首页 > 最新文献

Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
Tongue-flicking: an idiosyncratic displacement behaviour in a free-ranging and urban-dwelling population of Balinese long-tailed macaques 舌弹:巴厘岛长尾猕猴在自由放养和城市居住种群中的一种特殊位移行为
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-17 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10221
Sydney Chertoff, I. N. Wandia, J. Leca
This brief report describes the occurrence of an idiosyncratic behaviour, labelled tongue-flicking, that was performed by one subadult male long-tailed macaque living in a free-ranging population in Bali, Indonesia. Tongue-flicking may serve a similar purpose as a displacement behaviour in which the subject sticks his tongue out of his mouth and moves it either slightly up and down or in and out without bringing it all the way back into the mouth. Additionally, while abnormal behaviours in non-human animals are almost exclusively reported in captive individuals, the investigation of idiosyncratic behaviours such as tongue-flicking allows us to explore the potential occurrence of abnormal behaviours in free-ranging populations. This preliminary descriptive analysis of tongue-flicking aims to highlight the need for understanding the motivational bases and affective implications (e.g., welfare) of abnormal behaviours in captive and free-living animals.
这份简短的报告描述了印度尼西亚巴厘岛一只生活在自由放养种群中的亚成年雄性长尾猕猴的一种特殊行为,称为甩舌头。弹舌头的目的可能与一种位移行为类似,即受试者将舌头从嘴里伸出,轻轻地上下或进出,而不会一直回到嘴里。此外,虽然非人类动物的异常行为几乎只在圈养个体中报道,但对弹舌等特殊行为的调查使我们能够探索自由放养种群中异常行为的潜在发生。这项对舌头弹动的初步描述性分析旨在强调理解圈养和自由生活动物异常行为的动机基础和情感含义(如福利)的必要性。
{"title":"Tongue-flicking: an idiosyncratic displacement behaviour in a free-ranging and urban-dwelling population of Balinese long-tailed macaques","authors":"Sydney Chertoff, I. N. Wandia, J. Leca","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10221","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10221","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000This brief report describes the occurrence of an idiosyncratic behaviour, labelled tongue-flicking, that was performed by one subadult male long-tailed macaque living in a free-ranging population in Bali, Indonesia. Tongue-flicking may serve a similar purpose as a displacement behaviour in which the subject sticks his tongue out of his mouth and moves it either slightly up and down or in and out without bringing it all the way back into the mouth. Additionally, while abnormal behaviours in non-human animals are almost exclusively reported in captive individuals, the investigation of idiosyncratic behaviours such as tongue-flicking allows us to explore the potential occurrence of abnormal behaviours in free-ranging populations. This preliminary descriptive analysis of tongue-flicking aims to highlight the need for understanding the motivational bases and affective implications (e.g., welfare) of abnormal behaviours in captive and free-living animals.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49309108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ethogram for blacktip reef sharks (Carcharhinus melanopterus) 黑鳍礁鲨(Carcharhinus melanopterus)特征图
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-28 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10213
I. F. Porcher
The chondrichthyan lineage diverged from the osteichthyan line around 440 million years ago, resulting in a vast evolutionary gulf between modern elasmobranchs and other vertebrates. Though this has supported the assumption that as an ancient line, elasmobranchs are dangerously stupid, intelligent actions including social interactions have been noted in the field, while laboratory studies have confirmed a variety of cognitive capacities. Yet, due to fear of sharks and the difficulties of observing them in their aquatic environments, few ethological studies have been done, so their natural behaviour remains little known. On noting that the blacktip reef shark, Carcharhinus melanopterus, displayed complex actions during incidental meetings, a long-term ethological study of the species was carried out on the north shore of Mo’orea Island, French Polynesia. During the 6.5 years of the study, new behaviours continued to present. The 35 context-specific actions identified as comprising the behavioural repertoire of C. melanopterus are described.
