首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Empathy and parenthood: The moderating role of maternal trait empathy on parental burnout 共情与亲子关系:母亲特质共情对父母倦怠的调节作用
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-27 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12640
Tamar Kadosh-Laor, Liat Israeli-Ran, Ido Shalev, Florina Uzefovsky

The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; N = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, N = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.

同情他人的能力使我们能够有效地与他人互动,并可能特别进化到支持父母的角色和照顾。由于以往的研究只关注父母教养背景下的共情,例如父母的敏感性,因此人们对父母教养与共情特质之间的关系知之甚少。本研究旨在了解共情特质如何调节儿童负性情绪与父母倦怠之间的关系。研究了两个队列:(1)婴儿的父母(10-18个月大;N = 203)和(2)3-10岁儿童的父母,N = 201)。父母们填写了一系列在线问卷,评估母亲的同理心、父母的倦怠和孩子的性情。研究发现,高负性情绪水平与父母职业倦怠之间的关系受到母亲情感共情的特定方面的调节。我们的研究结果表明,当面对孩子的特征导致父母的更高要求时,母亲的情感共情可以缓冲父母的倦怠。
{"title":"Empathy and parenthood: The moderating role of maternal trait empathy on parental burnout","authors":"Tamar Kadosh-Laor,&nbsp;Liat Israeli-Ran,&nbsp;Ido Shalev,&nbsp;Florina Uzefovsky","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12640","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12640","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; <i>N</i> = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, <i>N</i> = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"605-620"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12640","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9778816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study 比较音乐和食物在年轻人和老年人中唤起的自传式记忆:一项日记研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-13 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12639
Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, Lia Kvavilashvili, Abbigail Ely, Mark Gill, Gemma Herbert

Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.

先前的研究发现,音乐比其他各种检索线索更能唤起生动、情绪化的自传式记忆。然而,这类研究的生态效度往往较低,并且受到相对有限的线索选择和主要是年轻人样本的限制。在这里,我们比较了音乐和食物作为年轻人和老年人日常生活中自传式记忆的线索。39名年龄在18-34岁之间的年轻人和39名年龄在60-77岁之间的老年人在两个独立的为期四天的时间段内,在纸日记中记录了他们的音乐和食物引发的自传式记忆。在这两个年龄组中,音乐触发的自传式记忆更频繁,非自愿记忆的比例更大,与食物线索相比,记忆对个人更重要。年龄差异对音乐和食物唤起的记忆的影响相似,老年人总是能回忆起更早、更不具体的记忆,他们认为这些记忆更积极、更生动、更有预演。然而,年轻人和老年人在记录记忆的数量或非自愿性质上没有区别。这项工作代表了理解成年期自然发生的音乐诱发自传式记忆的现象学的重要一步,并为音乐如何以及为什么可能比某些其他日常线索更有效地触发个人价值记忆提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study","authors":"Kelly Jakubowski,&nbsp;Amy M. Belfi,&nbsp;Lia Kvavilashvili,&nbsp;Abbigail Ely,&nbsp;Mark Gill,&nbsp;Gemma Herbert","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12639","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12639","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"580-604"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12639","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9854772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Food word processing in Chinese reading: A study of restrained eaters 汉语阅读中的食物词加工:一项限制进食者的研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-10 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12638
Changlin Luo, Mengyan Zhu, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma

Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.

与食物相关的注意偏差是指与非食物刺激相比,个体通常优先考虑与食物相关的奖励线索(如食物单词和食物图像);然而,对于节制饮食的人来说,研究结果并不一致。传统的食物注意偏差测试范式,如视觉探测任务和视觉搜索任务,可能不足以直接和准确地反映个体在不同认知阶段的食物词加工。在这项研究中,我们引入了边界范式来研究限制和不限制进食者的食物词注意偏差。当他们进行自然的句子阅读任务时,他们的眼球运动被记录下来。后期分析的结果表明,食物词的注视时间比非食物词的注视时间要短,这表明无论进食限制还是不进食限制,中央凹对食物词的处理都具有优势。前期分析结果表明,在食物词有效预览条件下,克制型进食者在目标前区域花费的时间更多,这表明克制型进食者在食物词加工上具有优势(即中央凹对中央凹效应)。中央凹食物字加工的优越性为解释一般群体中与食物相关的注意偏倚提供了新的见解。此外,限制进食者在旁中央凹加工中增强的食物词注意偏倚说明了个体特征在研究单词识别中的重要性。
{"title":"Food word processing in Chinese reading: A study of restrained eaters","authors":"Changlin Luo,&nbsp;Mengyan Zhu,&nbsp;Xiangling Zhuang,&nbsp;Guojie Ma","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12638","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12638","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"476-494"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9273486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Electrophysiological correlates of dispositional mindfulness: A quantitative and complexity EEG study 性情性正念的电生理相关:一项定量和复杂的脑电图研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-07 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12636
Nuria Victoria Aguerre, Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza, Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina, María Teresa Bajo

