The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; N = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, N = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.
{"title":"Empathy and parenthood: The moderating role of maternal trait empathy on parental burnout","authors":"Tamar Kadosh-Laor, Liat Israeli-Ran, Ido Shalev, Florina Uzefovsky","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12640","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12640","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; <i>N</i> = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, <i>N</i> = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"605-620"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12640","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9778816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, Lia Kvavilashvili, Abbigail Ely, Mark Gill, Gemma Herbert
Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.
{"title":"Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study","authors":"Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, Lia Kvavilashvili, Abbigail Ely, Mark Gill, Gemma Herbert","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12639","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12639","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"580-604"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12639","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9854772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Changlin Luo, Mengyan Zhu, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma
Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.
{"title":"Food word processing in Chinese reading: A study of restrained eaters","authors":"Changlin Luo, Mengyan Zhu, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12638","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12638","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"476-494"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9273486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Nuria Victoria Aguerre, Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza, Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina, María Teresa Bajo
While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.
{"title":"Electrophysiological correlates of dispositional mindfulness: A quantitative and complexity EEG study","authors":"Nuria Victoria Aguerre, Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza, Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina, María Teresa Bajo","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12636","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12636","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"566-579"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12636","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9790273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Maddison Lloyd, Nicole Sugden, Matt Thomas, Andrew McGrath, Clive Skilbeck
The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond - Snaith, 1983) is widely used; however, its factor structure is unclear, with studies reporting differing unidimensional, two-factor and three-factor models. We aimed to address some key theoretical and methodological issues contributing to inconsistencies in HADS structures across samples. We reviewed existing HADS models and compared their fit using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We also investigated methodological effects by comparing factor structures derived from Rasch and Principal Components Analysis (PCA) methods, as well as effects of a negative wording factor. An Australian community-dwelling sample consisting of 189 females and 158 males aged 17–86 (M = 35.73, SD = 17.41) completed the 14-item HADS. The Rasch Analysis, PCA and CFA all supported the original two-factor structure. Although some three-factor models had good fit, they had unacceptable reliability. In the CFA, a hierarchical bifactor model with a general distress factor and uncorrelated depression and anxiety subscales produced the best fit, but the general factor was not unidimensional. The addition of a negative wording factor improved model fit. These findings highlight the effects of differing methodologies in producing inconsistent HADS factor structures across studies. Further replication of model fit across samples and refinement of the HADS items is warranted.
