首页 > 最新文献

Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
Female mate choice for male size in a freshwater goby under experimental conditions eliminating inter-male interference 在消除雄性间干扰的实验条件下,淡水鰕虎鱼雌性对雄性体型的择偶要求
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10258
Sayaka Ito, Kei’ichiro Iguchi
The preference of females for male body size was examined in an experimental environment where direct interference between males was eliminated. In this environment, females preferred to spawn with larger males. To understand the preference of females for larger males, paternal care and egg defence abilities and paternal cannibalism intensity were further tested with respect to the body size of males. The dead egg rate for an egg mass during the paternal care period was not related to the body size of the paternal males. However, larger males could defend their egg masses more successfully against nest intruders than smaller ones. During the paternal egg care period, paternal males often consumed eggs in their nests. In particular, smaller males compared to females caused paternal brood cannibalism. High egg defence ability and low paternal cannibalism intensity are the most plausible reasons for females to choose larger males.
在实验环境中,消除了雄性之间的直接干扰,研究了雌性对雄性体型的偏好。在这种环境下,雌性更喜欢与体型较大的雄性一起产卵。为了了解雌性对较大雄性的偏好,进一步测试了雄性体型对父代照顾和卵子防御能力以及父代食人强度的影响。在父代照料期间,一个卵块的死卵率与父代雄性的体型无关。然而,与体型较小的雄性相比,体型较大的雄性能更成功地抵御巢穴入侵者。在父代护卵期间,父代雄性经常吃掉巢中的卵。与雌性相比,体型较小的雄性尤其会造成父代吃雏。雌性选择体型较大雄性的最合理的原因是高护卵能力和低父代食雏强度。
{"title":"Female mate choice for male size in a freshwater goby under experimental conditions eliminating inter-male interference","authors":"Sayaka Ito, Kei’ichiro Iguchi","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10258","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10258","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000The preference of females for male body size was examined in an experimental environment where direct interference between males was eliminated. In this environment, females preferred to spawn with larger males. To understand the preference of females for larger males, paternal care and egg defence abilities and paternal cannibalism intensity were further tested with respect to the body size of males. The dead egg rate for an egg mass during the paternal care period was not related to the body size of the paternal males. However, larger males could defend their egg masses more successfully against nest intruders than smaller ones. During the paternal egg care period, paternal males often consumed eggs in their nests. In particular, smaller males compared to females caused paternal brood cannibalism. High egg defence ability and low paternal cannibalism intensity are the most plausible reasons for females to choose larger males.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140490249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In their own language: cues guiding sticky spiral placement by Micrathena duodecimspinosa (Araneae: Araneidae) 用它们自己的语言:Micrathena duodecimspinosa(鹤形目:鹤科)放置粘性螺旋的线索
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-17 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10257
William G. Eberhard
Orb-web construction behaviour, a classic example of ‘innate’ behaviour, is highly flexible. Determining which cues guide construction behaviour is complicated by the strong correlations between some variables, and the difficulty of manipulating of some web variables in biologically realistic ways. This study utilized a new experimental technique, adding sticky lines to webs during construction, to examine cues that guide sticky spiral spacing. Deflections and subsequent reductions in spacing produced by experiments that involved relatively long radii resembled the reduced spacing pattern on short radii in control webs; radius length was thus not the cue eliciting reduced spacing in control webs. An alternative, that smaller inter-radius distances elicited reduced spacing, was supported by experimentally removing radii to increase the distances between radii. The new technique also elicited variation in responses that suggested that spiders may use mental cues such as memories or expectations of line positions to guide sticky spiral placement.
