首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Visual attention to own- versus other-race faces: Perspectives from learning mechanisms and task demands 对本种族与他种族面孔的视觉注意:来自学习机制和任务要求的视角
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12647
Janet H. Hsiao, Antoni B. Chan

Multiple factors have been proposed to contribute to the other-race effect in face recognition, including perceptual expertise and social-cognitive accounts. Here, we propose to understand the effect and its contributing factors from the perspectives of learning mechanisms that involve joint learning of visual attention strategies and internal representations for faces, which can be modulated by quality of contact with other-race individuals including emotional and motivational factors. Computational simulations of this process will enhance our understanding of interactions among factors and help resolve inconsistent results in the literature. In particular, since learning is driven by task demands, visual attention effects observed in different face-processing tasks, such as passive viewing or recognition, are likely to be task specific (although may be associated) and should be examined and compared separately. When examining visual attention strategies, the use of more data-driven and comprehensive eye movement measures, taking both spatial–temporal pattern and consistency of eye movements into account, can lead to novel discoveries in other-race face processing. The proposed framework and analysis methods may be applied to other tasks of real-life significance such as face emotion recognition, further enhancing our understanding of the relationship between learning and visual cognition.

已经提出了多种因素来促进面部识别中的其他种族效应,包括感知专长和社会认知账户。在此,我们建议从学习机制的角度来理解这种效应及其影响因素,包括视觉注意策略的联合学习和面孔的内部表征,这可以通过与其他种族个体的接触质量(包括情绪和动机因素)来调节。这一过程的计算模拟将增强我们对因素之间相互作用的理解,并有助于解决文献中不一致的结果。特别是,由于学习是由任务需求驱动的,在不同的面部处理任务中观察到的视觉注意效应,如被动观看或识别,可能是特定于任务的(尽管可能是相关的),应该单独检查和比较。在研究视觉注意策略时,使用更多的数据驱动和全面的眼球运动测量,考虑到眼球运动的时空模式和一致性,可以在其他种族的面部处理中获得新的发现。所提出的框架和分析方法可以应用于其他具有现实意义的任务,如面部情绪识别,进一步增强我们对学习与视觉认知之间关系的理解。
{"title":"Visual attention to own- versus other-race faces: Perspectives from learning mechanisms and task demands","authors":"Janet H. Hsiao,&nbsp;Antoni B. Chan","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12647","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12647","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Multiple factors have been proposed to contribute to the other-race effect in face recognition, including perceptual expertise and social-cognitive accounts. Here, we propose to understand the effect and its contributing factors from the perspectives of learning mechanisms that involve joint learning of visual attention strategies and internal representations for faces, which can be modulated by quality of contact with other-race individuals including emotional and motivational factors. Computational simulations of this process will enhance our understanding of interactions among factors and help resolve inconsistent results in the literature. In particular, since learning is driven by task demands, visual attention effects observed in different face-processing tasks, such as passive viewing or recognition, are likely to be task specific (although may be associated) and should be examined and compared separately. When examining visual attention strategies, the use of more data-driven and comprehensive eye movement measures, taking both spatial–temporal pattern and consistency of eye movements into account, can lead to novel discoveries in other-race face processing. The proposed framework and analysis methods may be applied to other tasks of real-life significance such as face emotion recognition, further enhancing our understanding of the relationship between learning and visual cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12647","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9530361","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Paradoxical decrease of imitation performance with age in children 儿童模仿能力随年龄增长而矛盾地下降
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-21 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12644
Giovanni Ottoboni, Alessio Toraldo, Riccardo Proietti, Angelo Cangelosi, Alessia Tessari