{"title":"The structure of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: Theoretical and methodological considerations","authors":"Maddison Lloyd, Nicole Sugden, Matt Thomas, Andrew McGrath, Clive Skilbeck","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12637","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12637","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond - Snaith, 1983) is widely used; however, its factor structure is unclear, with studies reporting differing unidimensional, two-factor and three-factor models. We aimed to address some key theoretical and methodological issues contributing to inconsistencies in HADS structures across samples. We reviewed existing HADS models and compared their fit using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We also investigated methodological effects by comparing factor structures derived from Rasch and Principal Components Analysis (PCA) methods, as well as effects of a negative wording factor. An Australian community-dwelling sample consisting of 189 females and 158 males aged 17–86 (<i>M</i> = 35.73, <i>SD</i> = 17.41) completed the 14-item HADS. The Rasch Analysis, PCA and CFA all supported the original two-factor structure. Although some three-factor models had good fit, they had unacceptable reliability. In the CFA, a hierarchical bifactor model with a general distress factor and uncorrelated depression and anxiety subscales produced the best fit, but the general factor was not unidimensional. The addition of a negative wording factor improved model fit. These findings highlight the effects of differing methodologies in producing inconsistent HADS factor structures across studies. Further replication of model fit across samples and refinement of the HADS items is warranted.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"457-475"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12637","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9636389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
<p>Science relies on the validity of concepts, especially when these come from other disciplines. The differentiation of human groups according to phenotypic appearance, for example the colour of their skin, seemed so obvious that after centuries only genetics could fundamentally revise this concept. For 50 years, attempts have been made to break away from racial categorization, among others in the recent Jena Declaration of 2019 (Fischer et al., <span>2019</span>). There is no biological justification for categorizing people into discrete groups; on the contrary, racism seeks a biological legitimation in its justification.</p><p>This Special Issue takes up the enormous challenge of how to deal with the realization that ‘race’ in humans is not a scientifically tenable category but that ‘race’ permeates psychological processing (Editorial of this Special Issue) especially in fields of psychology, for which the term is constitutive, such as the ‘other-‘race’-effect’. Other sciences may be curious to see how psychology discusses the downgrading of ‘races’ to groups of increasingly higher resolution or, for example, the individual-differences approach, and finally how giving up a concept may open new doors and provide new opportunities.</p><p>People have been categorizing each other based on appearance for millennia, as is still shown by the ongoing controversy on ancient Egyptian ‘races’. In the 19th century, early evolutionary thinking began to remodel this practice of classifying human groups using phenotypes, culminating in ideas of superior human ‘races’ and eugenics. An influential figure here is Herbert Spencer, who conceived the term ‘evolution’ before and differently from Darwin (Köchy, <span>2007</span>), and from whom the expression ‘survival of the fittest’ originated. He founded ‘biological Spencerism’ in Victorian England (Freeman et al., <span>1974</span>), Social Darwinism (even before German biologist Ernst Haeckel) and the supposedly biologically based form of white superiority. According to his theory, different groups of people would have inherent and distinct characteristics, which are displayed, for example, in the colour of their skin. Those characteristics would be linked to the population ancestry of its bearer.</p><p>Differences between human populations were further imagined using biological concepts, such as morphospecies, reflecting a static worldview, in which species were defined by certain characteristics selected by systematists. In essence, human groups were seen as immutable ‘classes’ (Mahner & Bunge, <span>1997</span>). This, however, does not reflect objective biology but rather represents a flawed construct of the human mind. Taxonomy is a rather static way of thinking, uninfluenced by the idea of evolution.</p><p>The central question was, and still is, the relationship of between-group variation (intergroup variation) to individual variability within a group (intragroup variation). Our view of human biologic