作为 "与生俱来 "行为的一个典型例子,球网的构建行为非常灵活。由于某些变量之间存在很强的相关性,而且难以以符合生物实际情况的方式操纵某些蛛网变量,因此确定哪些线索会引导蛛网的构建行为变得非常复杂。本研究采用了一种新的实验技术,即在构建过程中在蛛网上添加粘性线,以研究引导粘性螺旋间距的线索。在半径相对较长的实验中产生的偏转和随后的间距减小与对照网上半径较短的间距减小模式相似;因此,半径长度并不是导致对照网上间距减小的线索。通过实验去除半径以增加半径之间的距离,支持了另一种观点,即较小的半径间距会导致间距减小。新技术还能引起不同的反应,这表明蜘蛛可能会利用心理线索(如对线位置的记忆或期望)来指导粘附螺旋的位置。
{"title":"In their own language: cues guiding sticky spiral placement by Micrathena duodecimspinosa (Araneae: Araneidae)","authors":"William G. Eberhard","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10257","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10257","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Orb-web construction behaviour, a classic example of ‘innate’ behaviour, is highly flexible. Determining which cues guide construction behaviour is complicated by the strong correlations between some variables, and the difficulty of manipulating of some web variables in biologically realistic ways. This study utilized a new experimental technique, adding sticky lines to webs during construction, to examine cues that guide sticky spiral spacing. Deflections and subsequent reductions in spacing produced by experiments that involved relatively long radii resembled the reduced spacing pattern on short radii in control webs; radius length was thus not the cue eliciting reduced spacing in control webs. An alternative, that smaller inter-radius distances elicited reduced spacing, was supported by experimentally removing radii to increase the distances between radii. The new technique also elicited variation in responses that suggested that spiders may use mental cues such as memories or expectations of line positions to guide sticky spiral placement.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139526878","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vocal performance of one affects that of another 一个人的声乐表现会影响另一个人的声乐表现
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10256
Keith W. Sockman, Susan M. Lyons, Samuel P. Caro
The trill elements of many bird species’ songs have been hypothesized as honest signals of performance. However, the breadth of receiver responses to variation in the signaller’s trill performance is unknown. We exposed wild male Lincoln’s sparrows (Melospiza lincolnii) to two song treatments, one with low- and another with high-performance trills. We found no effect of treatment on measures of agonistic behaviour and song length. However, relative to the subjects’ trills following the high-performance treatment, those following the low-performance treatment were elevated in performance due to trill types with high frequency bandwidth in the third trill of songs. Treatment also affected trill duration through its syllable count in a manner that varied by the song’s trill number. Thus, the performance of a signal to which a receiver is exposed drives plasticity in his own performance in sequence-specific manner. Males may showcase their own performance in the presence of lower-performing rivals.
许多鸟类鸣声中的颤音被假定为诚实的表演信号。然而,接收者对信号发出者颤音表现变化的反应范围尚不清楚。我们对野生雄性林肯麻雀(Melospiza lincolnii)进行了两种歌曲处理,一种是低性能颤音,另一种是高性能颤音。我们发现,两种处理对鸟类的激动行为和鸣唱长度没有影响。然而,相对于受试者在高性能处理下的颤音,低性能处理下的颤音由于在歌曲的第三颤音中具有高频带宽而在性能上有所提高。处理还通过音节数影响颤音的持续时间,其方式因歌曲的颤音数而异。因此,接收者接触到的信号的表现会以特定顺序的方式驱动其自身表现的可塑性。雄性可能会在表现较差的对手面前展示自己的表现。
{"title":"Vocal performance of one affects that of another","authors":"Keith W. Sockman, Susan M. Lyons, Samuel P. Caro","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10256","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000The trill elements of many bird species’ songs have been hypothesized as honest signals of performance. However, the breadth of receiver responses to variation in the signaller’s trill performance is unknown. We exposed wild male Lincoln’s sparrows (Melospiza lincolnii) to two song treatments, one with low- and another with high-performance trills. We found no effect of treatment on measures of agonistic behaviour and song length. However, relative to the subjects’ trills following the high-performance treatment, those following the low-performance treatment were elevated in performance due to trill types with high frequency bandwidth in the third trill of songs. Treatment also affected trill duration through its syllable count in a manner that varied by the song’s trill number. Thus, the performance of a signal to which a receiver is exposed drives plasticity in his own performance in sequence-specific manner. Males may showcase their own performance in the presence of lower-performing rivals.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139445021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Explaining the survival of the sickest: altered walking patterns are linked with improved adult survival in Drosophila melanogaster grown with predators during larval development 解释重病者的生存:黑腹果蝇在幼虫发育过程中与捕食者一起生长,其行走方式的改变与成虫生存率的提高有关
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-02 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10254
S. Popovs, Maris Munkevics, Tatjana Krama, Ronalds Krams, Ē. Sļedevskis, G. Trakimas, Krists Zants, Tatjana Grigorjeva, V. Mizers, Vadims Kolbjonoks, Priit Jõers, I. Krams
Stress caused by predator exposure can lead to various behavioural, physiological, stoichiometric, and biochemical changes in prey. Prior research has shown that growth under predation stress can cause the development of a diabetes-like biochemical phenotype in fruit flies. Exposure to predator risk during larval development decreases flies’ walking activity, improving their antipredator strategies. However, it is unclear which elements of walking behaviour make flies less conspicuous to predators. This study shows that fruit flies () grown with spiders walk shorter distances, accelerate faster and spend more time in a state of motion without movement (i.e., stomping in place) than control flies (). Under predation risk, adult flies grown with spiders survived better than control flies. We suggest that motions without movement may resemble sickness behaviour for predators, which we propose as the main reason for their better survival under direct exposure to predator attacks.