Imitation development was studied in a cross-sectional design involving 174 primary-school children (aged 6–10), focusing on the effect of actions' complexity and error analysis to infer the underlying cognitive processes. Participants had to imitate the model's actions as if they were in front of a mirror (‘specularly’). Complexity varied across three levels: movements of a single limb; arm and leg of the same body side; or arm and leg of opposite body sides. While the overall error rate decreased with age, this was not true of all error categories. The rate of ‘side’ errors (using a limb of the wrong body side) paradoxically increased with age (from 9 years). However, with increasing age, the error rate also became less sensitive to the complexity of the action. This pattern is consistent with the hypothesis that older children have the working memory (WM) resources and the body knowledge necessary to imitate ‘anatomically’, which leads to additional side errors. Younger children might be paradoxically free from such interference because their WM and/or body knowledge are insufficient for anatomical imitation. Yet, their limited WM resources would prevent them from successfully managing the conflict between spatial codes involved in complex actions (e.g. moving the left arm and the right leg). We also found evidence that action side and content might be stored in separate short-term memory (STM) systems: increasing the number of sides to be encoded only affected side retrieval, but not content retrieval; symmetrically, increasing the content (number of movements) of the action only affected content retrieval, but not side retrieval. In conclusion, results suggest that anatomical imitation might interfere with specular imitation at age 9 and that STM storages for side and content of actions are separate.

采用横断面设计研究了174名6-10岁小学生的模仿发展,重点研究了动作复杂性和错误分析的影响,以推断潜在的认知过程。参与者必须模仿模特的动作,就好像他们在镜子前一样(“镜面”)。复杂性在三个层面上有所不同:单个肢体的运动;手臂与腿同侧身体;或身体两侧的手臂和腿。虽然总体错误率随着年龄的增长而下降,但并非所有错误类别都是如此。“侧面”错误(使用错误的身体一侧的肢体)的比率矛盾地随着年龄的增长而增加(从9岁开始)。然而,随着年龄的增长,错误率对动作的复杂性也变得不那么敏感。这种模式与假设相一致,即年龄较大的儿童拥有“解剖学”模仿所需的工作记忆(WM)资源和身体知识,这导致了额外的侧面错误。年幼的儿童可能自相矛盾地没有这种干扰,因为他们的WM和/或身体知识不足以进行解剖模仿。然而,他们有限的WM资源将阻止他们成功地管理复杂动作(例如移动左臂和右腿)中涉及的空间代码之间的冲突。我们还发现,行动侧和内容侧可能存储在不同的短期记忆系统中:增加编码的侧数仅用于影响侧检索,而不用于内容检索;对称地,增加动作的内容(移动次数)只影响内容检索,而不影响侧面检索。综上所述,解剖模仿可能会干扰9岁儿童的镜面模仿,并且动作的侧面和内容的STM存储是分开的。
{"title":"Paradoxical decrease of imitation performance with age in children","authors":"Giovanni Ottoboni,&nbsp;Alessio Toraldo,&nbsp;Riccardo Proietti,&nbsp;Angelo Cangelosi,&nbsp;Alessia Tessari","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12644","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12644","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Imitation development was studied in a cross-sectional design involving 174 primary-school children (aged 6–10), focusing on the effect of actions' complexity and error analysis to infer the underlying cognitive processes. Participants had to imitate the model's actions as if they were in front of a mirror (‘specularly’). Complexity varied across three levels: movements of a single limb; arm and leg of the same body side; or arm and leg of opposite body sides. While the overall error rate decreased with age, this was not true of all error categories. The rate of ‘side’ errors (using a limb of the wrong body side) paradoxically increased with age (from 9 years). However, with increasing age, the error rate also became less sensitive to the complexity of the action. This pattern is consistent with the hypothesis that older children have the working memory (WM) resources and the body knowledge necessary to imitate ‘anatomically’, which leads to additional side errors. Younger children might be paradoxically free from such interference because their WM and/or body knowledge are insufficient for anatomical imitation. Yet, their limited WM resources would prevent them from successfully managing the conflict between spatial codes involved in complex actions (e.g. moving the left arm and the right leg). We also found evidence that action side and content might be stored in separate short-term memory (STM) systems: increasing the number of sides to be encoded only affected side retrieval, but not content retrieval; symmetrically, increasing the content (number of movements) of the action only affected content retrieval, but not side retrieval. In conclusion, results suggest that anatomical imitation might interfere with specular imitation at age 9 and that STM storages for side and content of actions are separate.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12644","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9779426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Sleep as a mediator of the relationship between social class and health in higher education students 睡眠在高等教育学生社会阶层与健康关系中的中介作用
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-09 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12645
Romany McGuffog, Mark Rubin, Mark Boyes, Marie L. Caltabiano, James Collison, Geoff P. Lovell, Orla Muldoon, Stefania Paolini