软骨鱼纲谱系大约在4.4亿年前从骨鱼纲分化而来,导致现代蓝鳃类和其他脊椎动物之间存在巨大的进化鸿沟。尽管这支持了一种假设,即作为一个古老的谱系,蓝鳃类动物是危险的愚蠢,但包括社交互动在内的智能行为在该领域已经被注意到,而实验室研究已经证实了各种认知能力。然而,由于对鲨鱼的恐惧以及在水生环境中观察它们的困难,很少进行动物行为学研究,因此它们的自然行为仍然鲜为人知。注意到黑鳍礁鲨Carcharhinus melanopterus在偶然的会议上表现出复杂的行为,在法属波利尼西亚莫奥里亚岛北岸对该物种进行了长期的行为学研究。在6.5年的研究中,新的行为不断出现。描述了35种特定于环境的动作,这些动作被确定为构成黑翅蠊的行为谱系。
{"title":"Ethogram for blacktip reef sharks (Carcharhinus melanopterus)","authors":"I. F. Porcher","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10213","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10213","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000The chondrichthyan lineage diverged from the osteichthyan line around 440 million years ago, resulting in a vast evolutionary gulf between modern elasmobranchs and other vertebrates. Though this has supported the assumption that as an ancient line, elasmobranchs are dangerously stupid, intelligent actions including social interactions have been noted in the field, while laboratory studies have confirmed a variety of cognitive capacities. Yet, due to fear of sharks and the difficulties of observing them in their aquatic environments, few ethological studies have been done, so their natural behaviour remains little known. On noting that the blacktip reef shark, Carcharhinus melanopterus, displayed complex actions during incidental meetings, a long-term ethological study of the species was carried out on the north shore of Mo’orea Island, French Polynesia. During the 6.5 years of the study, new behaviours continued to present. The 35 context-specific actions identified as comprising the behavioural repertoire of C. melanopterus are described.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46214732","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Good fences make good neighbours: territorial male Cape fur seals use spatial acoustic map of neighbours 好围栏造就好邻居:领地雄性开普毛皮海豹使用邻居的空间声学地图
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-24 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10218
M. Martin, T. Gridley, S. Elwen, I. Charrier
In territorial species, individual recognition among neighbouring males is likely to reduce energy expenses and risk of injury associated with the costly period of maintaining territory during the breeding season. This study explored neighbour–stranger vocal recognition in male Cape fur seals, one of the most colonial and polygynous mammals. Playback experiments revealed that territorial males were able to recognise the calls of their neighbours, in combination with their relative spatial position to their own harem. No ‘dear-enemy’ nor ‘nasty neighbour’ effects were detected. However, the strongest responses observed were towards the calls of familiar neighbours played back from an incongruent location, simulating a situation in which a neighbour is outside its own territory. The colony structure and movements of the seals across the day could explain such results. This study has implications for understanding how vocal signals regulate interactions among males in polygynous mammals, particularly during the competitive mating period.
在有领地意识的物种中,邻近雄性之间的个体识别可能会减少能量消耗和在繁殖季节维护领地所带来的伤害风险。这项研究探索了雄性角海狗(一种最具群居性和一夫多妻制的哺乳动物)对邻居和陌生人的声音识别。回放实验显示,有领地意识的雄性能够识别邻居的叫声,结合它们与自己后宫的相对空间位置。没有发现“死敌”和“讨厌的邻居”效应。然而,观察到的最强烈的反应是对熟悉的邻居从一个不一致的位置播放的叫声,模拟邻居在自己的领土之外的情况。海豹在一天中的群体结构和活动可以解释这些结果。这项研究对于理解声音信号如何调节一夫多妻制哺乳动物雄性之间的相互作用,特别是在竞争性交配期间,具有重要意义。
{"title":"Good fences make good neighbours: territorial male Cape fur seals use spatial acoustic map of neighbours","authors":"M. Martin, T. Gridley, S. Elwen, I. Charrier","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10218","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10218","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In territorial species, individual recognition among neighbouring males is likely to reduce energy expenses and risk of injury associated with the costly period of maintaining territory during the breeding season. This study explored neighbour–stranger vocal recognition in male Cape fur seals, one of the most colonial and polygynous mammals. Playback experiments revealed that territorial males were able to recognise the calls of their neighbours, in combination with their relative spatial position to their own harem. No ‘dear-enemy’ nor ‘nasty neighbour’ effects were detected. However, the strongest responses observed were towards the calls of familiar neighbours played back from an incongruent location, simulating a situation in which a neighbour is outside its own territory. The colony structure and movements of the seals across the day could explain such results. This study has implications for understanding how vocal signals regulate interactions among males in polygynous mammals, particularly during the competitive mating period.