While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.

虽然越来越多的证据表明,性格正念与心理健康和认知增强有关,但迄今为止,只有少数人试图描述其神经基础。在本研究中,我们旨在通过静息状态和执行学习任务时的EEG定量和复杂性测量来探索气质正念的电生理特征。采用五方面正念问卷对120名参与者进行了5分钟闭眼静息状态和5分钟任务脑电图记录。我们假设,与低正念个体相比,高正念个体的大脑活动模式与(a)休息时较低的默认模式网络(DMN)参与(额叶伽马功率降低)和(b)从休息到任务的“任务准备”状态反映出更相似的模式(休息时总体q-EEG功率降低,但任务时没有)相关。性格正念与休息时额叶伽玛能量降低和休息时总能量降低显著相关,但与任务无关。此外,我们发现正念个体在任务执行过程中有更高熵的趋势,这一趋势最近在正念冥想中得到了报道。总之,我们的研究结果与冥想专家的研究结果相结合,表明高(倾向)正念似乎具有一种特定的电生理模式,即较少参与DMN和走神过程。
{"title":"Electrophysiological correlates of dispositional mindfulness: A quantitative and complexity EEG study","authors":"Nuria Victoria Aguerre,&nbsp;Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza,&nbsp;Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina,&nbsp;María Teresa Bajo","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12636","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12636","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"566-579"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12636","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9790273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The structure of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: Theoretical and methodological considerations 医院焦虑抑郁量表的结构:理论与方法的考量
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-06 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12637
Maddison Lloyd, Nicole Sugden, Matt Thomas, Andrew McGrath, Clive Skilbeck

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond - Snaith, 1983) is widely used; however, its factor structure is unclear, with studies reporting differing unidimensional, two-factor and three-factor models. We aimed to address some key theoretical and methodological issues contributing to inconsistencies in HADS structures across samples. We reviewed existing HADS models and compared their fit using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We also investigated methodological effects by comparing factor structures derived from Rasch and Principal Components Analysis (PCA) methods, as well as effects of a negative wording factor. An Australian community-dwelling sample consisting of 189 females and 158 males aged 17–86 (M = 35.73, SD = 17.41) completed the 14-item HADS. The Rasch Analysis, PCA and CFA all supported the original two-factor structure. Although some three-factor models had good fit, they had unacceptable reliability. In the CFA, a hierarchical bifactor model with a general distress factor and uncorrelated depression and anxiety subscales produced the best fit, but the general factor was not unidimensional. The addition of a negative wording factor improved model fit. These findings highlight the effects of differing methodologies in producing inconsistent HADS factor structures across studies. Further replication of model fit across samples and refinement of the HADS items is warranted.