A substantial body of research indicates that higher education students from lower social class backgrounds tend to have poorer health than those from higher social class backgrounds. To investigate sleep as a potential mediator of this relationship, online survey responses of students from five large Australian universities, one Irish university and one large Australian technical college were analysed in three studies (Study 1 N = 628; Study 2 N = 376; Study 3 N = 446). The results revealed that sleep quality, sleep duration, sleep disturbances, pre-sleep worries and sleep schedule variability mediated the relationship between social class and physical and mental health. Sleep remained a significant mediator when controlling for related variables and other mediators. Thus, the findings suggest that sleep partly explains social class differences in health. We discuss the importance of addressing sleep issues among students from lower social class backgrounds.

大量研究表明,社会阶层背景较低的高等教育学生往往比社会阶层背景较高的学生健康状况较差。为了研究睡眠作为这种关系的潜在中介,我们在三项研究中分析了来自澳大利亚五所大型大学、一所爱尔兰大学和一所澳大利亚大型技术学院的学生的在线调查反应(研究1 N = 628;研究2 N = 376;研究3 N = 446)。结果表明,睡眠质量、睡眠时间、睡眠障碍、睡眠前焦虑和睡眠时间变化在社会阶层与身心健康的关系中起中介作用。在控制相关变量和其他中介因素时,睡眠仍然是一个重要的中介。因此,研究结果表明睡眠在一定程度上解释了社会阶层在健康方面的差异。我们讨论了在社会阶层背景较低的学生中解决睡眠问题的重要性。
{"title":"Sleep as a mediator of the relationship between social class and health in higher education students","authors":"Romany McGuffog,&nbsp;Mark Rubin,&nbsp;Mark Boyes,&nbsp;Marie L. Caltabiano,&nbsp;James Collison,&nbsp;Geoff P. Lovell,&nbsp;Orla Muldoon,&nbsp;Stefania Paolini","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12645","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12645","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A substantial body of research indicates that higher education students from lower social class backgrounds tend to have poorer health than those from higher social class backgrounds. To investigate sleep as a potential mediator of this relationship, online survey responses of students from five large Australian universities, one Irish university and one large Australian technical college were analysed in three studies (Study 1 <i>N</i> = 628; Study 2 <i>N</i> = 376; Study 3 <i>N</i> = 446). The results revealed that sleep quality, sleep duration, sleep disturbances, pre-sleep worries and sleep schedule variability mediated the relationship between social class and physical and mental health. Sleep remained a significant mediator when controlling for related variables and other mediators. Thus, the findings suggest that sleep partly explains social class differences in health. We discuss the importance of addressing sleep issues among students from lower social class backgrounds.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12645","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9778866","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Event-related potential and behavioural evidence of goal-based expectations for consistent actions among group members 事件相关的潜力和行为证据的目标为基础的期望一致的行动在群体成员之间
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-07 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12643
Jun Yin, Dan Zhou, Danfeng Ai, Hongli Sun, Jipeng Duan, Zhongqiang Sun, Xiuyan Guo

Placeholder Text

People expect group members to act consistently. However, because actions are organized hierarchically, incorporating deep-level goals and shallow-level movements, it remains unclear what level of action is expected to be consistent among group members. We determined that these two levels of action representations can be dissociated in object-directed actions and measured the late positive potential (LPP), which indicates expectation. We found that participants identified a new agent's actions more quickly when this agent pursued a consistent goal while moving in a manner inconsistent with group members than when this agent pursued an inconsistent goal while moving in the same manner as group members. Moreover, this facilitation effect disappeared when the new agent was from a different group, revealing goal-based expectations for consistent actions among group members. The LPP amplitude during the action-expectation phase was greater for agents from the same group than for agents from a different group, suggesting that people implicitly generate clearer action expectations for group members than for other individuals. Additionally, the behavioural facilitation effect was observed when the goal of actions was clearly identifiable (i.e. performing rational actions to reach an external target) rather than when there was no clear association between actions and external targets (i.e. performing irrational actions). The LPP amplitude during the action-expectation phase was greater after observing rational actions than after observing irrational actions performed by two agents from the same group, and the expectation-related increase in LPP predicted the behavioural measurements of the facilitation effect. Hence, the behavioural and event-related potential evidence suggest that people implicitly expect group members to behave consistently according to goals rather than movements per se.

