首页 > 最新文献

Behavior Research Methods最新文献

英文 中文
Temporal stability of Bayesian belief updating in perceptual decision-making. 感知决策中贝叶斯信念更新的时间稳定性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02306-y
Isabella Goodwin, Robert Hester, Marta I Garrido

Bayesian inference suggests that perception is inferred from a weighted integration of prior contextual beliefs with current sensory evidence (likelihood) about the world around us. The perceived precision or uncertainty associated with prior and likelihood information is used to guide perceptual decision-making, such that more weight is placed on the source of information with greater precision. This provides a framework for understanding a spectrum of clinical transdiagnostic symptoms associated with aberrant perception, as well as individual differences in the general population. While behavioral paradigms are commonly used to characterize individual differences in perception as a stable characteristic, measurement reliability in these behavioral tasks is rarely assessed. To remedy this gap, we empirically evaluate the reliability of a perceptual decision-making task that quantifies individual differences in Bayesian belief updating in terms of the relative precision weighting afforded to prior and likelihood information (i.e., sensory weight). We analyzed data from participants (n = 37) who performed this task twice. We found that the precision afforded to prior and likelihood information showed high internal consistency and good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.73, 95% CI [0.53, 0.85]) when averaged across participants, as well as at the individual level using hierarchical modeling. Our results provide support for the assumption that Bayesian belief updating operates as a stable characteristic in perceptual decision-making. We discuss the utility and applicability of reliable perceptual decision-making paradigms as a measure of individual differences in the general population, as well as a diagnostic tool in psychiatric research.

贝叶斯推断法认为,感知是通过对我们周围世界的先验环境信念与当前感官证据(可能性)进行加权整合而推断出来的。与先验信息和可能性信息相关的感知精确度或不确定性被用来指导感知决策,因此,精确度更高的信息源会受到更多重视。这为理解与异常感知相关的一系列临床跨诊断症状以及普通人群的个体差异提供了一个框架。虽然行为范式通常被用来描述个体差异的感知稳定特征,但这些行为任务的测量可靠性却很少得到评估。为了弥补这一缺陷,我们通过经验评估了知觉决策任务的可靠性,该任务通过对先验信息和可能性信息(即感觉权重)的相对精确加权来量化贝叶斯信念更新的个体差异。我们分析了两次完成这项任务的参与者(37 人)的数据。我们发现,先验信息和可能性信息的精确度在对参与者进行平均时,以及在使用层次模型进行个体水平分析时,显示出较高的内部一致性和良好的测试-再测试可靠性(ICC = 0.73, 95% CI [0.53, 0.85])。我们的结果为贝叶斯信念更新作为感知决策中的稳定特征这一假设提供了支持。我们讨论了可靠的感知决策范式作为普通人群个体差异测量方法以及精神病学研究诊断工具的实用性和适用性。
{"title":"Temporal stability of Bayesian belief updating in perceptual decision-making.","authors":"Isabella Goodwin, Robert Hester, Marta I Garrido","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02306-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02306-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Bayesian inference suggests that perception is inferred from a weighted integration of prior contextual beliefs with current sensory evidence (likelihood) about the world around us. The perceived precision or uncertainty associated with prior and likelihood information is used to guide perceptual decision-making, such that more weight is placed on the source of information with greater precision. This provides a framework for understanding a spectrum of clinical transdiagnostic symptoms associated with aberrant perception, as well as individual differences in the general population. While behavioral paradigms are commonly used to characterize individual differences in perception as a stable characteristic, measurement reliability in these behavioral tasks is rarely assessed. To remedy this gap, we empirically evaluate the reliability of a perceptual decision-making task that quantifies individual differences in Bayesian belief updating in terms of the relative precision weighting afforded to prior and likelihood information (i.e., sensory weight). We analyzed data from participants (n = 37) who performed this task twice. We found that the precision afforded to prior and likelihood information showed high internal consistency and good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.73, 95% CI [0.53, 0.85]) when averaged across participants, as well as at the individual level using hierarchical modeling. Our results provide support for the assumption that Bayesian belief updating operates as a stable characteristic in perceptual decision-making. We discuss the utility and applicability of reliable perceptual decision-making paradigms as a measure of individual differences in the general population, as well as a diagnostic tool in psychiatric research.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335944/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138827989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the reliability of value-modulated attentional capture: An online replication and multiverse analysis. 价值调节注意力捕捉的可靠性:在线复制和多元宇宙分析。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02329-5
Francisco Garre-Frutos, Miguel A Vadillo, Felisa González, Juan Lupiáñez