捕食者造成的压力可导致猎物发生各种行为、生理、化学计量和生化变化。先前的研究表明,捕食压力下的生长会导致果蝇出现类似糖尿病的生化表型。在幼虫发育过程中暴露于捕食者的风险会减少果蝇的行走活动,从而改善它们的反捕食策略。然而,目前还不清楚行走行为的哪些因素会使苍蝇在捕食者面前不那么显眼。这项研究表明,与对照组苍蝇()相比,与蜘蛛一起生长的果蝇()行走的距离更短、加速更快,并且有更多的时间处于无运动状态(即原地跺脚)。在捕食风险下,与对照组相比,用蜘蛛培育的成蝇存活率更高。我们认为,不动的运动可能类似于捕食者的生病行为,这也是它们在直接遭受捕食者攻击时存活率较高的主要原因。
{"title":"Explaining the survival of the sickest: altered walking patterns are linked with improved adult survival in Drosophila melanogaster grown with predators during larval development","authors":"S. Popovs, Maris Munkevics, Tatjana Krama, Ronalds Krams, Ē. Sļedevskis, G. Trakimas, Krists Zants, Tatjana Grigorjeva, V. Mizers, Vadims Kolbjonoks, Priit Jõers, I. Krams","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10254","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10254","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Stress caused by predator exposure can lead to various behavioural, physiological, stoichiometric, and biochemical changes in prey. Prior research has shown that growth under predation stress can cause the development of a diabetes-like biochemical phenotype in fruit flies. Exposure to predator risk during larval development decreases flies’ walking activity, improving their antipredator strategies. However, it is unclear which elements of walking behaviour make flies less conspicuous to predators. This study shows that fruit flies () grown with spiders walk shorter distances, accelerate faster and spend more time in a state of motion without movement (i.e., stomping in place) than control flies (). Under predation risk, adult flies grown with spiders survived better than control flies. We suggest that motions without movement may resemble sickness behaviour for predators, which we propose as the main reason for their better survival under direct exposure to predator attacks.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139389699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stress, corticosterone, and colour-change in a toad with dynamic sexual dichromatism 压力、皮质酮和动态性二色性蟾蜍的颜色变化
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-02 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10255
Katrina M. Gardner, D. Mennill, Amy E.M. Newman, S. Doucet
Animals use colouration to serve diverse functions including camouflage, thermoregulation, and communication. Recent research has revealed that many anurans exhibit drastic colour changes and growing evidence supports that these changes are sexually selected signals. Male yellow toads, Incilius luetkenii, exhibit dynamic sexual dichromatism, changing from mud-brown to lemon-yellow during their brief breeding events. Toads darken when isolated in captivity, which is hypothesized to be a stress response, although the mechanisms driving this change have yet to be experimentally investigated. We confined breeding toads to small terrariums for four hours and predicted that colour and corticosterone levels would change in isolation. We found that toads darkened during isolation, but that corticosterone levels did not change with colour. Our correlational results suggest that corticosterone is not the main driver of colour change in yellow toads and highlight avenues for future research that may enhance our understanding of colour change in anurans.