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47693115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predator and duck behaviours at depredated nests in wetlands of Great Salt Lake, Utah 犹他州大盐湖湿地被掠夺巢穴中的捕食者和鸭子行为
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-21 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10217
M. Bell, M. Conover
Nest depredation is one of the greatest threats posed to ground-nesting ducks. We employed cameras to monitor 164 duck nests (71 cinnamon teal, Spatula cyanoptera, 44 gadwall, Mareca strepera, 38 mallard, Anas platyrhynchos, and 11 nests of unknown species) in the wetlands surrounding Great Salt Lake, from 2015–2021. Of the 164 nests, 21% were successful, 73% were depredated and 7% were abandoned. We observed predators at 99 of the 119 depredated nests; predators at 20 nests went undetected. Raccoons (Procyon lotor, depredated nests) and striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis, ) were the most common of the 99 nest predators recorded. Other predators that depredated nests included long-tailed weasels (Mustela frenata), northern harriers (Circus hudsonius), California gulls (Larus californicus), Sandhill cranes (Antigone canadensis), common ravens (Corvus corax), coyotes (Canis latrans) and red foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Neither the number of eggs removed per depredation event nor the number of eggs remaining varied by predator species. Depredated nests were easier for predators to find than undisturbed, incubated nests, resulting in 68% of depredated nests being visited by multiple predators. All hens detected the approach of a predator and flushed before the predator reached the nest; no hens attempted to defend their nest or attack the predator. Only 21% of hens returned to their depredated nest, and those that did remained off their nest an average of 33 h and 23 h after their nest was depredated by a raccoon or skunk, respectively. Seventeen percent of hens resumed incubation of their depredated nest, but only 1 nest to which a hen returned successfully hatched an egg. Depredation events of raccoons and skunks were not distributed randomly during the 24-hour day, but rather occurred most often during the night and nautical twilight, and rarely during the day. Depredation events of avian predators occurred during the day, rarely during twilight, and none during the night. Depredation events during the night were more likely when the wind was calm but temperature, humidity, and actual moon illumination had no impact. Depredation events by skunks and raccoons occurred more often during the 1st and 4th phases of the moon (new moon) than in the 2nd or 3rd phase.
巢穴掠夺是对地面筑巢鸭构成的最大威胁之一。2015年至2021年,我们使用摄像机监测了大盐湖周围湿地中的164个鸭巢(71个肉桂柚木、紫蝶、44个牛蛙、马六甲、38个绿头鸭、Anas platyrhynchos和11个未知物种的巢)。在164个巢穴中,21%成功,73%被掠夺,7%被遗弃。我们在119个被掠夺的巢穴中的99个观察到捕食者;20个巢穴中的捕食者没有被发现。浣熊(Procyon lotor,掠夺巢穴)和条纹臭鼬(Mephitis Mephitis,)是有记录的99种巢穴捕食者中最常见的。其他掠夺巢穴的捕食者包括长尾鼬(Mustela frenata)、北鹞(Circus hudsonius)、加利福尼亚海鸥(Larus californicus)、沙丘鹤(Antigone canadensis)、普通乌鸦(Corvus corax)、郊狼(Canis latrans)和赤狐(Vulpes Vulpes)。每次掠夺事件中被移除的卵子数量和剩余的卵子数量都不因捕食者物种而异。捕食者更容易找到被掠夺的巢穴,而不是未受干扰的孵化巢穴,导致68%的被掠夺巢穴被多个捕食者造访。所有母鸡都察觉到捕食者的靠近,并在捕食者到达巢穴之前脸红;没有母鸡试图保卫自己的巢穴或攻击捕食者。只有21%的母鸡回到了被掠夺的巢穴,而那些回到巢穴的母鸡分别在被浣熊或臭鼬掠夺巢穴后平均33小时和23小时离开巢穴。17%的母鸡恢复了它们被掠夺的巢穴的孵化,但只有一窝母鸡成功孵化出了蛋。浣熊和臭鼬的掠夺事件在24小时内并不是随机分布的,而是最常发生在夜间和海上黄昏,很少发生在白天。鸟类捕食者的掠夺事件发生在白天,很少发生在黄昏,也没有发生在夜间。当风很平静,但温度、湿度和实际的月球照明没有影响时,夜间发生剥夺事件的可能性更大。臭鼬和浣熊的掠夺事件发生在月球的第一和第四阶段(新月),而不是第二或第三阶段。