Stimuli predicting rewards are more likely to capture attention, even when they are not relevant to our current goals. Individual differences in value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC) have been associated with various psychopathological conditions in the scientific literature. However, the claim that this attentional bias can predict individual differences requires further exploration of the psychometric properties of the most common experimental paradigms. The current study replicated the VMAC effect in a large online sample (N = 182) and investigated the internal consistency, with a design that allowed us to measure the effect during learning (rewarded phase) and after acquisition, once feedback was omitted (unrewarded phase). Through the rewarded phase there was gradual increase of the VMAC effect, which did not decline significantly throughout the unrewarded phase. Furthermore, we conducted a reliability multiverse analysis for 288 different data preprocessing specifications across both phases. Specifications including more blocks in the analysis led to better reliability estimates in both phases, while specifications that removed more outliers also improved reliability, suggesting that specifications with more, but less noisy, trials led to better reliability estimates. Nevertheless, in most instances, especially those considering fewer blocks of trials, reliability estimates fell below the minimum recommended thresholds for research on individual differences. Given the present results, we encourage researchers working on VMAC to take into account reliability when designing studies aimed at capturing individual differences and provide recommendations to improve methodological practices.

预测奖励的刺激物更容易吸引注意力,即使它们与我们当前的目标无关。在科学文献中,价值调节注意捕捉(VMAC)的个体差异与各种精神病理学状况有关。然而,要宣称这种注意偏差可以预测个体差异,还需要进一步探索最常见实验范式的心理测量特性。本研究在一个大型在线样本(N = 182)中复制了 VMAC 效应,并调查了其内部一致性,其设计允许我们在学习期间(有奖励阶段)和学习后(无奖励阶段)测量该效应。在有奖励阶段,VMAC 效应逐渐增强,而在无奖励阶段,VMAC 效应没有明显下降。此外,我们还对两个阶段中的 288 种不同数据预处理规格进行了可靠性多重宇宙分析。在分析中包含更多区块的规格在两个阶段都能获得更好的可靠性估计,而去除更多异常值的规格也能提高可靠性,这表明包含更多但噪声更小的试验的规格能获得更好的可靠性估计。尽管如此,在大多数情况下,尤其是考虑较少试验块的情况下,可靠性估计值低于个体差异研究的最低建议阈值。鉴于目前的研究结果,我们鼓励研究人员在设计旨在捕捉个体差异的研究时考虑到可靠性,并为改进方法实践提供建议。
{"title":"On the reliability of value-modulated attentional capture: An online replication and multiverse analysis.","authors":"Francisco Garre-Frutos, Miguel A Vadillo, Felisa González, Juan Lupiáñez","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02329-5","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02329-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Stimuli predicting rewards are more likely to capture attention, even when they are not relevant to our current goals. Individual differences in value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC) have been associated with various psychopathological conditions in the scientific literature. However, the claim that this attentional bias can predict individual differences requires further exploration of the psychometric properties of the most common experimental paradigms. The current study replicated the VMAC effect in a large online sample (N = 182) and investigated the internal consistency, with a design that allowed us to measure the effect during learning (rewarded phase) and after acquisition, once feedback was omitted (unrewarded phase). Through the rewarded phase there was gradual increase of the VMAC effect, which did not decline significantly throughout the unrewarded phase. Furthermore, we conducted a reliability multiverse analysis for 288 different data preprocessing specifications across both phases. Specifications including more blocks in the analysis led to better reliability estimates in both phases, while specifications that removed more outliers also improved reliability, suggesting that specifications with more, but less noisy, trials led to better reliability estimates. Nevertheless, in most instances, especially those considering fewer blocks of trials, reliability estimates fell below the minimum recommended thresholds for research on individual differences. Given the present results, we encourage researchers working on VMAC to take into account reliability when designing studies aimed at capturing individual differences and provide recommendations to improve methodological practices.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335866/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139401624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating two small-sample corrections for fixed-effects standard errors and inferences in multilevel models with heteroscedastic, unbalanced, clustered data. 评估固定效应标准误差的两种小样本校正,以及具有异方差、不平衡、聚类数据的多层次模型中的推论。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02325-9
Yichi Zhang, Mark H C Lai

Multilevel modeling (MLM) is commonly used in psychological research to model clustered data. However, data in applied research usually violate one of the essential assumptions of MLM-homogeneity of variance. While the fixed-effect estimates produced by the maximum likelihood method remain unbiased, the standard errors for the fixed effects are misestimated, resulting in inaccurate inferences and inflated or deflated type I error rates. To correct the bias in fixed effects standard errors and provide valid inferences, small-sample corrections such as the Kenward-Roger (KR) adjustment and the adjusted cluster-robust standard errors (CR-SEs) with the Satterthwaite approximation for t tests have been used. The current study compares KR with random slope (RS) models and the adjusted CR-SEs with ordinary least squares (OLS), random intercept (RI) and RS models to analyze small, heteroscedastic, clustered data using a Monte Carlo simulation. Results show the KR procedure with RS models has large biases and inflated type I error rates for between-cluster effects in the presence of level 2 heteroscedasticity. In contrast, the adjusted CR-SEs generally yield results with acceptable biases and maintain type I error rates close to the nominal level for all examined models. Thus, when the interest is only in within-cluster effect, any model with the adjusted CR-SEs could be used. However, when the interest is to make accurate inferences of the between-cluster effect, researchers should use the adjusted CR-SEs with RS to have higher power and guard against unmodeled heterogeneity. We reanalyzed an example in Snijders & Bosker (2012) to demonstrate the use of the adjusted CR-SEs with different models.