人们希望小组成员的行为一致。然而,因为行动是分层组织的,包含了深层次的目标和浅层的行动,所以在团队成员中,什么层次的行动是一致的仍然是不清楚的。我们确定这两个层次的动作表征可以在对象导向动作中分离,并测量了表明期望的后期正电位(LPP)。我们发现,当一个新agent追求一致的目标,但与小组成员的移动方式不一致时,参与者识别新agent的动作要比这个agent追求不一致的目标,但与小组成员的移动方式相同时更快。此外,当新代理人来自不同的群体时,这种促进效应就消失了,这揭示了群体成员对一致行动的基于目标的期望。在行动期望阶段,来自同一组的行为主体的LPP振幅大于来自不同组的行为主体,这表明人们隐性地对群体成员产生比对其他个体更清晰的行动期望。此外,当行为目标明确时(即采取理性行动以达到外部目标),而不是当行为与外部目标之间没有明确关联时(即采取非理性行动),观察到行为促进效应。观察同一组两名被试的理性行为后,动作-期望阶段LPP的幅度大于观察非理性行为后的幅度,期望相关的LPP增加预测了促进效应的行为测量。因此,行为和事件相关的潜在证据表明,人们隐性地期望群体成员按照目标而不是行动本身一致行事。
{"title":"Event-related potential and behavioural evidence of goal-based expectations for consistent actions among group members","authors":"Jun Yin,&nbsp;Dan Zhou,&nbsp;Danfeng Ai,&nbsp;Hongli Sun,&nbsp;Jipeng Duan,&nbsp;Zhongqiang Sun,&nbsp;Xiuyan Guo","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12643","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12643","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Placeholder Text</h3>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <p>People expect group members to act consistently. However, because actions are organized hierarchically, incorporating deep-level goals and shallow-level movements, it remains unclear what level of action is expected to be consistent among group members. We determined that these two levels of action representations can be dissociated in object-directed actions and measured the late positive potential (LPP), which indicates expectation. We found that participants identified a new agent's actions more quickly when this agent pursued a consistent goal while moving in a manner inconsistent with group members than when this agent pursued an inconsistent goal while moving in the same manner as group members. Moreover, this facilitation effect disappeared when the new agent was from a different group, revealing goal-based expectations for consistent actions among group members. The LPP amplitude during the action-expectation phase was greater for agents from the same group than for agents from a different group, suggesting that people implicitly generate clearer action expectations for group members than for other individuals. Additionally, the behavioural facilitation effect was observed when the goal of actions was clearly identifiable (i.e. performing rational actions to reach an external target) rather than when there was no clear association between actions and external targets (i.e. performing irrational actions). The LPP amplitude during the action-expectation phase was greater after observing rational actions than after observing irrational actions performed by two agents from the same group, and the expectation-related increase in LPP predicted the behavioural measurements of the facilitation effect. Hence, the behavioural and event-related potential evidence suggest that people implicitly expect group members to behave consistently according to goals rather than movements per se.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9788897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Construal level among poor children: Executive function implications 贫困儿童的解释水平:执行功能的影响
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-04 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12642
Yi Ren, Chenyi Zuo, Hua Ming, Ying Jiang, Silin Huang

Poverty impedes children's executive function (EF). Therefore, it is necessary to mitigate the negative effect of poverty by developing efficient interventions to improve poor children's cognitive function. In three studies, we examined whether high-level construals can improve EF among poor children in China. In Study 1, we observed a positive relationship between family socioeconomic status and children's EF, which was moderated by construal level (n = 206; Mage = 9.71; 45.6% girls). In Study 2a, we experimentally induced high- versus low-level construals and found that poor children with high-level construals exhibited better EF than those with low-level construals (n = 65; Mage = 11.32; 47.7% girls). However, the same intervention did not affect the performance of affluent children in Study 2b (n = 63; Mage = 10.54; 54% girls). Moreover, we found that the interventional effects of high-level construals improved the ability of children living in poverty to make healthy decisions and delayed gratification in Study 3 (n = 74; Mage = 11.10; 45.9% girls). These findings may have implications for using high-level construals as an effective intervention to improve poor children's EF and cognitive capacity.