{"title":"Predator and duck behaviours at depredated nests in wetlands of Great Salt Lake, Utah","authors":"M. Bell, M. Conover","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10217","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10217","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Nest depredation is one of the greatest threats posed to ground-nesting ducks. We employed cameras to monitor 164 duck nests (71 cinnamon teal, Spatula cyanoptera, 44 gadwall, Mareca strepera, 38 mallard, Anas platyrhynchos, and 11 nests of unknown species) in the wetlands surrounding Great Salt Lake, from 2015–2021. Of the 164 nests, 21% were successful, 73% were depredated and 7% were abandoned. We observed predators at 99 of the 119 depredated nests; predators at 20 nests went undetected. Raccoons (Procyon lotor, depredated nests) and striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis, ) were the most common of the 99 nest predators recorded. Other predators that depredated nests included long-tailed weasels (Mustela frenata), northern harriers (Circus hudsonius), California gulls (Larus californicus), Sandhill cranes (Antigone canadensis), common ravens (Corvus corax), coyotes (Canis latrans) and red foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Neither the number of eggs removed per depredation event nor the number of eggs remaining varied by predator species. Depredated nests were easier for predators to find than undisturbed, incubated nests, resulting in 68% of depredated nests being visited by multiple predators. All hens detected the approach of a predator and flushed before the predator reached the nest; no hens attempted to defend their nest or attack the predator. Only 21% of hens returned to their depredated nest, and those that did remained off their nest an average of 33 h and 23 h after their nest was depredated by a raccoon or skunk, respectively. Seventeen percent of hens resumed incubation of their depredated nest, but only 1 nest to which a hen returned successfully hatched an egg. Depredation events of raccoons and skunks were not distributed randomly during the 24-hour day, but rather occurred most often during the night and nautical twilight, and rarely during the day. Depredation events of avian predators occurred during the day, rarely during twilight, and none during the night. Depredation events during the night were more likely when the wind was calm but temperature, humidity, and actual moon illumination had no impact. Depredation events by skunks and raccoons occurred more often during the 1st and 4th phases of the moon (new moon) than in the 2nd or 3rd phase.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49550859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A review of the behaviours of the Chondrichthyes: a multi-species ethogram for the chimaeras, sharks, and rays 软骨鱼类行为的综述:嵌合体、鲨鱼和鳐鱼的多物种谱系图
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-12 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10214
A. Klimley, I. F. Porcher, Eric E. G. Clua, H. L. Pratt
In this review of the behavioural patterns of chondrichthyan fishes, we have strived to produce a comprehensive catalogue of events and states and develop standardized terminology. Hence, actions that are slightly different, will be pooled under inclusive titles. Those used by different investigators are included in quotations within the textual descriptions of the motor patterns. This standardized ethogram will ideally lead to an increase in inter-observer reliability, giving researchers more confidence when reading colleagues’ papers that report behaviours that appear similar to theirs despite being described for different species. The descriptions are presented under the following categories: (1) maintenance (2) courtship (3) filter feeding (4) scavenging (5) predation (6) sociality (7) aggression and (8) defence. The many actions are illustrated by line drawings and photographs in composite figures with an attempt to provide an example of each action for a chimaera, shark, and ray. The diversity of patterns is evident from this ethogram, consistent with observation that the brain-to-body mass ratios of cartilaginous fishes are greater than a third of the bird species and greater than those for some mammalian species. The major impetus for assembling this ethogram is to demonstrate the diversity of behaviours exhibited by members of the Chondrichthyes and to dispel the apocryphal belief that members of this taxon are ‘simple feeding machines’.