医院焦虑抑郁量表(HADS);Zigmond - Snaith, 1983)被广泛使用;然而,其因素结构尚不清楚,研究报告了不同的一维,两因素和三因素模型。我们的目的是解决一些关键的理论和方法问题,导致HADS结构在样本中不一致。我们回顾了现有的HADS模型,并使用验证性因子分析(CFA)比较了它们的拟合性。我们还通过比较来自Rasch和主成分分析(PCA)方法的因子结构以及负面措辞因子的影响来研究方法效应。调查对象为澳大利亚社区居民,共有189名女性和158名男性,年龄在17-86岁之间(M = 35.73, SD = 17.41)。Rasch分析、PCA和CFA均支持原始的双因子结构。虽然一些三因素模型具有良好的拟合性,但它们的可靠性令人无法接受。在CFA中,具有一般痛苦因素和不相关的抑郁和焦虑子量表的分层双因素模型产生了最佳拟合,但一般因素不是一维的。添加负的措辞因子改善了模型拟合。这些发现强调了不同的研究方法在不同研究中产生不一致的HADS因子结构的影响。进一步复制模型拟合的样本和改进HADS项目是必要的。
{"title":"The structure of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: Theoretical and methodological considerations","authors":"Maddison Lloyd,&nbsp;Nicole Sugden,&nbsp;Matt Thomas,&nbsp;Andrew McGrath,&nbsp;Clive Skilbeck","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12637","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12637","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond - Snaith, 1983) is widely used; however, its factor structure is unclear, with studies reporting differing unidimensional, two-factor and three-factor models. We aimed to address some key theoretical and methodological issues contributing to inconsistencies in HADS structures across samples. We reviewed existing HADS models and compared their fit using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We also investigated methodological effects by comparing factor structures derived from Rasch and Principal Components Analysis (PCA) methods, as well as effects of a negative wording factor. An Australian community-dwelling sample consisting of 189 females and 158 males aged 17–86 (<i>M</i> = 35.73, <i>SD</i> = 17.41) completed the 14-item HADS. The Rasch Analysis, PCA and CFA all supported the original two-factor structure. Although some three-factor models had good fit, they had unacceptable reliability. In the CFA, a hierarchical bifactor model with a general distress factor and uncorrelated depression and anxiety subscales produced the best fit, but the general factor was not unidimensional. The addition of a negative wording factor improved model fit. These findings highlight the effects of differing methodologies in producing inconsistent HADS factor structures across studies. Further replication of model fit across samples and refinement of the HADS items is warranted.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"457-475"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12637","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9636389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Human populations are not biologically and genetically discrete 人类种群在生物学和基因上并不是离散的
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12635
Martin S. Fischer, Johannes Krause
<p>Science relies on the validity of concepts, especially when these come from other disciplines. The differentiation of human groups according to phenotypic appearance, for example the colour of their skin, seemed so obvious that after centuries only genetics could fundamentally revise this concept. For 50 years, attempts have been made to break away from racial categorization, among others in the recent Jena Declaration of 2019 (Fischer et al., <span>2019</span>). There is no biological justification for categorizing people into discrete groups; on the contrary, racism seeks a biological legitimation in its justification.</p><p>This Special Issue takes up the enormous challenge of how to deal with the realization that ‘race’ in humans is not a scientifically tenable category but that ‘race’ permeates psychological processing (Editorial of this Special Issue) especially in fields of psychology, for which the term is constitutive, such as the ‘other-‘race’-effect’. Other sciences may be curious to see how psychology discusses the downgrading of ‘races’ to groups of increasingly higher resolution or, for example, the individual-differences approach, and finally how giving up a concept may open new doors and provide new opportunities.