科学依赖于概念的有效性,尤其是当这些概念来自其他学科时。人类群体根据表型(例如皮肤颜色)的差异,似乎是如此明显,以至于几个世纪后,只有遗传学才能从根本上改变这一概念。50年来,人们一直试图摆脱种族分类,其中包括最近的《2019年耶拿宣言》(Fischer et al., 2019)。没有生物学上的理由把人分成不同的群体;相反,种族主义在其辩护中寻求生物学上的正当性。本期特刊提出了一个巨大的挑战,即如何认识到人类的“种族”并不是一个科学上站住脚的范畴,而是“种族”渗透到心理过程中(本期特刊社论),特别是在心理学领域,这个术语是构成要素,比如“他者-“种族”-效应。其他科学可能会好奇地看到心理学如何讨论将“种族”降级为分辨率越来越高的群体,或者,例如,个体差异方法,以及最终放弃一个概念如何打开新的大门并提供新的机会。几千年来,人们一直根据外貌对彼此进行分类,关于古埃及“种族”的争论至今仍在继续。在19世纪,早期的进化思想开始重塑这种使用表型对人类群体进行分类的做法,最终产生了优越人类“种族”和优生学的想法。赫伯特·斯宾塞(Herbert Spencer)是这里的一位有影响力的人物,他在达尔文之前提出了“进化”一词,但与达尔文不同(Köchy, 2007),“适者生存”一词也是从他那里起源的。他在维多利亚时代的英国创立了“生物斯宾塞主义”(Freeman et al., 1974),社会达尔文主义(甚至在德国生物学家恩斯特·海克尔之前)和所谓的基于生物学的白人优越论。根据他的理论,不同的人群会有固有的和独特的特征,这些特征表现在他们的肤色上。这些特征将与携带者的种群血统有关。人类种群之间的差异进一步利用生物学概念,如形态物种,反映了一种静态的世界观,在这种世界观中,物种是由系统学家选择的某些特征来定义的。从本质上讲,人类群体被视为不可改变的“类”(Mahner &;邦基集团,1997年)。然而,这并不能反映客观的生物学,而是代表了人类思维的一种有缺陷的结构。分类学是一种相当静态的思维方式,不受进化观念的影响。中心问题过去是,现在仍然是,组间变异(组间变异)与组内个体变异(组内变异)的关系。然而,在20世纪60年代,随着分子生物学的出现及其在群体遗传学中的应用,我们对人类生物学历史的看法开始发生转变(Lewontin, 1972)。分子证据为观察物种形成过程和分类划分提供了新的标准,这些标准很容易应用于人类群体。在对血型、线粒体DNA和y染色体等简单生物标记进行早期研究之后(Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994),近年来,由于高通量DNA测序技术的出现,对人类遗传多样性的研究出现了爆炸式增长,破译了数百万人的基因组。这些基因组研究的重点是人类基因组中的多态位点,即DNA序列中的变异,也称为单核苷酸多态性(snp),在个体之间存在差异,并且根据中性进化的随机过程随时间而变化。这些snp大多不在基因中,因此对表型没有影响。最近分离的种群仍然有许多共同的snp,只是在许多代之后,这个比例会减少。与人类参考基因组相比,每个人携带400万至700万个snp和数百万个更大的结构变异(the 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2015)。这种遗传变异的数量级比经典分类学所依据的形态特征要多。通过基因组分析,可以详细地追溯种群的遗传历史和亲缘关系,同时也明确地反驳了早期和当前的“种族”概念。研究表明,任何特定人群内的遗传变异几乎与人群之间的变异一样高(the 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2015)。在基因组中甚至没有发现一个位置代表两个大陆种群的所有个体之间的固定差异。 这一事实也包括来自已灭绝人类谱系的基因变异,比如非非洲人身上的尼安德特人DNA,以及东南亚和大洋洲当代人身上的丹尼索瓦人DNA。而是不同人群之间基因变异的频率不同。从进化的角度来看,人类是一个相对年轻的物种;在过去的5万年里,人类在整个世界定居所造成的地理隔离的遗传影响很小,并且经常被二次混合事件所覆盖(Lazaridis等人,2016)。这种遗传变异的一小部分将产生表型后果。局部适应的一个例子是与皮肤色素多态性有关的基因。在低纬度地区,黑色素水平的增加导致肤色变深是为了防止紫外线辐射,而在高纬度地区,黑色素水平的降低导致肤色变浅有利于促进维生素D的合成,而维生素D的合成取决于紫外线照射(另见特刊专栏1的社论)。然而,后一种机制自20世纪30年代以来就已为人所知(Murray, 1934)。仅对过去人类基因组的分析就表明,冰河时代的欧洲狩猎采集者大多是深色皮肤,浅色皮肤色素沉着的高流行率仅在青铜时代的欧洲才达到(Mathieson et al., 2015)。我们目前的理解是,早期深色皮肤的农业社会必须通过发展浅色皮肤色素来适应低维生素D饮食和高纬度地区,并传播到中欧和北欧(克劳斯&Trappe, 2019)。因此,根据肤色对人类群体进行分类是没有意义的,因为这种特征反映的是纬度,而不是人口历史。此外,皮肤色素沉着远不是一个二元特征,有无数的色调随着靠近赤道而逐渐改变。变异的梯度是人类种群的规律,而不是离散的边界。随着样本量和种族多样性的增加,全基因组关联研究(GWAS),如最近关于烟草和酒精消费的研究(Saunders et al., 2022),表明祖先具有较弱的预测能力,强调表型和潜在的遗传变异存在于人类祖先的深处,而不仅仅存在于他们之间。最后但并非最不重要的是,这不仅是一个变异性感知的问题,而且是人脸识别能力的问题,这里是熟悉面孔和不熟悉面孔感知之间的定性差异(Jenkins等人,2018),这与本期特刊的主题密切相关。两位作者均声明没有利益冲突。
{"title":"Human populations are not biologically and genetically discrete","authors":"Martin S. Fischer, Johannes Krause","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12635","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12635","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Science relies on the validity of concepts, especially when these come from other disciplines. The differentiation of human groups according to phenotypic appearance, for example the colour of their skin, seemed so obvious that after centuries only genetics could fundamentally revise this concept. For 50 years, attempts have been made to break away from racial categorization, among others in the recent Jena Declaration of 2019 (Fischer et al., <span>2019</span>). There is no biological justification for categorizing people into discrete groups; on the contrary, racism seeks a biological legitimation in its justification.</p><p>This Special Issue takes up the enormous challenge of how to deal with the realization that ‘race’ in humans is not a scientifically tenable category but that ‘race’ permeates psychological processing (Editorial of this Special Issue) especially in fields of psychology, for which the term is constitutive, such as the ‘other-‘race’-effect’. Other sciences may be curious to see how psychology discusses the downgrading of ‘races’ to groups of increasingly higher resolution or, for example, the individual-differences approach, and finally how giving up a concept may open new doors and provide new opportunities.</p><p>People have been categorizing each other based on appearance for millennia, as is still shown by the ongoing controversy on ancient Egyptian ‘races’. In the 19th century, early evolutionary thinking began to remodel this practice of classifying human groups using phenotypes, culminating in ideas of superior human ‘races’ and eugenics. An influential figure here is Herbert Spencer, who conceived the term ‘evolution’ before and differently from Darwin (Köchy, <span>2007</span>), and from whom the expression ‘survival of the fittest’ originated. He founded ‘biological Spencerism’ in Victorian England (Freeman et al., <span>1974</span>), Social Darwinism (even before German biologist Ernst Haeckel) and the supposedly biologically based form of white superiority. According to his theory, different groups of people would have inherent and distinct characteristics, which are displayed, for example, in the colour of their skin. Those characteristics would be linked to the population ancestry of its bearer.</p><p>Differences between human populations were further imagined using biological concepts, such as morphospecies, reflecting a static worldview, in which species were defined by certain characteristics selected by systematists. In essence, human groups were seen as immutable ‘classes’ (Mahner & Bunge, <span>1997</span>). This, however, does not reflect objective biology but rather represents a flawed construct of the human mind. Taxonomy is a rather static way of thinking, uninfluenced by the idea of evolution.</p><p>The central question was, and still is, the relationship of between-group variation (intergroup variation) to individual variability within a group (intragroup variation). Our view of human biologic","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 S1","pages":"14-16"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12635","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9623035","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Francesco Foroni, Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos, Rand Wilcox, Fernanda de Bastiani, Gün R. Semin
