales for detecting careless and insufficient effort survey responders. 验证 IDRIS 和 IDRIA:用于检测粗心和不充分努力的调查应答者的两种不频繁/频繁量表。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02452-x
Cameron S Kay

To detect careless and insufficient effort (C/IE) survey responders, researchers can use infrequency items - items that almost no one agrees with (e.g., "When a friend greets me, I generally try to say nothing back") - and frequency items - items that almost everyone agrees with (e.g., "I try to listen when someone I care about is telling me something"). Here, we provide initial validation for two sets of these items: the 14-item Invalid Responding Inventory for Statements (IDRIS) and the 6-item Invalid Responding Inventory for Adjectives (IDRIA). Across six studies (N1 = 536; N2 = 701; N3 = 500; N4 = 499; N5 = 629, N6 = 562), we found consistent evidence that the IDRIS is capable of detecting C/IE responding among statement-based scales (e.g., the HEXACO-PI-R) and the IDRIA is capable of detecting C/IE responding among both adjective-based scales (e.g., the Lex-20) and adjective-derived scales (e.g., the BFI-2). These findings were robust across different analytic approaches (e.g., Pearson correlations; Spearman rank-order correlations), different indices of C/IE responding (e.g., person-total correlations; semantic synonyms; horizontal cursor variability), and different sample types (e.g., US undergraduate students; Nigerian survey panel participants). Taken together, these results provide promising evidence for the utility of the IDRIS and IDRIA in detecting C/IE responding.

为了检测粗心和不够努力(C/IE)的调查回答者,研究人员可以使用低频项目--几乎没有人同意的项目(例如,"当朋友向我打招呼时,我一般尽量不回话")和高频项目--几乎每个人都同意的项目(例如,"当我关心的人告诉我一些事情时,我会尽量倾听")。在此,我们对这两组项目进行了初步验证:14 个项目的 "陈述句无效反应量表"(IDRIS)和 6 个项目的 "形容词无效反应量表"(IDRIA)。在六项研究(N1 = 536;N2 = 701;N3 = 500;N4 = 499;N5 = 629,N6 = 562)中,我们发现了一致的证据,即 IDRIS 能够在基于陈述的量表(如 HEXACO-PI-R)中检测出 C/IE 反应,而 IDRIA 能够在基于形容词的量表(如 Lex-20)和形容词衍生量表(如 BFI-2)中检测出 C/IE 反应。这些结果在不同的分析方法(如皮尔逊相关;斯皮尔曼秩序相关)、不同的C/IE反应指数(如人-总相关;语义同义词;水平光标变异性)和不同的样本类型(如美国本科生;尼日利亚调查小组参与者)中都是稳健的。总之,这些结果为 IDRIS 和 IDRIA 在检测 C/IE 反应方面的实用性提供了有希望的证据。
{"title":"Validating the IDRIS and IDRIA: Two infrequency/frequency scales for detecting careless and insufficient effort survey responders.","authors":"Cameron S Kay","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02452-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02452-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To detect careless and insufficient effort (C/IE) survey responders, researchers can use infrequency items - items that almost no one agrees with (e.g., \"When a friend greets me, I generally try to say nothing back\") - and frequency items - items that almost everyone agrees with (e.g., \"I try to listen when someone I care about is telling me something\"). Here, we provide initial validation for two sets of these items: the 14-item Invalid Responding Inventory for Statements (IDRIS) and the 6-item Invalid Responding Inventory for Adjectives (IDRIA). Across six studies (N<sub>1</sub> = 536; N<sub>2</sub> = 701; N<sub>3</sub> = 500; N<sub>4</sub> = 499; N<sub>5</sub> = 629, N<sub>6</sub> = 562), we found consistent evidence that the IDRIS is capable of detecting C/IE responding among statement-based scales (e.g., the HEXACO-PI-R) and the IDRIA is capable of detecting C/IE responding among both adjective-based scales (e.g., the Lex-20) and adjective-derived scales (e.g., the BFI-2). These findings were robust across different analytic approaches (e.g., Pearson correlations; Spearman rank-order correlations), different indices of C/IE responding (e.g., person-total correlations; semantic synonyms; horizontal cursor variability), and different sample types (e.g., US undergraduate students; Nigerian survey panel participants). Taken together, these results provide promising evidence for the utility of the IDRIS and IDRIA in detecting C/IE responding.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141557942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavior Research Methods
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1