贫困阻碍了儿童的执行功能。因此,有必要通过制定有效的干预措施来改善贫困儿童的认知功能,以减轻贫困的负面影响。在三项研究中,我们考察了高水平识解是否能提高中国贫困儿童的英语水平。在研究1中,我们观察到家庭社会经济地位与儿童EF之间存在正相关关系,这一关系被解释水平调节(n = 206;法师= 9.71;45.6%的女孩)。在研究2a中,我们通过实验诱导高水平和低水平的识解,发现具有高水平识解的贫困儿童比具有低水平识解的贫困儿童表现出更好的EF (n = 65;法师= 11.32;47.7%的女孩)。然而,在研究2b中,同样的干预并没有影响富裕儿童的表现(n = 63;法师= 10.54;54%的女孩)。此外,我们在研究3中发现,高水平识解的干预作用提高了贫困儿童做出健康决定和延迟满足的能力(n = 74;法师= 11.10;45.9%的女孩)。这些发现可能对使用高水平识解作为一种有效的干预措施来改善贫困儿童的EF和认知能力具有启示意义。
{"title":"Construal level among poor children: Executive function implications","authors":"Yi Ren,&nbsp;Chenyi Zuo,&nbsp;Hua Ming,&nbsp;Ying Jiang,&nbsp;Silin Huang","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12642","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12642","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Poverty impedes children's executive function (EF). Therefore, it is necessary to mitigate the negative effect of poverty by developing efficient interventions to improve poor children's cognitive function. In three studies, we examined whether high-level construals can improve EF among poor children in China. In Study 1, we observed a positive relationship between family socioeconomic status and children's EF, which was moderated by construal level (<i>n</i> = 206; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 9.71; 45.6% girls). In Study 2a, we experimentally induced high- versus low-level construals and found that poor children with high-level construals exhibited better EF than those with low-level construals (<i>n</i> = 65; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 11.32; 47.7% girls). However, the same intervention did not affect the performance of affluent children in Study 2b (<i>n</i> = 63; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 10.54; 54% girls). Moreover, we found that the interventional effects of high-level construals improved the ability of children living in poverty to make healthy decisions and delayed gratification in Study 3 (<i>n</i> = 74; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 11.10; 45.9% girls). These findings may have implications for using high-level construals as an effective intervention to improve poor children's EF and cognitive capacity.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10164642","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Pain in the eye of the beholder: Variations in pain visual representations as a function of face ethnicity and culture 旁观者眼中的疼痛:面部种族和文化对疼痛视觉表征的影响
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-02 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12641
Francis Gingras, Daniel Fiset, Marie-Pier Plouffe-Demers, Andréa Deschênes, Stéphanie Cormier, Hélène Forget, Caroline Blais

Pain experienced by Black individuals is systematically underestimated, and recent studies have shown that part of this bias is rooted in perceptual factors. We used Reverse Correlation to estimate visual representations of the pain expression in Black and White faces, in participants originating from both Western and African countries. Groups of raters were then asked to evaluate the presence of pain and other emotions in these representations. A second group of White raters then evaluated those same representations placed over a neutral background face (50% White; 50% Black). Image-based analyses show significant effects of culture and face ethnicity, but no interaction between the two factors. Western representations were more likely to be judged as expressing pain than African representations. For both cultural groups, raters also perceived more pain in White face representations than in Black face representations. However, when changing the background stimulus to the neutral background face, this effect of face ethnic profile disappeared. Overall, these results suggest that individuals have different expectations of how pain is expressed by Black and White individuals, and that cultural factors may explain a part of this phenomenon