在这篇关于软骨鱼行为模式的综述中,我们努力制作一个事件和状态的综合目录,并开发标准化术语。因此,略有不同的操作将被合并到包含标题下。由不同研究者使用的那些包含在运动模式文本描述中的引文中。理想情况下,这种标准化的谱图将增加观察者之间的可靠性,使研究人员在阅读同事的论文时更有信心,这些论文报告的行为与他们的相似,尽管描述的是不同的物种。描述可分为以下几类:(1)维持(2)求偶(3)滤食(4)食腐(5)捕食(6)群居(7)攻击(8)防御。许多动作都是通过线条图和合成图的照片来说明的,试图为chimaera, shark和ray提供每个动作的例子。从这幅谱图中可以明显看出模式的多样性,这与观察结果相一致,即软骨鱼的脑与身体的质量比大于鸟类的三分之一,并且大于某些哺乳动物物种。收集这张族谱的主要动机是为了证明软骨鱼目成员表现出的行为多样性,并消除这种分类单元成员是“简单的进食机器”的虚假信念。
{"title":"A review of the behaviours of the Chondrichthyes: a multi-species ethogram for the chimaeras, sharks, and rays","authors":"A. Klimley, I. F. Porcher, Eric E. G. Clua, H. L. Pratt","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10214","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10214","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In this review of the behavioural patterns of chondrichthyan fishes, we have strived to produce a comprehensive catalogue of events and states and develop standardized terminology. Hence, actions that are slightly different, will be pooled under inclusive titles. Those used by different investigators are included in quotations within the textual descriptions of the motor patterns. This standardized ethogram will ideally lead to an increase in inter-observer reliability, giving researchers more confidence when reading colleagues’ papers that report behaviours that appear similar to theirs despite being described for different species. The descriptions are presented under the following categories: (1) maintenance (2) courtship (3) filter feeding (4) scavenging (5) predation (6) sociality (7) aggression and (8) defence. The many actions are illustrated by line drawings and photographs in composite figures with an attempt to provide an example of each action for a chimaera, shark, and ray. The diversity of patterns is evident from this ethogram, consistent with observation that the brain-to-body mass ratios of cartilaginous fishes are greater than a third of the bird species and greater than those for some mammalian species. The major impetus for assembling this ethogram is to demonstrate the diversity of behaviours exhibited by members of the Chondrichthyes and to dispel the apocryphal belief that members of this taxon are ‘simple feeding machines’.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42360136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Opposing effects of vigilance and foraging on escape behaviour in hooded crows 警惕和觅食对戴帽乌鸦逃跑行为的相反影响
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-03-28 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10216
Ivana Novčič
Escape represents an important component of animals’ antipredator behaviour entailing both benefits and costs dependent on a moment an animal flees upon predator’s approach. In this study, I examined how the level of vigilance and foraging activity affected escape decision in the urban hooded crow Corvus cornix, predicting that alert distance (AD) and flight initiation distance (FID) should be positively affected by the level of vigilance and negatively affected by foraging activity, whereas buffer distance (BD) should be negatively affected by the level of vigilance and positively affected by foraging activity. Using LMMs it was shown that percent of time crows allocated to vigilance was positively correlated with AD and FID, whereas foraging activity of crows had negative impact on AD and FID. In addition, both AD and FID were positively related to starting distance (SD), while AD was also positively influenced by tree coverage. BD was positively affected by foraging activity and AD. This study demonstrated that more vigilant birds detected predators earlier, which is in accordance with the major function of vigilance. Also, it was shown that foraging crows delayed their escape, once the predator has been detected, as benefits of delayed flight, such as feeding on a profitable food item or within a profitable patch, may outweigh costs, which is consistent with the optimal escape theory.
逃跑是动物反捕食者行为的一个重要组成部分,它的收益和成本都取决于动物在捕食者接近时逃跑的那一刻。本研究考察了警惕水平和觅食活动对城市冠鸦逃跑决策的影响,预测警戒距离(AD)和飞行起始距离(FID)受到警惕水平的正影响,觅食活动的负影响;缓冲距离(BD)受到警惕水平的负影响,觅食活动的正影响。利用lmm模型研究发现,乌鸦的警戒时间百分比与AD和FID呈正相关,而乌鸦的觅食活动对AD和FID有负相关影响。此外,AD和FID都与起始距离(SD)呈正相关,而AD也受到树木盖度的正影响。采食活动和AD对BD有正向影响。研究表明,警惕性越强的鸟类发现捕食者的时间越早,这与警惕性的主要功能是一致的。此外,研究还表明,一旦发现捕食者,觅食的乌鸦会推迟逃跑,因为延迟飞行的好处,比如在有利可图的食物上觅食或在有利可图的斑块上觅食,可能会超过成本,这与最优逃跑理论是一致的。
{"title":"Opposing effects of vigilance and foraging on escape behaviour in hooded crows","authors":"Ivana Novčič","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10216","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10216","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Escape represents an important component of animals’ antipredator behaviour entailing both benefits and costs dependent on a moment an animal flees upon predator’s approach. In this study, I examined how the level of vigilance and foraging activity affected escape decision in the urban hooded crow Corvus cornix, predicting that alert distance (AD) and flight initiation distance (FID) should be positively affected by the level of vigilance and negatively affected by foraging activity, whereas buffer distance (BD) should be negatively affected by the level of vigilance and positively affected by foraging activity. Using LMMs it was shown that percent of time crows allocated to vigilance was positively correlated with AD and FID, whereas foraging activity of crows had negative impact on AD and FID. In addition, both AD and FID were positively related to starting distance (SD), while AD was also positively influenced by tree coverage. BD was positively affected by foraging activity and AD. This study demonstrated that more vigilant birds detected predators earlier, which is in accordance with the major function of vigilance. Also, it was shown that foraging crows delayed their escape, once the predator has been detected, as benefits of delayed flight, such as feeding on a profitable food item or within a profitable patch, may outweigh costs, which is consistent with the optimal escape theory.