</p><p>People have been categorizing each other based on appearance for millennia, as is still shown by the ongoing controversy on ancient Egyptian ‘races’. In the 19th century, early evolutionary thinking began to remodel this practice of classifying human groups using phenotypes, culminating in ideas of superior human ‘races’ and eugenics. An influential figure here is Herbert Spencer, who conceived the term ‘evolution’ before and differently from Darwin (Köchy, <span>2007</span>), and from whom the expression ‘survival of the fittest’ originated. He founded ‘biological Spencerism’ in Victorian England (Freeman et al., <span>1974</span>), Social Darwinism (even before German biologist Ernst Haeckel) and the supposedly biologically based form of white superiority. According to his theory, different groups of people would have inherent and distinct characteristics, which are displayed, for example, in the colour of their skin. Those characteristics would be linked to the population ancestry of its bearer.</p><p>Differences between human populations were further imagined using biological concepts, such as morphospecies, reflecting a static worldview, in which species were defined by certain characteristics selected by systematists. In essence, human groups were seen as immutable ‘classes’ (Mahner & Bunge, <span>1997</span>). This, however, does not reflect objective biology but rather represents a flawed construct of the human mind. Taxonomy is a rather static way of thinking, uninfluenced by the idea of evolution.</p><p>The central question was, and still is, the relationship of between-group variation (intergroup variation) to individual variability within a group (intragroup variation). Our view of human biologic
科学依赖于概念的有效性,尤其是当这些概念来自其他学科时。人类群体根据表型(例如皮肤颜色)的差异,似乎是如此明显,以至于几个世纪后,只有遗传学才能从根本上改变这一概念。50年来,人们一直试图摆脱种族分类,其中包括最近的《2019年耶拿宣言》(Fischer et al., 2019)。没有生物学上的理由把人分成不同的群体;相反,种族主义在其辩护中寻求生物学上的正当性。本期特刊提出了一个巨大的挑战,即如何认识到人类的“种族”并不是一个科学上站住脚的范畴,而是“种族”渗透到心理过程中(本期特刊社论),特别是在心理学领域,这个术语是构成要素,比如“他者-“种族”-效应。其他科学可能会好奇地看到心理学如何讨论将“种族”降级为分辨率越来越高的群体,或者,例如,个体差异方法,以及最终放弃一个概念如何打开新的大门并提供新的机会。几千年来,人们一直根据外貌对彼此进行分类,关于古埃及“种族”的争论至今仍在继续。在19世纪,早期的进化思想开始重塑这种使用表型对人类群体进行分类的做法,最终产生了优越人类“种族”和优生学的想法。赫伯特·斯宾塞(Herbert Spencer)是这里的一位有影响力的人物,他在达尔文之前提出了“进化”一词,但与达尔文不同(Köchy, 2007),“适者生存”一词也是从他那里起源的。他在维多利亚时代的英国创立了“生物斯宾塞主义”(Freeman et al., 1974),社会达尔文主义(甚至在德国生物学家恩斯特·海克尔之前)和所谓的基于生物学的白人优越论。根据他的理论,不同的人群会有固有的和独特的特征,这些特征表现在他们的肤色上。这些特征将与携带者的种群血统有关。人类种群之间的差异进一步利用生物学概念,如形态物种,反映了一种静态的世界观,在这种世界观中,物种是由系统学家选择的某些特征来定义的。从本质上讲,人类群体被视为不可改变的“类”(Mahner &;邦基集团,1997年)。然而,这并不能反映客观的生物学,而是代表了人类思维的一种有缺陷的结构。分类学是一种相当静态的思维方式,不受进化观念的影响。中心问题过去是,现在仍然是,组间变异(组间变异)与组内个体变异(组内变异)的关系。然而,在20世纪60年代,随着分子生物学的出现及其在群体遗传学中的应用,我们对人类生物学历史的看法开始发生转变(Lewontin, 1972)。分子证据为观察物种形成过程和分类划分提供了新的标准,这些标准很容易应用于人类群体。在对血型、线粒体DNA和y染色体等简单生物标记进行早期研究之后(Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994),近年来,由于高通量DNA测序技术的出现,对人类遗传多样性的研究出现了爆炸式增长,破译了数百万人的基因组。这些基因组研究的重点是人类基因组中的多态位点,即DNA序列中的变异,也称为单核苷酸多态性(snp),在个体之间存在差异,并且根据中性进化的随机过程随时间而变化。这些snp大多不在基因中,因此对表型没有影响。最近分离的种群仍然有许多共同的snp,只是在许多代之后,这个比例会减少。与人类参考基因组相比,每个人携带400万至700万个snp和数百万个更大的结构变异(the 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2015)。这种遗传变异的数量级比经典分类学所依据的形态特征要多。通过基因组分析,可以详细地追溯种群的遗传历史和亲缘关系,同时也明确地反驳了早期和当前的“种族”概念。研究表明,任何特定人群内的遗传变异几乎与人群之间的变异一样高(the 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2015)。在基因组中甚至没有发现一个位置代表两个大陆种群的所有个体之间的固定差异。 这一事实也包括来自已灭绝人类谱系的基因变异,比如非非洲人身上的尼安德特人DNA,以及东南亚和大洋洲当代人身上的丹尼索瓦人DNA。而是不同人群之间基因变异的频率不同。从进化的角度来看,人类是一个相对年轻的物种;在过去的5万年里,人类在整个世界定居所造成的地理隔离的遗传影响很小,并且经常被二次混合事件所覆盖(Lazaridis等人,2016)。这种遗传变异的一小部分将产生表型后果。局部适应的一个例子是与皮肤色素多态性有关的基因。在低纬度地区,黑色素水平的增加导致肤色变深是为了防止紫外线辐射,而在高纬度地区,黑色素水平的降低导致肤色变浅有利于促进维生素D的合成,而维生素D的合成取决于紫外线照射(另见特刊专栏1的社论)。然而,后一种机制自20世纪30年代以来就已为人所知(Murray, 1934)。仅对过去人类基因组的分析就表明,冰河时代的欧洲狩猎采集者大多是深色皮肤,浅色皮肤色素沉着的高流行率仅在青铜时代的欧洲才达到(Mathieson et al., 2015)。我们目前的理解是,早期深色皮肤的农业社会必须通过发展浅色皮肤色素来适应低维生素D饮食和高纬度地区,并传播到中欧和北欧(克劳斯&Trappe, 2019)。因此,根据肤色对人类群体进行分类是没有意义的,因为这种特征反映的是纬度,而不是人口历史。此外,皮肤色素沉着远不是一个二元特征,有无数的色调随着靠近赤道而逐渐改变。变异的梯度是人类种群的规律,而不是离散的边界。随着样本量和种族多样性的增加,全基因组关联研究(GWAS),如最近关于烟草和酒精消费的研究(Saunders et al., 2022),表明祖先具有较弱的预测能力,强调表型和潜在的遗传变异存在于人类祖先的深处,而不仅仅存在于他们之间。最后但并非最不重要的是,这不仅是一个变异性感知的问题,而且是人脸识别能力的问题,这里是熟悉面孔和不熟悉面孔感知之间的定性差异(Jenkins等人,2018),这与本期特刊的主题密切相关。两位作者均声明没有利益冲突。