动物利用颜色来实现各种功能,包括伪装、体温调节和交流。最近的研究发现,许多无尾类动物的体色会发生急剧变化,越来越多的证据表明,这些变化是性选择信号。雄性黄蟾蜍(Incilius luetkenii)表现出动态的性二色性,在短暂的繁殖期间从泥棕色变为柠檬黄色。蟾蜍在圈养隔离时颜色会变深,这被认为是一种应激反应,但这种变化的驱动机制还有待实验研究。我们将正在繁殖的蟾蜍关在小型饲养箱内四小时,并预测在隔离状态下蟾蜍的颜色和皮质酮水平会发生变化。我们发现,蟾蜍在隔离期间颜色变深,但皮质酮水平并不随颜色变化而变化。我们的相关研究结果表明,皮质酮并不是黄蟾蜍颜色变化的主要驱动因素,并强调了未来研究的途径,这可能会加深我们对无尾目动物颜色变化的了解。
{"title":"Stress, corticosterone, and colour-change in a toad with dynamic sexual dichromatism","authors":"Katrina M. Gardner, D. Mennill, Amy E.M. Newman, S. Doucet","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10255","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10255","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Animals use colouration to serve diverse functions including camouflage, thermoregulation, and communication. Recent research has revealed that many anurans exhibit drastic colour changes and growing evidence supports that these changes are sexually selected signals. Male yellow toads, Incilius luetkenii, exhibit dynamic sexual dichromatism, changing from mud-brown to lemon-yellow during their brief breeding events. Toads darken when isolated in captivity, which is hypothesized to be a stress response, although the mechanisms driving this change have yet to be experimentally investigated. We confined breeding toads to small terrariums for four hours and predicted that colour and corticosterone levels would change in isolation. We found that toads darkened during isolation, but that corticosterone levels did not change with colour. Our correlational results suggest that corticosterone is not the main driver of colour change in yellow toads and highlight avenues for future research that may enhance our understanding of colour change in anurans.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139391537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flight order does not influence individuals’ flight initiation distance in small bird flocks 飞行顺序不会影响小型鸟群中个体的飞行起始距离
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-15 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10253
Jin-Ming Zhao, Hong-Chuang Liu, Si-Yu Li, Yi-Hang Ding, Jun-Jie Wang, Yan Zhang
Researchers often measure the flight initiation distance (FID) of a selected individual to represent the FID of the whole groups which may vary with their flight order (the sequence to flee) and their spatial position within a group. In this study, we examined FID of all individuals in small bird flocks in urban environments when approached by an investigator. We found that individuals’ FID did not vary with their flight orders within these flocks. Moreover, the FID of the nearest individual, often used as a proxy for the entire flock, did not differ from the average FID of all flock members. This suggests that the conventional method of measuring the nearest individual’s FID to represent is a valid approximation for representing the behaviour of the entire flock in similar contexts. These results have important implications for understanding anti-predator strategies in group-living animals and can inform future studies in this field.
研究人员通常测量选定个体的飞行起始距离(FID)来代表整个群体的飞行起始距离,而飞行起始距离可能会随着个体的飞行顺序(逃离顺序)和个体在群体中的空间位置而变化。在这项研究中,我们考察了城市环境中小型鸟群中所有个体在被调查人员接近时的飞行起始距离。我们发现,在这些鸟群中,个体的 FID 并不随其飞行顺序而变化。此外,通常被用来代表整个鸟群的最近个体的 FID 与所有鸟群成员的平均 FID 并无差别。这表明,在类似情况下,测量最近个体的FID来代表整个鸟群行为的传统方法是有效的近似方法。这些结果对于理解群居动物的反捕食策略具有重要意义,并可为今后该领域的研究提供参考。
{"title":"Flight order does not influence individuals’ flight initiation distance in small bird flocks","authors":"Jin-Ming Zhao, Hong-Chuang Liu, Si-Yu Li, Yi-Hang Ding, Jun-Jie Wang, Yan Zhang","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10253","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10253","url":null,"abstract":"Researchers often measure the flight initiation distance (FID) of a selected individual to represent the FID of the whole groups which may vary with their flight order (the sequence to flee) and their spatial position within a group. In this study, we examined FID of all individuals in small bird flocks in urban environments when approached by an investigator. We found that individuals’ FID did not vary with their flight orders within these flocks. Moreover, the FID of the nearest individual, often used as a proxy for the entire flock, did not differ from the average FID of all flock members. This suggests that the conventional method of measuring the nearest individual’s FID to represent is a valid approximation for representing the behaviour of the entire flock in similar contexts. These results have important implications for understanding anti-predator strategies in group-living animals and can inform future studies in this field.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139272505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prey size preference in the tokay gecko (Gekko gecko) 壁虎对猎物大小的偏好(壁虎)