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43154210","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sharing your snack: unusual observation of a chick–chick feeding occurrence in colonial king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus) 分享你的零食:对殖民地帝企鹅(Aptenodytes patagonicus)雏鸟喂养事件的不寻常观察
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-03-21 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10212
A. Cillard, Tatiana Fuentes Rodriguez, J. Robin, P. Bize, A. Stier, Vincent A. Viblanc
King penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) are colonial seabirds presenting an extraordinary breeding cycle. Parents take over 14 months to raise a single chick to independence, upon which fledglings depart at sea for more than a year. Juveniles often return to the colony within the three austral summers following departure, and before the age of first reproduction (around 5–6 years old), possibly to acquire the essential skills involved in breeding. Little to nothing is known on the acquisition of parental behaviour. Here, we report an anecdotal, yet highly unusual, observation of chick–chick feeding behaviour in this species. The behaviour is highly unusual in that two non-sibling chicks, not yet independent, and hatched at different times (early and late) of the same breeding season were observed, the older chick feeding the younger one. Whereas alloparental feeding is known to occur in this species, this is the first reported observation of a chick–chick feeding event. This unusual behaviour raises the question of whether the early social environment plays a role in the acquisition of essential parenting skills in this species.
帝企鹅(Aptenodytes patagonicus)是一种具有非凡繁殖周期的殖民地海鸟。父母要花14个月的时间把一只雏鸟培养成独立的雏鸟,然后雏鸟在海上离开一年多。幼鸟通常在离开后的三个南方夏季,在第一次繁殖年龄(大约5-6岁)之前返回殖民地,可能是为了获得繁殖的基本技能。关于父母行为的习得,我们几乎一无所知。在这里,我们报告了一个轶事,但非常不寻常的观察,在这个物种的小鸡喂养行为。这种行为是非常不寻常的,因为观察到两只没有兄弟姐妹的小鸡,还没有独立,在同一繁殖季节的不同时间(早期和晚期)孵化,大的小鸡喂养小的。虽然同种异体摄食已知发生在该物种中,但这是首次报道观察到小鸡与小鸡的摄食事件。这种不寻常的行为引发了一个问题,即早期的社会环境是否在这个物种获得基本育儿技能方面发挥了作用。
{"title":"Sharing your snack: unusual observation of a chick–chick feeding occurrence in colonial king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus)","authors":"A. Cillard, Tatiana Fuentes Rodriguez, J. Robin, P. Bize, A. Stier, Vincent A. Viblanc","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10212","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10212","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000King penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) are colonial seabirds presenting an extraordinary breeding cycle. Parents take over 14 months to raise a single chick to independence, upon which fledglings depart at sea for more than a year. Juveniles often return to the colony within the three austral summers following departure, and before the age of first reproduction (around 5–6 years old), possibly to acquire the essential skills involved in breeding. Little to nothing is known on the acquisition of parental behaviour. Here, we report an anecdotal, yet highly unusual, observation of chick–chick feeding behaviour in this species. The behaviour is highly unusual in that two non-sibling chicks, not yet independent, and hatched at different times (early and late) of the same breeding season were observed, the older chick feeding the younger one. Whereas alloparental feeding is known to occur in this species, this is the first reported observation of a chick–chick feeding event. This unusual behaviour raises the question of whether the early social environment plays a role in the acquisition of essential parenting skills in this species.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45854428","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A historical approach to describing the complex behaviour of a large species of carnivorous shark. Case study No. 1: the scalloped hammerhead, Sphyrna lewini 描述一种大型食肉鲨鱼复杂行为的历史方法。案例研究1:双髻鲨(Sphyrna lewini
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-03-21 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10211
A. Klimley
A historical example, case study No. 1, is given illustrating the complexity of interactions between sharks in their natural environment. Scalloped hammerhead sharks, Sphyrna lewini, form polarized schools at offshore islands and seamounts. Females perform a display while inside a school to force smaller individuals to the periphery. The Corkscrew consists of a rapid burst of swimming into a tight looping trajectory, with the shark rotating its torso almost 360° on its longitudinal axis. This conveys a shark’s intent to contact the recipient with a Hit. This is competition for a position at the centre of the school. Males dash into the schools, directing emphatic tail beats to one side with a Torso Thrust, propelling the shark‘s anterior torso laterally, to pair with a dominant female. The pair then moves outside the school to copulate in midwater with the male biting the female’s body and inserting his clasper within her uterus.