多层次建模(MLM)常用于心理学研究中的聚类数据建模。然而,应用研究中的数据通常会违反多层次建模的一个基本假设--方差同质性。虽然最大似然法得出的固定效应估计值仍然无偏,但固定效应的标准误差却被误估,从而导致推论不准确,I 类错误率上升或下降。为了纠正固定效应标准误差的偏差并提供有效的推论,人们使用了小样本校正方法,如 Kenward-Roger(KR)调整和调整后的聚类标准误差(CR-SEs),并使用 Satterthwaite 近似方法进行 t 检验。本研究将 KR 与随机斜率(RS)模型进行了比较,并将调整后的 CR-SE 与普通最小二乘法(OLS)、随机截距(RI)和 RS 模型进行了比较,以使用蒙特卡罗模拟分析小型异方差聚类数据。结果表明,在存在二级异方差的情况下,采用 RS 模型的 KR 程序在聚类间效应方面存在较大偏差和 I 类错误率。与此相反,调整后的 CR-SE 通常会产生可接受的偏差,并在所有考察模型中保持接近名义水平的 I 类误差率。因此,如果只关注集群内效应,任何具有调整后 CR-SE 的模型都可以使用。然而,当研究人员希望准确推断聚类间效应时,则应使用带 RS 的调整 CR-SE,以获得更高的功率并防止未建模的异质性。我们重新分析了 Snijders & Bosker(2012 年)中的一个例子,以演示如何使用不同模型的调整后 CR-SE。
{"title":"Evaluating two small-sample corrections for fixed-effects standard errors and inferences in multilevel models with heteroscedastic, unbalanced, clustered data.","authors":"Yichi Zhang, Mark H C Lai","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02325-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02325-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multilevel modeling (MLM) is commonly used in psychological research to model clustered data. However, data in applied research usually violate one of the essential assumptions of MLM-homogeneity of variance. While the fixed-effect estimates produced by the maximum likelihood method remain unbiased, the standard errors for the fixed effects are misestimated, resulting in inaccurate inferences and inflated or deflated type I error rates. To correct the bias in fixed effects standard errors and provide valid inferences, small-sample corrections such as the Kenward-Roger (KR) adjustment and the adjusted cluster-robust standard errors (CR-SEs) with the Satterthwaite approximation for t tests have been used. The current study compares KR with random slope (RS) models and the adjusted CR-SEs with ordinary least squares (OLS), random intercept (RI) and RS models to analyze small, heteroscedastic, clustered data using a Monte Carlo simulation. Results show the KR procedure with RS models has large biases and inflated type I error rates for between-cluster effects in the presence of level 2 heteroscedasticity. In contrast, the adjusted CR-SEs generally yield results with acceptable biases and maintain type I error rates close to the nominal level for all examined models. Thus, when the interest is only in within-cluster effect, any model with the adjusted CR-SEs could be used. However, when the interest is to make accurate inferences of the between-cluster effect, researchers should use the adjusted CR-SEs with RS to have higher power and guard against unmodeled heterogeneity. We reanalyzed an example in Snijders & Bosker (2012) to demonstrate the use of the adjusted CR-SEs with different models.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139696871","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Standardizing norms for 1286 colored pictures in Cantonese. 1286 幅粤语彩色图片的标准化规范。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02362-y
Jing Zhong, Weike Huang, Keyi Kang, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia, Christos Pliatsikas, Haoyun Zhang

This study established psycholinguistic norms in Cantonese for a set of 1286 colored pictures sourced from several picture databases, including 750 colored line drawings from MultiPic (Duñabeitia et al., 2018) and 536 photographs selected for McRae et al. (2005) concepts. The pictures underwent rigorous normalization processes. We provided picture characteristics including name and concept agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and frequency of modal responses. Through correlational analyses, we observed strong interrelationships among these variables. We also compared the current Cantonese norming to other languages and demonstrated similarity and variations among different languages. Additionally, we embraced the multilingual diversity within the current sample, and found that higher Cantonese proficiency but lower non-native language proficiency were associated with better spoken picture naming. Last but not least, we validated the predictive power of normed variables calculated from typed responses to spoken picture naming, and the consistency between typed and spoken responses. The present norming provides a timely and valuable alternative for researchers in the field of psycholinguistics, especially those studying Cantonese production and lexical retrieval. All raw data, analysis scripts, and final norming results are available online as psycholinguistic norms for Cantonese in the following link at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/dz9j6/?view_only=a452d8a56c92430b9dedf21ac26b1bc1 .