黑人所经历的痛苦被系统性地低估了,最近的研究表明,这种偏见部分源于感知因素。我们使用反向相关来估计来自西方和非洲国家的黑人和白人面部疼痛表情的视觉表征。然后要求一组评分者评估这些表征中疼痛和其他情绪的存在。然后,第二组白人评分者对同样的表现进行评估,这些表现放在中性背景的脸上(50%是白人;50%的黑人)。基于图像的分析显示文化和面部种族有显著影响,但两者之间没有相互作用。与非洲的表现相比,西方的表现更有可能被认为是在表达痛苦。在这两个文化群体中,评分者在白人面孔表征中比在黑人面孔表征中感知到更多的痛苦。然而,当将背景刺激改为中性背景面部时,这种影响消失了。总的来说,这些结果表明,个体对黑人和白人如何表达疼痛有不同的期望,文化因素可能解释了这一现象的一部分
{"title":"Pain in the eye of the beholder: Variations in pain visual representations as a function of face ethnicity and culture","authors":"Francis Gingras,&nbsp;Daniel Fiset,&nbsp;Marie-Pier Plouffe-Demers,&nbsp;Andréa Deschênes,&nbsp;Stéphanie Cormier,&nbsp;Hélène Forget,&nbsp;Caroline Blais","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12641","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12641","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Pain experienced by Black individuals is systematically underestimated, and recent studies have shown that part of this bias is rooted in perceptual factors. We used <i>Reverse Correlation</i> to estimate visual representations of the pain expression in Black and White faces, in participants originating from both Western and African countries. Groups of raters were then asked to evaluate the presence of pain and other emotions in these representations. A second group of White raters then evaluated those same representations placed over a neutral background face (50% White; 50% Black). Image-based analyses show significant effects of culture and face ethnicity, but no interaction between the two factors. Western representations were more likely to be judged as expressing pain than African representations. For both cultural groups, raters also perceived more pain in White face representations than in Black face representations. However, when changing the background stimulus to the neutral background face, this effect of face ethnic profile disappeared. Overall, these results suggest that individuals have different expectations of how pain is expressed by Black and White individuals, and that cultural factors may explain a part of this phenomenon</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12641","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9787989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Empathy and parenthood: The moderating role of maternal trait empathy on parental burnout 共情与亲子关系:母亲特质共情对父母倦怠的调节作用
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-27 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12640
Tamar Kadosh-Laor, Liat Israeli-Ran, Ido Shalev, Florina Uzefovsky

The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; N = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, N = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.

同情他人的能力使我们能够有效地与他人互动,并可能特别进化到支持父母的角色和照顾。由于以往的研究只关注父母教养背景下的共情,例如父母的敏感性,因此人们对父母教养与共情特质之间的关系知之甚少。本研究旨在了解共情特质如何调节儿童负性情绪与父母倦怠之间的关系。研究了两个队列:(1)婴儿的父母(10-18个月大;N = 203)和(2)3-10岁儿童的父母,N = 201)。父母们填写了一系列在线问卷,评估母亲的同理心、父母的倦怠和孩子的性情。研究发现,高负性情绪水平与父母职业倦怠之间的关系受到母亲情感共情的特定方面的调节。我们的研究结果表明,当面对孩子的特征导致父母的更高要求时,母亲的情感共情可以缓冲父母的倦怠。
{"title":"Empathy and parenthood: The moderating role of maternal trait empathy on parental burnout","authors":"Tamar Kadosh-Laor,&nbsp;Liat Israeli-Ran,&nbsp;Ido Shalev,&nbsp;Florina Uzefovsky","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12640","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12640","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to empathize with others enables us to effectively interact with each other and may have specifically evolved to support parental roles and caregiving. The relationship between parenting and trait empathy is little understood as previous research focused on empathy exclusively in the context of parenting, for example parental sensitivity. Here we aimed to understand how trait empathy may moderate the association between child's negative emotionality and parental burnout. Two cohorts were examined (1) parents of infants (10–18 months old; <i>N</i> = 203) and (2) parents of children (3–10 years old, <i>N</i> = 201). Parents filled out a battery of online questionnaires assessing maternal empathy, parental burnout and child temperament. We found that the relationship between higher levels of negative emotionality and parental burnout is moderated by specific aspects of maternal emotional empathy. Our findings suggest that maternal emotional empathy acts as a buffer against parental burnout when faced with a child's characteristics that incur higher parental demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12640","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9778816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study 比较音乐和食物在年轻人和老年人中唤起的自传式记忆:一项日记研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-13 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12639
Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, Lia Kvavilashvili, Abbigail Ely, Mark Gill, Gemma Herbert

Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.