一个历史上的例子,案例研究1,说明了鲨鱼在自然环境中相互作用的复杂性。双髻鲨,双髻鲨,在近海岛屿和海底山形成两极分化的鱼群。雌性在鱼群内表演表演,迫使较小的个体离开。在“开瓶器”中,鲨鱼迅速游向一个紧密的环形轨迹,躯干在其纵轴上旋转近360度。这传达了一个鲨鱼的意图,以打击联系收件人。这是对学校中心位置的竞争。雄鲨冲进鱼群,用躯干推力将有力的尾巴拍打向一侧,推动鲨鱼的前躯干向一侧移动,与占统治地位的雌鲨配对。然后,这对情侣游到鱼群外,在水中交配,雄鱼咬着雌鱼的身体,把他的爪子插入她的子宫。
{"title":"A historical approach to describing the complex behaviour of a large species of carnivorous shark. Case study No. 1: the scalloped hammerhead, Sphyrna lewini","authors":"A. Klimley","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10211","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10211","url":null,"abstract":"A historical example, case study No. 1, is given illustrating the complexity of interactions between sharks in their natural environment. Scalloped hammerhead sharks, Sphyrna lewini, form polarized schools at offshore islands and seamounts. Females perform a display while inside a school to force smaller individuals to the periphery. The Corkscrew consists of a rapid burst of swimming into a tight looping trajectory, with the shark rotating its torso almost 360° on its longitudinal axis. This conveys a shark’s intent to contact the recipient with a Hit. This is competition for a position at the centre of the school. Males dash into the schools, directing emphatic tail beats to one side with a Torso Thrust, propelling the shark‘s anterior torso laterally, to pair with a dominant female. The pair then moves outside the school to copulate in midwater with the male biting the female’s body and inserting his clasper within her uterus.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64591590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Eyes over stomach: companion dogs choose the larger quantity by sight, irrespective of the actual reward eaten 眼睛重于胃:同伴狗会根据视觉选择数量较大的食物,而不考虑实际吃到的奖励
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-03-21 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10215
M. Loconsole, L. Regolin, G. Marliani, M. Mattioli, Elena Pietschmann, P. Accorsi, S. Normando
Domestic dogs tested in a free-choice task between two sets of food prefer that of a larger quantity. A recent study pointed out the critical role of the first trial. Dogs succeeded only after being allowed at least once to eat the selected food. Here we explore the importance of the actual experience of consuming the selected reward on dogs’ capability to redirect their choice to solve numerical discriminations. Dogs tested in the 2 vs 4 and 1 vs 8 comparisons could never consume the quantity selected but always obtained a single dogs’ treat. Despite never experiencing a difference in the eaten quantity, dogs discriminated between the two sets, preferring the larger. Whereas they behaved at chance at first choice, they successfully redirected their preference toward the larger set already on their second trial. We discuss our results in terms of motivational biases toward the larger quantity that can bear relevant ecological value.