本研究为一组来自多个图片数据库的 1286 幅彩色图片建立了粤语心理语言学规范,其中包括来自 MultiPic(Duñabeitia 等人,2018 年)的 750 幅彩色线条图和根据 McRae 等人(2005 年)的概念选取的 536 幅照片。这些图片都经过了严格的规范化处理。我们提供的图片特征包括名称和概念的一致性、熟悉度、视觉复杂性和模态反应频率。通过相关分析,我们观察到这些变量之间存在密切的相互关系。我们还将目前的粤语常模与其他语言进行了比较,证明了不同语言之间的相似性和差异性。此外,我们还接受了当前样本中的多语言多样性,并发现较高的粤语水平和较低的非母语水平与较好的图片命名口语有关。最后但并非最不重要的一点是,我们验证了根据打字回答计算出的常模变量对口语图片命名的预测能力,以及打字回答和口语回答之间的一致性。本规范为心理语言学领域的研究人员,尤其是研究粤语生产和词汇检索的研究人员提供了一个及时而有价值的选择。所有原始数据、分析脚本和最终的规范化结果均可作为粤语心理语言学规范在以下链接的开放科学框架中在线获取:https://osf.io/dz9j6/?view_only=a452d8a56c92430b9dedf21ac26b1bc1 。
{"title":"Standardizing norms for 1286 colored pictures in Cantonese.","authors":"Jing Zhong, Weike Huang, Keyi Kang, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia, Christos Pliatsikas, Haoyun Zhang","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02362-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02362-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study established psycholinguistic norms in Cantonese for a set of 1286 colored pictures sourced from several picture databases, including 750 colored line drawings from MultiPic (Duñabeitia et al., 2018) and 536 photographs selected for McRae et al. (2005) concepts. The pictures underwent rigorous normalization processes. We provided picture characteristics including name and concept agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and frequency of modal responses. Through correlational analyses, we observed strong interrelationships among these variables. We also compared the current Cantonese norming to other languages and demonstrated similarity and variations among different languages. Additionally, we embraced the multilingual diversity within the current sample, and found that higher Cantonese proficiency but lower non-native language proficiency were associated with better spoken picture naming. Last but not least, we validated the predictive power of normed variables calculated from typed responses to spoken picture naming, and the consistency between typed and spoken responses. The present norming provides a timely and valuable alternative for researchers in the field of psycholinguistics, especially those studying Cantonese production and lexical retrieval. All raw data, analysis scripts, and final norming results are available online as psycholinguistic norms for Cantonese in the following link at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/dz9j6/?view_only=a452d8a56c92430b9dedf21ac26b1bc1 .</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139911957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond alpha and omega: The accuracy of single-test reliability estimators in unidimensional continuous data. 超越阿尔法和欧米茄:单维连续数据中单次测试可靠性估计的准确性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02361-z
Eunseong Cho

Coefficient alpha is commonly used as a reliability estimator. However, several estimators are believed to be more accurate than alpha, with factor analysis (FA) estimators being the most commonly recommended. Furthermore, unstandardized estimators are considered more accurate than standardized estimators. In other words, the existing literature suggests that unstandardized FA estimators are the most accurate regardless of data characteristics. To test whether this conventional knowledge is appropriate, this study examines the accuracy of 12 estimators using a Monte Carlo simulation. The results show that several estimators are more accurate than alpha, including both FA and non-FA estimators. The most accurate on average is a standardized FA estimator. Unstandardized estimators (e.g., alpha) are less accurate on average than the corresponding standardized estimators (e.g., standardized alpha). However, the accuracy of estimators is affected to varying degrees by data characteristics (e.g., sample size, number of items, outliers). For example, standardized estimators are more accurate than unstandardized estimators with a small sample size and many outliers, and vice versa. The greatest lower bound is the most accurate when the number of items is 3 but severely overestimates reliability when the number of items is more than 3. In conclusion, estimators have their advantageous data characteristics, and no estimator is the most accurate for all data characteristics.