4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-13 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10251
Anja Probst, Eva Ringler, Birgit Szabo
Abstract The optimal foraging theory posits that animals aim to maximize energy intake while minimizing predation and handling costs during foraging. Most observed animal behaviour supports this theory, but occasional deviations provide insights into the ecological factors that shape foraging decisions. We tested prey-size preference using a two-choice test between different prey sizes in tokay geckos. We expected geckos to prefer larger prey and decision latencies to be longer when discrimination was more difficult and when small prey was offered. Geckos preferred larger prey when the size difference was large, although decision latency remained consistent. This aligns with prior research on sit-and-wait predators. Together with previous findings showing freezing behaviour after prey capture in tokay geckos, our findings suggest a strong influence of predator avoidance on foraging decisions opening up a new avenue for future research investigating the link between decision making and predator avoidance in tokay geckos.
最优觅食理论认为,动物在觅食过程中以能量摄入最大化为目标,同时使捕食和处理成本最小化。大多数观察到的动物行为都支持这一理论,但偶尔的偏差也能让我们深入了解影响觅食决策的生态因素。我们使用两种选择测试来测试东华壁虎对不同猎物大小的偏好。我们预计壁虎更倾向于选择更大的猎物,当识别更困难和提供小猎物时,决策潜伏期更长。当体型差异较大时,壁虎偏好较大的猎物,尽管决策延迟保持一致。这与先前对坐等捕食者的研究一致。我们的研究结果表明,捕食者的回避对觅食决策有很大的影响,这为未来研究捕食者的回避与决策之间的联系开辟了一条新的途径。
{"title":"Prey size preference in the tokay gecko (Gekko gecko)","authors":"Anja Probst, Eva Ringler, Birgit Szabo","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10251","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10251","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The optimal foraging theory posits that animals aim to maximize energy intake while minimizing predation and handling costs during foraging. Most observed animal behaviour supports this theory, but occasional deviations provide insights into the ecological factors that shape foraging decisions. We tested prey-size preference using a two-choice test between different prey sizes in tokay geckos. We expected geckos to prefer larger prey and decision latencies to be longer when discrimination was more difficult and when small prey was offered. Geckos preferred larger prey when the size difference was large, although decision latency remained consistent. This aligns with prior research on sit-and-wait predators. Together with previous findings showing freezing behaviour after prey capture in tokay geckos, our findings suggest a strong influence of predator avoidance on foraging decisions opening up a new avenue for future research investigating the link between decision making and predator avoidance in tokay geckos.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136352262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nuthatch (Sitta europaea) responses towards simulated territorial intrusion (STI) vary in strength during the non-breeding season — a matter of temperature and photoperiod? 在非繁殖期,Nuthatch (Sitta europaea)对模拟领土入侵(STI)的反应强度不同,这是温度和光周期的问题。
4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10252
Christoph Randler, Nadine Kalb, Mylène Dutour
Abstract Many bird species defend their territories against conspecifics during the breeding season. Although some temperate species have been observed to defend territories outside of the breeding season, such cases are rarely noted in the literature, and it remains largely unknown which environmental factors influence such behaviour. In this study, we investigated non-breeding territorial behaviour in the Eurasian nuthatch ( Sitta europaea ). Territorial responses were elicited with a simulated territory intrusion (playbacks of calls combined with a wooden model of nuthatch) in autumn, winter and spring. Territorial responses did not differ between autumn and winter but differed between the autumn/winter and spring; they declined until about mid-December (winter) and increased after that into the spring. We found an increase in territorial responses with increasing photoperiod (day length) in autumn and with decreasing ambient temperature during spring. Our study provides novel evidence about non-breeding territorial behaviour in temperate birds and indicates that this behaviour is influenced by environmental factors.