在一项自由选择两组食物的任务中,家犬更喜欢数量较多的那一组。最近的一项研究指出了第一次试验的关键作用。狗只有在被允许至少吃一次选定的食物后才能成功。在这里,我们探讨了实际经验的重要性,消费选定的奖励对狗的能力,以重新定向他们的选择,以解决数字歧视。在2比4和1比8对比中测试的狗永远不会消耗所选择的数量,但总是获得单一狗的食物。尽管从来没有经历过吃的数量的差异,狗区分了两组,更喜欢大的。尽管他们在第一次选择中表现得很偶然,但在第二次试验中,他们成功地将自己的偏好转向了更大的一组。我们讨论了我们的结果,动机偏向于更大的数量,可以承担相关的生态价值。
{"title":"Eyes over stomach: companion dogs choose the larger quantity by sight, irrespective of the actual reward eaten","authors":"M. Loconsole, L. Regolin, G. Marliani, M. Mattioli, Elena Pietschmann, P. Accorsi, S. Normando","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10215","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10215","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Domestic dogs tested in a free-choice task between two sets of food prefer that of a larger quantity. A recent study pointed out the critical role of the first trial. Dogs succeeded only after being allowed at least once to eat the selected food. Here we explore the importance of the actual experience of consuming the selected reward on dogs’ capability to redirect their choice to solve numerical discriminations. Dogs tested in the 2 vs 4 and 1 vs 8 comparisons could never consume the quantity selected but always obtained a single dogs’ treat. Despite never experiencing a difference in the eaten quantity, dogs discriminated between the two sets, preferring the larger. Whereas they behaved at chance at first choice, they successfully redirected their preference toward the larger set already on their second trial. We discuss our results in terms of motivational biases toward the larger quantity that can bear relevant ecological value.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48073743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ring a bell? Variable recognition of a multicomponent auditory stimulus associated with predation risk by zebrafish responding to full and partial matches 有印象斑马鱼对完全匹配和部分匹配的反应对与捕食风险相关的多组分听觉刺激的可变识别
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-02-15 DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10210
Molly I. M. Johnson, Kathryn A. Hanson, Jacklyn M. Simon, Ananda Shastri, B. Wisenden
Many fishes learn to recognize correlates of predation risk by pairing novel stimuli with injury-released chemical cues released from damaged epidermal tissues. Here, zebrafish were conditioned to associate predation risk with a three-note auditory stimulus C5 (523.25 Hz) + E5 (659.25 Hz) + G5 (783.99 Hz), then tested for responses to either only one tone (C), two of the components (C + G) or the full three-note chord (C + E + G). Zebrafish conditioned with alarm cues and C + E + G responded significantly more intensely to the full C + E + G stimulus or to partial representation of the original mix (C + G) than they did to the single element (C) of the original C + E + G conditioning stimulus. The lack of a response to the single component alone may be a failure to recognize the stimulus or an interpretation that the partial component is an indicator of low risk.
许多鱼类通过将新的刺激与受损表皮组织释放的损伤释放的化学线索配对来学习识别捕食风险的相关性。在这里,斑马鱼习惯于将捕食风险与三个音调的听觉刺激C5 (523.25 Hz) + E5 (659.25 Hz) + G5 (783.99 Hz)联系起来,然后测试对一个音调(C)的反应,在警报提示和C + E + G条件下,斑马鱼对完整的C + E + G刺激或原始组合的部分表征(C + G)的反应明显强于对原始C + E + G条件刺激的单一元素(C)的反应。对单一成分缺乏反应可能是未能识别刺激或将部分成分解释为低风险指标。
{"title":"Ring a bell? Variable recognition of a multicomponent auditory stimulus associated with predation risk by zebrafish responding to full and partial matches","authors":"Molly I. M. Johnson, Kathryn A. Hanson, Jacklyn M. Simon, Ananda Shastri, B. Wisenden","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10210","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Many fishes learn to recognize correlates of predation risk by pairing novel stimuli with injury-released chemical cues released from damaged epidermal tissues. Here, zebrafish were conditioned to associate predation risk with a three-note auditory stimulus C5 (523.25 Hz) + E5 (659.25 Hz) + G5 (783.99 Hz), then tested for responses to either only one tone (C), two of the components (C + G) or the full three-note chord (C + E + G). Zebrafish conditioned with alarm cues and C + E + G responded significantly more intensely to the full C + E + G stimulus or to partial representation of the original mix (C + G) than they did to the single element (C) of the original C + E + G conditioning stimulus. The lack of a response to the single component alone may be a failure to recognize the stimulus or an interpretation that the partial component is an indicator of low risk.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46142469","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1