{"title":"Human populations are not biologically and genetically discrete","authors":"Martin S. Fischer,&nbsp;Johannes Krause","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12635","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12635","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;Science relies on the validity of concepts, especially when these come from other disciplines. The differentiation of human groups according to phenotypic appearance, for example the colour of their skin, seemed so obvious that after centuries only genetics could fundamentally revise this concept. For 50 years, attempts have been made to break away from racial categorization, among others in the recent Jena Declaration of 2019 (Fischer et al., &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;). There is no biological justification for categorizing people into discrete groups; on the contrary, racism seeks a biological legitimation in its justification.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;This Special Issue takes up the enormous challenge of how to deal with the realization that ‘race’ in humans is not a scientifically tenable category but that ‘race’ permeates psychological processing (Editorial of this Special Issue) especially in fields of psychology, for which the term is constitutive, such as the ‘other-‘race’-effect’. Other sciences may be curious to see how psychology discusses the downgrading of ‘races’ to groups of increasingly higher resolution or, for example, the individual-differences approach, and finally how giving up a concept may open new doors and provide new opportunities.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;People have been categorizing each other based on appearance for millennia, as is still shown by the ongoing controversy on ancient Egyptian ‘races’. In the 19th century, early evolutionary thinking began to remodel this practice of classifying human groups using phenotypes, culminating in ideas of superior human ‘races’ and eugenics. An influential figure here is Herbert Spencer, who conceived the term ‘evolution’ before and differently from Darwin (Köchy, &lt;span&gt;2007&lt;/span&gt;), and from whom the expression ‘survival of the fittest’ originated. He founded ‘biological Spencerism’ in Victorian England (Freeman et al., &lt;span&gt;1974&lt;/span&gt;), Social Darwinism (even before German biologist Ernst Haeckel) and the supposedly biologically based form of white superiority. According to his theory, different groups of people would have inherent and distinct characteristics, which are displayed, for example, in the colour of their skin. Those characteristics would be linked to the population ancestry of its bearer.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Differences between human populations were further imagined using biological concepts, such as morphospecies, reflecting a static worldview, in which species were defined by certain characteristics selected by systematists. In essence, human groups were seen as immutable ‘classes’ (Mahner &amp; Bunge, &lt;span&gt;1997&lt;/span&gt;). This, however, does not reflect objective biology but rather represents a flawed construct of the human mind. Taxonomy is a rather static way of thinking, uninfluenced by the idea of evolution.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;The central question was, and still is, the relationship of between-group variation (intergroup variation) to individual variability within a group (intragroup variation). Our view of human biologic","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 S1","pages":"14-16"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12635","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9623035","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A multi-analyses approach of inductive/deductive asymmetry in the affective priming paradigm 情感启动范式中归纳/演绎不对称的多重分析方法
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-30 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12634
Francesco Foroni, Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos, Rand Wilcox, Fernanda de Bastiani, Gün R. Semin