先前的研究发现,音乐比其他各种检索线索更能唤起生动、情绪化的自传式记忆。然而,这类研究的生态效度往往较低,并且受到相对有限的线索选择和主要是年轻人样本的限制。在这里,我们比较了音乐和食物作为年轻人和老年人日常生活中自传式记忆的线索。39名年龄在18-34岁之间的年轻人和39名年龄在60-77岁之间的老年人在两个独立的为期四天的时间段内,在纸日记中记录了他们的音乐和食物引发的自传式记忆。在这两个年龄组中,音乐触发的自传式记忆更频繁,非自愿记忆的比例更大,与食物线索相比,记忆对个人更重要。年龄差异对音乐和食物唤起的记忆的影响相似,老年人总是能回忆起更早、更不具体的记忆,他们认为这些记忆更积极、更生动、更有预演。然而,年轻人和老年人在记录记忆的数量或非自愿性质上没有区别。这项工作代表了理解成年期自然发生的音乐诱发自传式记忆的现象学的重要一步,并为音乐如何以及为什么可能比某些其他日常线索更有效地触发个人价值记忆提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Comparing music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in young and older adults: A diary study","authors":"Kelly Jakubowski,&nbsp;Amy M. Belfi,&nbsp;Lia Kvavilashvili,&nbsp;Abbigail Ely,&nbsp;Mark Gill,&nbsp;Gemma Herbert","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12639","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12639","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research has found that music brings back more vivid and emotional autobiographical memories than various other retrieval cues. However, such studies have often been low in ecological validity and constrained by relatively limited cue selection and predominantly young adult samples. Here, we compared music to food as cues for autobiographical memories in everyday life in young and older adults. In two separate four-day periods, 39 younger (ages 18–34) and 39 older (ages 60–77) adults recorded their music- and food-evoked autobiographical memories in paper diaries. Across both age groups, music triggered more frequent autobiographical memories, a greater proportion of involuntary memories, and memories rated as more personally important in comparison to food cues. Age differences impacted music- and food-evoked memories similarly, with older adults consistently recalling older and less specific memories, which they rated as more positive, vivid, and rehearsed. However, young and older adults did not differ in the number or involuntary nature of their recorded memories. This work represents an important step in understanding the phenomenology of naturally occurring music-evoked autobiographical memories across adulthood and provides new insights into how and why music may be a more effective trigger for personally valued memories than certain other everyday cues.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12639","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9854772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Food word processing in Chinese reading: A study of restrained eaters 汉语阅读中的食物词加工:一项限制进食者的研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-10 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12638
Changlin Luo, Mengyan Zhu, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma

Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.