系数 α 通常被用作可靠性估计指标。然而,有几种估计因子被认为比 alpha 更准确,其中因子分析(FA)估计因子是最常推荐的。此外,非标准化估计值被认为比标准化估计值更准确。换句话说,现有文献表明,无论数据特征如何,非标准化的 FA 估计器都是最准确的。为了检验这一传统认识是否恰当,本研究使用蒙特卡罗模拟法检验了 12 个估计器的准确性。结果表明,有几种估计器比阿尔法更准确,包括 FA 和非 FA 估计器。平均准确度最高的是标准化 FA 估计器。非标准化估计器(如 alpha)的平均准确度低于相应的标准化估计器(如标准化 alpha)。然而,估计器的准确性在不同程度上受到数据特征(如样本大小、项目数量、异常值)的影响。例如,在样本量小、异常值多的情况下,标准化估计值比非标准化估计值更准确,反之亦然。当项目数为 3 时,最大下限是最准确的,但当项目数超过 3 时,则严重高估了可靠性。 总之,估计器有其有利的数据特征,没有一种估计器对所有数据特征都是最准确的。
{"title":"Beyond alpha and omega: The accuracy of single-test reliability estimators in unidimensional continuous data.","authors":"Eunseong Cho","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02361-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02361-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Coefficient alpha is commonly used as a reliability estimator. However, several estimators are believed to be more accurate than alpha, with factor analysis (FA) estimators being the most commonly recommended. Furthermore, unstandardized estimators are considered more accurate than standardized estimators. In other words, the existing literature suggests that unstandardized FA estimators are the most accurate regardless of data characteristics. To test whether this conventional knowledge is appropriate, this study examines the accuracy of 12 estimators using a Monte Carlo simulation. The results show that several estimators are more accurate than alpha, including both FA and non-FA estimators. The most accurate on average is a standardized FA estimator. Unstandardized estimators (e.g., alpha) are less accurate on average than the corresponding standardized estimators (e.g., standardized alpha). However, the accuracy of estimators is affected to varying degrees by data characteristics (e.g., sample size, number of items, outliers). For example, standardized estimators are more accurate than unstandardized estimators with a small sample size and many outliers, and vice versa. The greatest lower bound is the most accurate when the number of items is 3 but severely overestimates reliability when the number of items is more than 3. In conclusion, estimators have their advantageous data characteristics, and no estimator is the most accurate for all data characteristics.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139929816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lightness constancy in reality, in virtual reality, and on flat-panel displays. 现实、虚拟现实和平板显示器中的亮度恒定性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02352-0
Khushbu Y Patel, Laurie M Wilcox, Laurence T Maloney, Krista A Ehinger, Jaykishan Y Patel, Emma Wiedenmann, Richard F Murray

Virtual reality (VR) displays are being used in an increasingly wide range of applications. However, previous work shows that viewers often perceive scene properties very differently in real and virtual environments and so realistic perception of virtual stimuli should always be a carefully tested conclusion, not an assumption. One important property for realistic scene perception is surface color. To evaluate how well virtual platforms support realistic perception of achromatic surface color, we assessed lightness constancy in a physical apparatus with real lights and surfaces, in a commercial VR headset, and on a traditional flat-panel display. We found that lightness constancy was good in all three environments, though significantly better in the real environment than on the flat-panel display. We also found that variability across observers was significantly greater in VR and on the flat-panel display than in the physical environment. We conclude that these discrepancies should be taken into account in applications where realistic perception is critical but also that in many cases VR can be used as a flexible alternative to flat-panel displays and a reasonable proxy for real environments.

虚拟现实(VR)显示器的应用越来越广泛。然而,以往的研究表明,观众在真实环境和虚拟环境中对场景属性的感知往往大相径庭,因此对虚拟刺激物的逼真感知应始终是一个经过仔细测试的结论,而不是一个假设。逼真场景感知的一个重要属性是表面颜色。为了评估虚拟平台对消色差表面颜色逼真感知的支持程度,我们在带有真实灯光和表面的物理设备、商用 VR 头显和传统平板显示器上评估了亮度恒定性。我们发现,这三种环境中的光度恒定性都很好,但真实环境中的光度恒定性明显好于平板显示器上的光度恒定性。我们还发现,在 VR 和平板显示器上,观察者之间的差异明显大于物理环境中的差异。我们的结论是,在对真实感知至关重要的应用中应考虑到这些差异,但在许多情况下,虚拟现实可以灵活地替代平板显示器,并合理地替代真实环境。
{"title":"Lightness constancy in reality, in virtual reality, and on flat-panel displays.","authors":"Khushbu Y Patel, Laurie M Wilcox, Laurence T Maloney, Krista A Ehinger, Jaykishan Y Patel, Emma Wiedenmann, Richard F Murray","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02352-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02352-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Virtual reality (VR) displays are being used in an increasingly wide range of applications. However, previous work shows that viewers often perceive scene properties very differently in real and virtual environments and so realistic perception of virtual stimuli should always be a carefully tested conclusion, not an assumption. One important property for realistic scene perception is surface color. To evaluate how well virtual platforms support realistic perception of achromatic surface color, we assessed lightness constancy in a physical apparatus with real lights and surfaces, in a commercial VR headset, and on a traditional flat-panel display. We found that lightness constancy was good in all three environments, though significantly better in the real environment than on the flat-panel display. We also found that variability across observers was significantly greater in VR and on the flat-panel display than in the physical environment. We conclude that these discrepancies should be taken into account in applications where realistic perception is critical but also that in many cases VR can be used as a flexible alternative to flat-panel displays and a reasonable proxy for real environments.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140038686","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preregistration in practice: A comparison of preregistered and non-preregistered studies in psychology. 实践中的预注册:心理学预注册和非预注册研究的比较。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02277-0
Olmo R van den Akker, Marcel A L M van Assen, Marjan Bakker, Mahmoud Elsherif, Tsz Keung Wong, Jelte M Wicherts