Rapidly evaluating our environment's beneficial and detrimental features is critical for our successful functioning. A classic paradigm used to investigate such fast and automatic evaluations is the affective priming (AP) paradigm, where participants classify valenced target stimuli (e.g., words) as good or bad while ignoring the valenced primes (e.g., words). We investigate the differential impact that verbs and adjectives used as primes and targets have on the AP paradigm. Based on earlier work on the Linguistic Category Model, we expect AP effect to be modulated by non-evaluative properties of the word stimuli, such as the linguistic category (e.g., if the prime is an adjective and the target is a verb versus the reverse). A reduction in the magnitude of the priming effect was predicted for adjective–verb prime-target pairs compared to verb–adjective prime-target pairs. Moreover, we implemented a modified crowdsourcing of statistical analyses implementing independently three different statistical approaches. Deriving our conclusions on the converging/diverging evidence provided by the different approaches, we show a clear deductive/inductive asymmetry in AP paradigm (exp. 1), that this asymmetry does not require a focus on the evaluative dimension to emerge (exp. 2) and that the semantic-based asymmetry weakly extends to valence (exp. 3).

快速评估环境的有益和有害特征对我们的成功运作至关重要。用于研究这种快速和自动评估的经典范式是情感启动(AP)范式,其中参与者将有价目标刺激(例如,单词)分类为好或坏,而忽略有价启动(例如,单词)。我们研究了动词和形容词作为启动物和目标物对AP范式的不同影响。基于先前对语言范畴模型的研究,我们预计AP效应会受到单词刺激的非评价性质的调节,例如语言范畴(例如,如果启动是形容词,目标是动词,反之亦然)。与动词-形容词启动-目标配对相比,预测形容词-动词启动-目标配对的启动效应强度会降低。此外,我们实施了一种改进的统计分析众包,独立实施了三种不同的统计方法。根据不同方法提供的收敛/发散证据得出我们的结论,我们显示了AP范式中明确的演绎/归纳不对称(经验1),这种不对称不需要关注评价维度(经验2),并且基于语义的不对称弱扩展到价(经验3)。
{"title":"A multi-analyses approach of inductive/deductive asymmetry in the affective priming paradigm","authors":"Francesco Foroni,&nbsp;Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos,&nbsp;Rand Wilcox,&nbsp;Fernanda de Bastiani,&nbsp;Gün R. Semin","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12634","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12634","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Rapidly evaluating our environment's beneficial and detrimental features is critical for our successful functioning. A classic paradigm used to investigate such fast and automatic evaluations is the affective priming (AP) paradigm, where participants classify valenced <i>target</i> stimuli (e.g., words) as good or bad while ignoring the valenced <i>primes</i> (e.g., words). We investigate the differential impact that verbs and adjectives used as primes and targets have on the AP paradigm. Based on earlier work on the Linguistic Category Model, we expect AP effect to be modulated by non-evaluative properties of the <i>word stimuli</i>, such as the linguistic category (e.g., if the prime is an adjective and the target is a verb versus the reverse). A reduction in the magnitude of the priming effect was predicted for adjective–verb prime-target pairs compared to verb–adjective prime-target pairs. Moreover, we implemented a modified crowdsourcing of statistical analyses implementing independently three different statistical approaches. Deriving our conclusions on the converging/diverging evidence provided by the different approaches, we show a clear deductive/inductive asymmetry in AP paradigm (exp. 1), that this asymmetry does not require a focus on the evaluative dimension to emerge (exp. 2) and that the semantic-based asymmetry weakly extends to valence (exp. 3).</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"550-565"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12634","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10163565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How living in economically unequal societies shapes our minds and our social lives 生活在经济不平等的社会中如何塑造我们的思想和社会生活
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-27 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12632
Kim Peters, Jolanda Jetten