Rapidly evaluating our environment's beneficial and detrimental features is critical for our successful functioning. A classic paradigm used to investigate such fast and automatic evaluations is the affective priming (AP) paradigm, where participants classify valenced target stimuli (e.g., words) as good or bad while ignoring the valenced primes (e.g., words). We investigate the differential impact that verbs and adjectives used as primes and targets have on the AP paradigm. Based on earlier work on the Linguistic Category Model, we expect AP effect to be modulated by non-evaluative properties of the word stimuli, such as the linguistic category (e.g., if the prime is an adjective and the target is a verb versus the reverse). A reduction in the magnitude of the priming effect was predicted for adjective–verb prime-target pairs compared to verb–adjective prime-target pairs. Moreover, we implemented a modified crowdsourcing of statistical analyses implementing independently three different statistical approaches. Deriving our conclusions on the converging/diverging evidence provided by the different approaches, we show a clear deductive/inductive asymmetry in AP paradigm (exp. 1), that this asymmetry does not require a focus on the evaluative dimension to emerge (exp. 2) and that the semantic-based asymmetry weakly extends to valence (exp. 3).
{"title":"A multi-analyses approach of inductive/deductive asymmetry in the affective priming paradigm","authors":"Francesco Foroni, Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos, Rand Wilcox, Fernanda de Bastiani, Gün R. Semin","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12634","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12634","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Rapidly evaluating our environment's beneficial and detrimental features is critical for our successful functioning. A classic paradigm used to investigate such fast and automatic evaluations is the affective priming (AP) paradigm, where participants classify valenced <i>target</i> stimuli (e.g., words) as good or bad while ignoring the valenced <i>primes</i> (e.g., words). We investigate the differential impact that verbs and adjectives used as primes and targets have on the AP paradigm. Based on earlier work on the Linguistic Category Model, we expect AP effect to be modulated by non-evaluative properties of the <i>word stimuli</i>, such as the linguistic category (e.g., if the prime is an adjective and the target is a verb versus the reverse). A reduction in the magnitude of the priming effect was predicted for adjective–verb prime-target pairs compared to verb–adjective prime-target pairs. Moreover, we implemented a modified crowdsourcing of statistical analyses implementing independently three different statistical approaches. Deriving our conclusions on the converging/diverging evidence provided by the different approaches, we show a clear deductive/inductive asymmetry in AP paradigm (exp. 1), that this asymmetry does not require a focus on the evaluative dimension to emerge (exp. 2) and that the semantic-based asymmetry weakly extends to valence (exp. 3).</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"550-565"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12634","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10163565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In recent decades, the economies of many countries have produced increasingly unequal outcomes for the rich and poor. This economic trend has attracted interest from members of the media, public and political classes as well as researchers who are interested in its societal implications. While this research has traditionally been the purview of economists and sociologists, there has been a burgeoning growth in research that has sought to understand the psychology of economic inequality. In this review, we summarize this work, focusing on two major themes: (1) how people perceive the scale of economic inequality and appraise its significance, and (2) how living in an economically unequal environment shapes people's social lives. Together, this work affirms claims that economic inequality is ‘the defining issue of our time’ (Obama, 2013) with a great deal of destructive potential. We identify important questions that await further research attention.