Preregistration has gained traction as one of the most promising solutions to improve the replicability of scientific effects. In this project, we compared 193 psychology studies that earned a Preregistration Challenge prize or preregistration badge to 193 related studies that were not preregistered. In contrast to our theoretical expectations and prior research, we did not find that preregistered studies had a lower proportion of positive results (Hypothesis 1), smaller effect sizes (Hypothesis 2), or fewer statistical errors (Hypothesis 3) than non-preregistered studies. Supporting our Hypotheses 4 and 5, we found that preregistered studies more often contained power analyses and typically had larger sample sizes than non-preregistered studies. Finally, concerns about the publishability and impact of preregistered studies seem unwarranted, as preregistered studies did not take longer to publish and scored better on several impact measures. Overall, our data indicate that preregistration has beneficial effects in the realm of statistical power and impact, but we did not find robust evidence that preregistration prevents p-hacking and HARKing (Hypothesizing After the Results are Known).

预注册已成为提高科学效果可复制性的最有希望的解决方案之一。在这个项目中,我们将193项获得预注册挑战奖或预注册徽章的心理学研究与193项未预注册的相关研究进行了比较。与我们的理论预期和先前的研究相比,我们没有发现预登记研究的阳性结果比例(假设1)、效应大小(假设2)或统计误差(假设3)低于未预登记研究。支持我们的假设4和假设5,我们发现预注册研究更经常包含功率分析,并且通常比非预注册研究具有更大的样本量。最后,对预注册研究的可发表性和影响的担忧似乎是没有根据的,因为预注册研究发表的时间不长,而且在几个影响指标上得分更高。总的来说,我们的数据表明,预注册在统计能力和影响领域具有有益的影响,但我们没有发现强有力的证据表明预注册可以防止p黑客攻击和HARKing(结果已知后的假设)。
{"title":"Preregistration in practice: A comparison of preregistered and non-preregistered studies in psychology.","authors":"Olmo R van den Akker, Marcel A L M van Assen, Marjan Bakker, Mahmoud Elsherif, Tsz Keung Wong, Jelte M Wicherts","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02277-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02277-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Preregistration has gained traction as one of the most promising solutions to improve the replicability of scientific effects. In this project, we compared 193 psychology studies that earned a Preregistration Challenge prize or preregistration badge to 193 related studies that were not preregistered. In contrast to our theoretical expectations and prior research, we did not find that preregistered studies had a lower proportion of positive results (Hypothesis 1), smaller effect sizes (Hypothesis 2), or fewer statistical errors (Hypothesis 3) than non-preregistered studies. Supporting our Hypotheses 4 and 5, we found that preregistered studies more often contained power analyses and typically had larger sample sizes than non-preregistered studies. Finally, concerns about the publishability and impact of preregistered studies seem unwarranted, as preregistered studies did not take longer to publish and scored better on several impact measures. Overall, our data indicate that preregistration has beneficial effects in the realm of statistical power and impact, but we did not find robust evidence that preregistration prevents p-hacking and HARKing (Hypothesizing After the Results are Known).</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335781/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72208135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lingualyzer: A computational linguistic tool for multilingual and multidimensional text analysis. Lingualyzer:一个用于多语言和多维文本分析的计算语言工具。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-29 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02284-1
Guido M Linders, Max M Louwerse

Most natural language models and tools are restricted to one language, typically English. For researchers in the behavioral sciences investigating languages other than English, and for those researchers who would like to make cross-linguistic comparisons, hardly any computational linguistic tools exist, particularly none for those researchers who lack deep computational linguistic knowledge or programming skills. Yet, for interdisciplinary researchers in a variety of fields, ranging from psycholinguistics, social psychology, cognitive psychology, education, to literary studies, there certainly is a need for such a cross-linguistic tool. In the current paper, we present Lingualyzer ( https://lingualyzer.com ), an easily accessible tool that analyzes text at three different text levels (sentence, paragraph, document), which includes 351 multidimensional linguistic measures that are available in 41 different languages. This paper gives an overview of Lingualyzer, categorizes its hundreds of measures, demonstrates how it distinguishes itself from other text quantification tools, explains how it can be used, and provides validations. Lingualyzer is freely accessible for scientific purposes using an intuitive and easy-to-use interface.