在繁殖季节,许多鸟类都在保护自己的领地免受同种异种的侵害。虽然已经观察到一些温带物种在繁殖季节之外保卫领土,但这种情况在文献中很少被注意到,而且影响这种行为的环境因素在很大程度上仍然是未知的。在本研究中,我们调查了欧亚nuchath (Sitta europaea)的非繁殖领土行为。在秋天、冬天和春天,通过模拟领土入侵(播放叫声与木制的nu茅草雀模型相结合)引发了领土反应。地域反应在秋冬季节无显著差异,但在秋冬季节与春季有显著差异;它们一直下降到12月中旬(冬季),然后上升到春季。我们发现,随着秋季光照周期(日长)的增加和春季环境温度的降低,区域响应增加。我们的研究为温带鸟类的非繁殖领土行为提供了新的证据,并表明这种行为受环境因素的影响。
{"title":"Nuthatch (Sitta europaea) responses towards simulated territorial intrusion (STI) vary in strength during the non-breeding season — a matter of temperature and photoperiod?","authors":"Christoph Randler, Nadine Kalb, Mylène Dutour","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10252","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10252","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Many bird species defend their territories against conspecifics during the breeding season. Although some temperate species have been observed to defend territories outside of the breeding season, such cases are rarely noted in the literature, and it remains largely unknown which environmental factors influence such behaviour. In this study, we investigated non-breeding territorial behaviour in the Eurasian nuthatch ( Sitta europaea ). Territorial responses were elicited with a simulated territory intrusion (playbacks of calls combined with a wooden model of nuthatch) in autumn, winter and spring. Territorial responses did not differ between autumn and winter but differed between the autumn/winter and spring; they declined until about mid-December (winter) and increased after that into the spring. We found an increase in territorial responses with increasing photoperiod (day length) in autumn and with decreasing ambient temperature during spring. Our study provides novel evidence about non-breeding territorial behaviour in temperate birds and indicates that this behaviour is influenced by environmental factors.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135092048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shifting incubation rhythms in response to predation risk and the length of the response in mountain bluebirds 改变孵化节奏以应对捕食风险和山地蓝鸟的反应时间
4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10250
Simon P. Tkaczyk, Douglas P. Chivers, Karen L. Wiebe
Abstract Parent birds may alter incubation rhythms in response to predation risk but few studies have examined the recovery time immediately after exposure to a predator. Here, we examined incubation rhythms in mountain bluebirds ( Sialia currucoides ) in response to a simulated nest predator, a taxidermy-mounted squirrel. We used data loggers (iButtons) to measure the recess (off-bout) length, recess rate, and constancy of incubation and found no relationship between incubation rhythms and female age, body size and aggressiveness. Incubating females reacted to the predator by reducing nest visitation rates and increasing recess length but did not change incubation constancy. Instead, constancy was negatively associated with ambient temperature. Changes in incubation behaviour lasted about 48 h before returning to pre-exposure patterns. Our results suggest that modifying incubation rhythms is costly for female birds and the demand to regulate egg temperature efficiently limits the length of behavioural responses to the threat of nest predation.