{"title":"How living in economically unequal societies shapes our minds and our social lives","authors":"Kim Peters, Jolanda Jetten","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12632","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12632","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In recent decades, the economies of many countries have produced increasingly unequal outcomes for the rich and poor. This economic trend has attracted interest from members of the media, public and political classes as well as researchers who are interested in its societal implications. While this research has traditionally been the purview of economists and sociologists, there has been a burgeoning growth in research that has sought to understand the psychology of economic inequality. In this review, we summarize this work, focusing on two major themes: (1) how people perceive the scale of economic inequality and appraise its significance, and (2) how living in an economically unequal environment shapes people's social lives. Together, this work affirms claims that economic inequality is ‘the defining issue of our time’ (Obama, 2013) with a great deal of destructive potential. We identify important questions that await further research attention.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"515-531"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12632","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9621915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Integrative processing is traditionally believed to be dependent on consciousness. While earlier studies within the last decade reported many types of integration under subliminal conditions (i.e. without perceptual awareness), these findings are widely challenged recently. This review evaluates the current evidence for 10 types of subliminal integration that are widely studied: arithmetic processing, object-context integration, multi-word processing, same-different processing, multisensory integration and 5 different types of associative learning. Potential methodological issues concerning awareness measures are also taken into account. It is concluded that while there is currently no reliable evidence for subliminal integration, this does not necessarily refute ‘unconscious’ integration defined through non-subliminal (e.g. implicit) approaches.
{"title":"Unconscious integration: Current evidence for integrative processing under subliminal conditions","authors":"Zher-Wen, Rongjun Yu","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12631","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12631","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Integrative processing is traditionally believed to be dependent on consciousness. While earlier studies within the last decade reported many types of integration under subliminal conditions (i.e. without perceptual awareness), these findings are widely challenged recently. This review evaluates the current evidence for 10 types of subliminal integration that are widely studied: arithmetic processing, object-context integration, multi-word processing, same-different processing, multisensory integration and 5 different types of associative learning. Potential methodological issues concerning awareness measures are also taken into account. It is concluded that while there is currently no reliable evidence for subliminal integration, this does not necessarily refute ‘unconscious’ integration defined through non-subliminal (e.g. implicit) approaches.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"430-456"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12631","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9320955","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We rarely become familiar with the voice of another person in isolation but usually also have access to visual identity information, thus learning to recognize their voice and face in parallel. There are conflicting findings as to whether learning to recognize voices in audiovisual vs audio-only settings is advantageous or detrimental to learning. One prominent finding shows that the presence of a face overshadows the voice, hindering voice identity learning by capturing listeners' attention (Face Overshadowing Effect; FOE). In the current study, we tested the proposal that the effect of audiovisual training on voice identity learning is driven by attentional processes. Participants learned to recognize voices through either audio-only training (Audio-Only) or through three versions of audiovisual training, where a face was presented alongside the voices. During audiovisual training, the faces were either looking at the camera (Direct Gaze), were looking to the side (Averted Gaze) or had closed eyes (No Gaze). We found a graded effect of gaze on voice identity learning: Voice identity recognition was most accurate after audio-only training and least accurate after audiovisual training including direct gaze, constituting a FOE. While effect sizes were overall small, the magnitude of FOE was halved for the Averted and No Gaze conditions. With direct gaze being associated with increased attention capture compared to averted or no gaze, the current findings suggest that incidental attention capture at least partially underpins the FOE. We discuss these findings in light of visual dominance effects and the relative informativeness of faces vs voices for identity perception.
{"title":"The effects of the presence of a face and direct eye gaze on voice identity learning","authors":"Nadine Lavan, Nisha Ramanik Bamaniya, Moha-Maryam Muse, Raffaella Lucy Monica Price, Isabelle Mareschal","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12633","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12633","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We rarely become familiar with the voice of another person in isolation but usually also have access to visual identity information, thus learning to recognize their voice and face in parallel. There are conflicting findings as to whether learning to recognize voices in audiovisual vs audio-only settings is advantageous or detrimental to learning. One prominent finding shows that the presence of a face overshadows the voice, hindering voice identity learning by capturing listeners' attention (Face Overshadowing Effect; FOE). In the current study, we tested the proposal that the effect of audiovisual training on voice identity learning is driven by attentional processes. Participants learned to recognize voices through either audio-only training (Audio-Only) or through three versions of audiovisual training, where a face was presented alongside the voices. During audiovisual training, the faces were either looking at the camera (Direct Gaze), were looking to the side (Averted Gaze) or had closed eyes (No Gaze). We found a graded effect of gaze on voice identity learning: Voice identity recognition was most accurate after audio-only training and least accurate after audiovisual training including direct gaze, constituting a FOE. While effect sizes were overall small, the magnitude of FOE was halved for the Averted and No Gaze conditions. With direct gaze being associated with increased attention capture compared to averted or no gaze, the current findings suggest that incidental attention capture at least partially underpins the FOE. We discuss these findings in light of visual dominance effects and the relative informativeness of faces vs voices for identity perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"537-549"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12633","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9784344","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}