大多数自然语言模型和工具仅限于一种语言,通常是英语。对于研究英语以外语言的行为科学研究人员,以及那些想要进行跨语言比较的研究人员来说,几乎没有任何计算语言工具存在,特别是那些缺乏深度计算语言知识或编程技能的研究人员。然而,对于从心理语言学、社会心理学、认知心理学、教育学到文学研究等各个领域的跨学科研究人员来说,肯定需要这样一种跨语言的工具。在本文中,我们介绍了Lingualyzer (https://lingualyzer.com),这是一个易于访问的工具,可以分析三个不同文本级别(句子,段落,文档)的文本,其中包括41种不同语言的351种多维语言度量。本文概述了Lingualyzer,对它的数百个度量进行了分类,演示了它如何区别于其他文本量化工具,解释了如何使用它,并提供了验证。Lingualyzer使用直观和易于使用的界面免费访问科学目的。
{"title":"Lingualyzer: A computational linguistic tool for multilingual and multidimensional text analysis.","authors":"Guido M Linders, Max M Louwerse","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02284-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02284-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most natural language models and tools are restricted to one language, typically English. For researchers in the behavioral sciences investigating languages other than English, and for those researchers who would like to make cross-linguistic comparisons, hardly any computational linguistic tools exist, particularly none for those researchers who lack deep computational linguistic knowledge or programming skills. Yet, for interdisciplinary researchers in a variety of fields, ranging from psycholinguistics, social psychology, cognitive psychology, education, to literary studies, there certainly is a need for such a cross-linguistic tool. In the current paper, we present Lingualyzer ( https://lingualyzer.com ), an easily accessible tool that analyzes text at three different text levels (sentence, paragraph, document), which includes 351 multidimensional linguistic measures that are available in 41 different languages. This paper gives an overview of Lingualyzer, categorizes its hundreds of measures, demonstrates how it distinguishes itself from other text quantification tools, explains how it can be used, and provides validations. Lingualyzer is freely accessible for scientific purposes using an intuitive and easy-to-use interface.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335911/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138457550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Some students are more equal: Performance in Author Recognition Test and Title Recognition Test modulated by print exposure and academic background. 有些学生更加平等:作者识别测试和标题识别测试的成绩受印刷接触和学术背景的影响。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02330-y
Marion Hug, Julian Jarosch, Christiane Eichenauer, Selina Pennella, Franziska Kretzschmar, Pascal Nicklas

Reading is a key skill for university students. The Author Recognition Test (ART) and the Title Recognition Test (TRT) have both been used for decades to measure print exposure which correlates with reading and other linguistic skills. Given the available evidence for interindividual differences in reading skills, this study addresses three open issues. First, to what extent do ART and TRT scores correlate with individual differences regarding students' study programs? Second, how do these results correlate with the self-reported time spent reading different types of text genres (e.g., fiction, nonfiction) per week? And third, this study compares ART and TRT to one another. We tested students from six study programs in the humanities and (medical) sciences which vary in the amount and kind of reading material required for study success. We found that students perform significantly differently in the ART and the TRT depending on their field of study. Students in a study program focusing on fiction and literature perform best overall. We also replicated the well-known effect of age on ART and TRT scores: older students have better scores. We did not find reliable effects of reading time on test performance, whereas individual creative writing habits did positively predict ART/TRT test results. These results raise a number of important questions regarding the ART/TRT in general and regarding interindividual differences in personal reading and writing habits and the change in reading habits in times of media convergence.

阅读是大学生的一项关键技能。几十年来,作者识别测验(ART)和标题识别测验(TRT)都被用来测量与阅读和其他语言技能相关的印刷品接触情况。鉴于有证据表明阅读能力存在个体差异,本研究探讨了三个未决问题。首先,ART 和 TRT 分数在多大程度上与学生学习计划的个体差异相关?第二,这些结果与自我报告的每周阅读不同类型文本(如小说、非小说)的时间有何关联?第三,本研究对 ART 和 TRT 进行了比较。我们对来自人文科学和(医学)科学领域的六个学习项目的学生进行了测试,这些项目对学习成功所需的阅读材料的数量和种类要求各不相同。我们发现,不同专业的学生在 ART 和 TRT 中的表现明显不同。以小说和文学为重点的学习计划的学生整体表现最佳。我们还证实了众所周知的年龄对 ART 和 TRT 成绩的影响:年龄较大的学生成绩较好。我们没有发现阅读时间对考试成绩有可靠的影响,而个人的创意写作习惯确实对 ART/TRT 考试成绩有积极的预测作用。这些结果提出了一些重要问题,涉及 ART/TRT 的总体情况、个人阅读和写作习惯的个体差异以及媒体融合时代阅读习惯的变化。
{"title":"Some students are more equal: Performance in Author Recognition Test and Title Recognition Test modulated by print exposure and academic background.","authors":"Marion Hug, Julian Jarosch, Christiane Eichenauer, Selina Pennella, Franziska Kretzschmar, Pascal Nicklas","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02330-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02330-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reading is a key skill for university students. The Author Recognition Test (ART) and the Title Recognition Test (TRT) have both been used for decades to measure print exposure which correlates with reading and other linguistic skills. Given the available evidence for interindividual differences in reading skills, this study addresses three open issues. First, to what extent do ART and TRT scores correlate with individual differences regarding students' study programs? Second, how do these results correlate with the self-reported time spent reading different types of text genres (e.g., fiction, nonfiction) per week? And third, this study compares ART and TRT to one another. We tested students from six study programs in the humanities and (medical) sciences which vary in the amount and kind of reading material required for study success. We found that students perform significantly differently in the ART and the TRT depending on their field of study. Students in a study program focusing on fiction and literature perform best overall. We also replicated the well-known effect of age on ART and TRT scores: older students have better scores. We did not find reliable effects of reading time on test performance, whereas individual creative writing habits did positively predict ART/TRT test results. These results raise a number of important questions regarding the ART/TRT in general and regarding interindividual differences in personal reading and writing habits and the change in reading habits in times of media convergence.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335832/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139401626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effectiveness of warning statements in reducing careless responding in crowdsourced online surveys. 在众包在线调查中,警告语句对减少粗心应答的有效性。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02321-z
Florian Brühlmann, Zgjim Memeti, Lena F Aeschbach, Sebastian A C Perrig, Klaus Opwis