与食物相关的注意偏差是指与非食物刺激相比,个体通常优先考虑与食物相关的奖励线索(如食物单词和食物图像);然而,对于节制饮食的人来说,研究结果并不一致。传统的食物注意偏差测试范式,如视觉探测任务和视觉搜索任务,可能不足以直接和准确地反映个体在不同认知阶段的食物词加工。在这项研究中,我们引入了边界范式来研究限制和不限制进食者的食物词注意偏差。当他们进行自然的句子阅读任务时,他们的眼球运动被记录下来。后期分析的结果表明,食物词的注视时间比非食物词的注视时间要短,这表明无论进食限制还是不进食限制,中央凹对食物词的处理都具有优势。前期分析结果表明,在食物词有效预览条件下,克制型进食者在目标前区域花费的时间更多,这表明克制型进食者在食物词加工上具有优势(即中央凹对中央凹效应)。中央凹食物字加工的优越性为解释一般群体中与食物相关的注意偏倚提供了新的见解。此外,限制进食者在旁中央凹加工中增强的食物词注意偏倚说明了个体特征在研究单词识别中的重要性。
{"title":"Food word processing in Chinese reading: A study of restrained eaters","authors":"Changlin Luo,&nbsp;Mengyan Zhu,&nbsp;Xiangling Zhuang,&nbsp;Guojie Ma","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12638","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12638","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Food-related attentional bias refers that individuals typically prioritize rewarding food-related cues (e.g. food words and food images) compared with non-food stimuli; however, the findings are inconsistent for restrained eaters. Traditional paradigms used to test food-related attentional bias, such as visual probe tasks and visual search tasks, may not directly and accurately enough to reflect individuals' food-word processing at different cognitive stages. In this study, we introduced the boundary paradigm to investigate food-word attentional bias for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. Eye movements were recorded when they performed a naturalistic sentence-reading task. The results of later-stage analyses showed that food words were fixated on for less time than non-food words, which indicated a superiority of foveal food-word processing for both restrained and unrestrained eaters. The results of early-stage analyses showed that restrained eaters spent more time on pre-target regions in the food-word valid preview conditions, which indicated a parafoveal food-word processing superiority for restrained eaters (i.e. the parafoveal-on-foveal effect). The superiority of foveal food-word processing provides new insights into explaining food-related attentional bias in general groups. Additionally, the enhanced food-word attentional bias in parafoveal processing for restrained eaters illustrates the importance of individual characteristics in studying word recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9273486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Electrophysiological correlates of dispositional mindfulness: A quantitative and complexity EEG study 性情性正念的电生理相关:一项定量和复杂的脑电图研究
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-07 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12636
Nuria Victoria Aguerre, Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza, Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina, María Teresa Bajo

While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.

虽然越来越多的证据表明,性格正念与心理健康和认知增强有关,但迄今为止,只有少数人试图描述其神经基础。在本研究中,我们旨在通过静息状态和执行学习任务时的EEG定量和复杂性测量来探索气质正念的电生理特征。采用五方面正念问卷对120名参与者进行了5分钟闭眼静息状态和5分钟任务脑电图记录。我们假设,与低正念个体相比,高正念个体的大脑活动模式与(a)休息时较低的默认模式网络(DMN)参与(额叶伽马功率降低)和(b)从休息到任务的“任务准备”状态反映出更相似的模式(休息时总体q-EEG功率降低,但任务时没有)相关。性格正念与休息时额叶伽玛能量降低和休息时总能量降低显著相关,但与任务无关。此外,我们发现正念个体在任务执行过程中有更高熵的趋势,这一趋势最近在正念冥想中得到了报道。总之,我们的研究结果与冥想专家的研究结果相结合,表明高(倾向)正念似乎具有一种特定的电生理模式,即较少参与DMN和走神过程。
{"title":"Electrophysiological correlates of dispositional mindfulness: A quantitative and complexity EEG study","authors":"Nuria Victoria Aguerre,&nbsp;Carlos Javier Gómez-Ariza,&nbsp;Antonio José Ibáñez-Molina,&nbsp;María Teresa Bajo","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12636","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12636","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While growing evidence supports that dispositional mindfulness relates to psychological health and cognitive enhancement, to date there have been only a few attempts to characterize its neural underpinnings. In the present study, we aimed at exploring the electrophysiological (EEG) signature of dispositional mindfulness using quantitative and complexity measures of EEG during resting state and while performing a learning task. Hundred twenty participants were assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and underwent 5 min eyes-closed resting state and 5 min at task EEG recording. We hypothesized that high mindfulness individuals would show patterns of brain activity related to (a) lower involvement of the default mode network (DMN) at rest (reduced frontal gamma power) and (b) a state of ‘task readiness’ reflected in a more similar pattern from rest to task (reduced overall q-EEG power at rest but not at task), as compared to their low mindfulness counterparts. Dispositional mindfulness was significantly linked to reduced frontal gamma power at rest and lower overall power during rest but not at task. In addition, we found a trend towards higher entropy during task performance in mindful individuals, which has recently been reported during mindfulness meditation. Altogether, our results add to those from expert meditators to show that high (dispositional) mindfulness seems to have a specific electrophysiological pattern characteristic of less involvement of the DMN and mind-wandering processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12636","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9790273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1