父母鸟可能会改变孵化节律以应对捕食者的风险,但很少有研究调查暴露于捕食者后立即恢复的时间。在这里,我们研究了山蓝鸟(Sialia currucoides)对模拟的巢穴捕食者——松鼠的反应的孵化节奏。我们使用数据记录仪(iButtons)测量间歇(非间歇)长度、间歇率和孵育的持续性,发现孵育节律与女性年龄、体型和攻击性没有关系。孵化中的雌蜂对捕食者的反应是减少巢访率和增加休息时间,但不改变孵化的稳定性。相反,恒常性与环境温度呈负相关。孵育行为的变化持续了大约48小时,然后恢复到暴露前的模式。我们的研究结果表明,改变孵化节奏对雌鸟来说是昂贵的,有效调节卵温的需求限制了对巢穴捕食威胁的行为反应的长度。
{"title":"Shifting incubation rhythms in response to predation risk and the length of the response in mountain bluebirds","authors":"Simon P. Tkaczyk, Douglas P. Chivers, Karen L. Wiebe","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10250","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10250","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Parent birds may alter incubation rhythms in response to predation risk but few studies have examined the recovery time immediately after exposure to a predator. Here, we examined incubation rhythms in mountain bluebirds ( Sialia currucoides ) in response to a simulated nest predator, a taxidermy-mounted squirrel. We used data loggers (iButtons) to measure the recess (off-bout) length, recess rate, and constancy of incubation and found no relationship between incubation rhythms and female age, body size and aggressiveness. Incubating females reacted to the predator by reducing nest visitation rates and increasing recess length but did not change incubation constancy. Instead, constancy was negatively associated with ambient temperature. Changes in incubation behaviour lasted about 48 h before returning to pre-exposure patterns. Our results suggest that modifying incubation rhythms is costly for female birds and the demand to regulate egg temperature efficiently limits the length of behavioural responses to the threat of nest predation.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135092052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The divergence of mobility and activity associated with anti-predator adaptations in land snails 陆地蜗牛反捕食者适应性的迁移和活动分化
4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10249
Yuta Morii, Ryota Kimura, Rion Sato, Nana Shiobara, Honoka Maeda, Kaede Nakagawa, Ririka Ito, Reiichi Ueki
Abstract The extreme divergence of phenotypes between two closely related terrestrial gastropods, Karaftohelix editha and K. gainesi (Camaenidae, Stylommatophora), has been suggested as a result of anti-predator adaptations because K. editha and K. gainesi showed shy and bold anti-predator behaviours, respectively. In this study, we measured the crawling speeds in the laboratory and the exploration (the crawled distances during every 6 h) and the activity (the active/inactive statuses) in the field for each species. As a result, K. gainesi crawled faster under ordinary conditions and moreover upregulated its crawling speed 1.21–1.28 times after the stimulus. The activity pattern of K. editha was typical of nocturnal species, but K. gainesi remained active even under daylight. These results might suggest that K. editha and K. gainesi were deemed as shy/inactive and bold/active species, respectively, and that behavioural syndromes existed between shyness–boldness and activity personalities.
摘要两种近缘陆生腹足动物(Camaenidae, stylommatophhora)卡拉托螺旋埃迪塔(Karaftohelix editha)和甘奈西(K. gainesi)之间的表型差异很大,这可能是由于抗捕食者适应的结果,因为埃迪塔和甘奈西分别表现出害羞和大胆的抗捕食者行为。在这项研究中,我们测量了每个物种在实验室的爬行速度和探索(每6小时爬行距离)和活动(活跃/不活跃状态)。结果表明,在正常条件下,甘菊的爬行速度更快,刺激后其爬行速度上调1.21-1.28倍。伊迪沙的活动模式是典型的夜间活动,而甘菊在白天也保持活跃。这些结果可能表明,伊迪塔和盖尼西分别被认为是害羞/不活跃和大胆/活跃的物种,并且在害羞-大胆和活动人格之间存在行为综合征。
{"title":"The divergence of mobility and activity associated with anti-predator adaptations in land snails","authors":"Yuta Morii, Ryota Kimura, Rion Sato, Nana Shiobara, Honoka Maeda, Kaede Nakagawa, Ririka Ito, Reiichi Ueki","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10249","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10249","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The extreme divergence of phenotypes between two closely related terrestrial gastropods, Karaftohelix editha and K. gainesi (Camaenidae, Stylommatophora), has been suggested as a result of anti-predator adaptations because K. editha and K. gainesi showed shy and bold anti-predator behaviours, respectively. In this study, we measured the crawling speeds in the laboratory and the exploration (the crawled distances during every 6 h) and the activity (the active/inactive statuses) in the field for each species. As a result, K. gainesi crawled faster under ordinary conditions and moreover upregulated its crawling speed 1.21–1.28 times after the stimulus. The activity pattern of K. editha was typical of nocturnal species, but K. gainesi remained active even under daylight. These results might suggest that K. editha and K. gainesi were deemed as shy/inactive and bold/active species, respectively, and that behavioural syndromes existed between shyness–boldness and activity personalities.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136069688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1