Carelessness or insufficient effort responding is a widespread problem in online research, with estimates ranging from 3% to almost 50% of participants in online surveys being inattentive. While detecting carelessness has been subject to multiple studies, the factors that reduce or prevent carelessness are not as well understood. Initial evidence suggests that warning statements prior to study participation may reduce carelessness, but there is a lack of conclusive high-powered studies. This preregistered randomized controlled experiment aimed to test the effectiveness of a warning statement and an improved implementation of a warning statement in reducing participant inattention. A study with 812 participants recruited on Amazon Mechanical Turk was conducted. Results suggest that presenting a warning statement is not effective in reducing carelessness. However, requiring participants to actively type the warning statement statistically significantly reduced carelessness as measured with self-reported diligence, even-odd consistency, psychometric synonyms and antonyms, and individual response variability. The active warning statements also led to statistically significantly more attrition and potentially deterred those who were likely to be careless from even participating in this study. We show that the current standard practice of implementing warning statements is ineffective and novel methods to prevent and deter carelessness are needed.

粗心大意或回答不够努力是在线调查中普遍存在的问题,据估计,在线调查中注意力不集中的参与者比例从 3% 到近 50% 不等。虽然对粗心的检测已经进行了多项研究,但对减少或防止粗心的因素却不甚了解。初步证据表明,参与研究前的警告声明可能会减少粗心,但目前还缺乏确凿的高效力研究。这项预先登记的随机对照实验旨在测试警告声明和改进后的警告声明在减少参与者注意力不集中方面的效果。研究在亚马逊 Mechanical Turk 上招募了 812 名参与者。结果表明,提出警告语句并不能有效减少粗心。然而,根据自我报告的勤奋程度、偶数一致性、心理测量同义词和反义词以及个人反应变异性来衡量,要求参与者主动键入警告语句在统计学上显著降低了粗心程度。从统计学角度看,主动警告语句也会导致更多的自然减员,并有可能阻止那些可能粗心的人参与本研究。我们的研究表明,目前实施警告语句的标准做法是无效的,因此需要采用新的方法来预防和阻止粗心行为。
{"title":"The effectiveness of warning statements in reducing careless responding in crowdsourced online surveys.","authors":"Florian Brühlmann, Zgjim Memeti, Lena F Aeschbach, Sebastian A C Perrig, Klaus Opwis","doi":"10.3758/s13428-023-02321-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-023-02321-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Carelessness or insufficient effort responding is a widespread problem in online research, with estimates ranging from 3% to almost 50% of participants in online surveys being inattentive. While detecting carelessness has been subject to multiple studies, the factors that reduce or prevent carelessness are not as well understood. Initial evidence suggests that warning statements prior to study participation may reduce carelessness, but there is a lack of conclusive high-powered studies. This preregistered randomized controlled experiment aimed to test the effectiveness of a warning statement and an improved implementation of a warning statement in reducing participant inattention. A study with 812 participants recruited on Amazon Mechanical Turk was conducted. Results suggest that presenting a warning statement is not effective in reducing carelessness. However, requiring participants to actively type the warning statement statistically significantly reduced carelessness as measured with self-reported diligence, even-odd consistency, psychometric synonyms and antonyms, and individual response variability. The active warning statements also led to statistically significantly more attrition and potentially deterred those who were likely to be careless from even participating in this study. We show that the current standard practice of implementing warning statements is ineffective and novel methods to prevent and deter carelessness are needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11335820/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139490781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavior Research Methods
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1