In recent decades, the economies of many countries have produced increasingly unequal outcomes for the rich and poor. This economic trend has attracted interest from members of the media, public and political classes as well as researchers who are interested in its societal implications. While this research has traditionally been the purview of economists and sociologists, there has been a burgeoning growth in research that has sought to understand the psychology of economic inequality. In this review, we summarize this work, focusing on two major themes: (1) how people perceive the scale of economic inequality and appraise its significance, and (2) how living in an economically unequal environment shapes people's social lives. Together, this work affirms claims that economic inequality is ‘the defining issue of our time’ (Obama, 2013) with a great deal of destructive potential. We identify important questions that await further research attention.

近几十年来,许多国家的经济对富人和穷人造成了越来越不平等的结果。这一经济趋势引起了媒体、公众和政治阶层成员以及对其社会影响感兴趣的研究人员的兴趣。虽然这一研究传统上是经济学家和社会学家的研究范围,但试图理解经济不平等心理的研究正在迅速增长。在这篇综述中,我们总结了这些工作,重点关注两个主要主题:(1)人们如何感知经济不平等的规模并评估其重要性;(2)生活在经济不平等的环境中如何塑造人们的社会生活。总之,这项工作证实了经济不平等是“我们这个时代的决定性问题”(奥巴马,2013),具有巨大的破坏性潜力。我们确定了等待进一步研究关注的重要问题。
{"title":"How living in economically unequal societies shapes our minds and our social lives","authors":"Kim Peters,&nbsp;Jolanda Jetten","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12632","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12632","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In recent decades, the economies of many countries have produced increasingly unequal outcomes for the rich and poor. This economic trend has attracted interest from members of the media, public and political classes as well as researchers who are interested in its societal implications. While this research has traditionally been the purview of economists and sociologists, there has been a burgeoning growth in research that has sought to understand the psychology of economic inequality. In this review, we summarize this work, focusing on two major themes: (1) how people perceive the scale of economic inequality and appraise its significance, and (2) how living in an economically unequal environment shapes people's social lives. Together, this work affirms claims that economic inequality is ‘the defining issue of our time’ (Obama, 2013) with a great deal of destructive potential. We identify important questions that await further research attention.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"515-531"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12632","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9621915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Unconscious integration: Current evidence for integrative processing under subliminal conditions 无意识整合:潜意识条件下整合加工的最新证据
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12631
Zher-Wen, Rongjun Yu

Integrative processing is traditionally believed to be dependent on consciousness. While earlier studies within the last decade reported many types of integration under subliminal conditions (i.e. without perceptual awareness), these findings are widely challenged recently. This review evaluates the current evidence for 10 types of subliminal integration that are widely studied: arithmetic processing, object-context integration, multi-word processing, same-different processing, multisensory integration and 5 different types of associative learning. Potential methodological issues concerning awareness measures are also taken into account. It is concluded that while there is currently no reliable evidence for subliminal integration, this does not necessarily refute ‘unconscious’ integration defined through non-subliminal (e.g. implicit) approaches.

传统上认为综合处理依赖于意识。虽然过去十年中的早期研究报告了在潜意识条件下(即没有感知意识)的许多类型的整合,但这些发现最近受到了广泛的挑战。这篇综述评估了目前被广泛研究的10种潜意识整合的证据:算术处理、对象-上下文整合、多词处理、相同-不同处理、多感觉整合和5种不同类型的联想学习。还考虑到有关提高认识措施的潜在方法问题。结论是,虽然目前没有可靠的证据表明潜意识整合,但这并不一定反驳通过非潜意识(如内隐)方法定义的“无意识”整合。
{"title":"Unconscious integration: Current evidence for integrative processing under subliminal conditions","authors":"Zher-Wen,&nbsp;Rongjun Yu","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12631","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12631","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Integrative processing is traditionally believed to be dependent on consciousness. While earlier studies within the last decade reported many types of integration under subliminal conditions (i.e. without perceptual awareness), these findings are widely challenged recently. This review evaluates the current evidence for 10 types of subliminal integration that are widely studied: arithmetic processing, object-context integration, multi-word processing, same-different processing, multisensory integration and 5 different types of associative learning. Potential methodological issues concerning awareness measures are also taken into account. It is concluded that while there is currently no reliable evidence for subliminal integration, this does not necessarily refute ‘unconscious’ integration defined through non-subliminal (e.g. implicit) approaches.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 2","pages":"430-456"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12631","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9320955","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The effects of the presence of a face and direct eye gaze on voice identity learning 面孔的存在和直视对语音识别学习的影响
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12633
Nadine Lavan, Nisha Ramanik Bamaniya, Moha-Maryam Muse, Raffaella Lucy Monica Price, Isabelle Mareschal

We rarely become familiar with the voice of another person in isolation but usually also have access to visual identity information, thus learning to recognize their voice and face in parallel. There are conflicting findings as to whether learning to recognize voices in audiovisual vs audio-only settings is advantageous or detrimental to learning. One prominent finding shows that the presence of a face overshadows the voice, hindering voice identity learning by capturing listeners' attention (Face Overshadowing Effect; FOE). In the current study, we tested the proposal that the effect of audiovisual training on voice identity learning is driven by attentional processes. Participants learned to recognize voices through either audio-only training (Audio-Only) or through three versions of audiovisual training, where a face was presented alongside the voices. During audiovisual training, the faces were either looking at the camera (Direct Gaze), were looking to the side (Averted Gaze) or had closed eyes (No Gaze). We found a graded effect of gaze on voice identity learning: Voice identity recognition was most accurate after audio-only training and least accurate after audiovisual training including direct gaze, constituting a FOE. While effect sizes were overall small, the magnitude of FOE was halved for the Averted and No Gaze conditions. With direct gaze being associated with increased attention capture compared to averted or no gaze, the current findings suggest that incidental attention capture at least partially underpins the FOE. We discuss these findings in light of visual dominance effects and the relative informativeness of faces vs voices for identity perception.

我们很少孤立地熟悉另一个人的声音,但通常也有机会获得视觉识别信息,从而学会同时识别他们的声音和面孔。关于学习在视听和纯音频环境中识别声音对学习是有利还是有害,研究结果相互矛盾。一个突出的发现表明,脸的存在掩盖了声音,通过捕捉听者的注意力来阻碍声音身份的学习(脸遮蔽效应;敌人)。在本研究中,我们验证了视听训练对语音识别学习的影响是由注意过程驱动的。参与者要么通过纯音频训练(audio-only),要么通过三种版本的视听训练(在声音旁边放一张脸)来学习识别声音。在视听训练中,这些脸要么看着摄像机(直视),要么看着旁边(避开注视),要么闭着眼睛(不注视)。我们发现凝视对语音身份学习的影响是分级的:语音身份识别在纯音频训练后最准确,而在包括直接凝视的视听训练后最不准确,构成FOE。虽然总体效应大小很小,但在避免凝视和无凝视条件下,FOE的大小减少了一半。与回避或不注视相比,直视与更多的注意力捕获有关,目前的研究结果表明,偶然的注意力捕获至少部分地支撑了敌人。我们从视觉优势效应和面孔与声音对身份感知的相对信息性的角度来讨论这些发现。
{"title":"The effects of the presence of a face and direct eye gaze on voice identity learning","authors":"Nadine Lavan,&nbsp;Nisha Ramanik Bamaniya,&nbsp;Moha-Maryam Muse,&nbsp;Raffaella Lucy Monica Price,&nbsp;Isabelle Mareschal","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12633","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12633","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We rarely become familiar with the voice of another person in isolation but usually also have access to visual identity information, thus learning to recognize their voice and face in parallel. There are conflicting findings as to whether learning to recognize voices in audiovisual vs audio-only settings is advantageous or detrimental to learning. One prominent finding shows that the presence of a face overshadows the voice, hindering voice identity learning by capturing listeners' attention (Face Overshadowing Effect; FOE). In the current study, we tested the proposal that the effect of audiovisual training on voice identity learning is driven by attentional processes. Participants learned to recognize voices through either audio-only training (Audio-Only) or through three versions of audiovisual training, where a face was presented alongside the voices. During audiovisual training, the faces were either looking at the camera (Direct Gaze), were looking to the side (Averted Gaze) or had closed eyes (No Gaze). We found a graded effect of gaze on voice identity learning: Voice identity recognition was most accurate after audio-only training and least accurate after audiovisual training including direct gaze, constituting a FOE. While effect sizes were overall small, the magnitude of FOE was halved for the Averted and No Gaze conditions. With direct gaze being associated with increased attention capture compared to averted or no gaze, the current findings suggest that incidental attention capture at least partially underpins the FOE. We discuss these findings in light of visual dominance effects and the relative informativeness of faces vs voices for identity perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"114 3","pages":"537-549"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12633","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9784344","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1