首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Language about gender/sex should be used intentionally and flexibly. 关于性别/性的语言应该有意而灵活地使用。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-15 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12769
Thekla Morgenroth

In their paper, 'Conceptualizing transgender experiences in psychology: Do we have a 'true' gender?' Jackson and Bussey (British Journal of Psychology, 115, 723) critique the idea of having a 'true' gender and propose that the term 'transgender experience' may be more appropriate than 'transgender identity'. In this commentary, I reflect on the usefulness of the terms transgender identity and transgender experience and argue that both hold value and can contribute to a more nuanced discussion of gender/sex. I use the discussion of these two terms as a springboard to make a broader point: As researchers, we should use language about gender/sex flexibly and intentionally. As psychologists, we are trained to use clear operationalizations for the constructs we study, yet it is often unclear whether authors refer to biological sex, gender identity, social perception, or socialization when they speak of 'women' and 'men'. I encourage researchers to be more mindful in their language use and to engage with the nuanced terms that gender scholars (including Jackson and Bussey) have put forward when discussing gender/sex - both in the context of transgender identities and experiences and when discussing cisgender identities and experiences.

在他们的论文《心理学中跨性别体验的概念化:我们有‘真正的’性别吗?》Jackson和Bussey (British Journal of Psychology, 115,723)批评了拥有“真实”性别的想法,并提出“跨性别体验”这个词可能比“跨性别身份”更合适。在这篇评论中,我反思了跨性别认同和跨性别体验这两个术语的有用性,并认为它们都有价值,可以促进对性别/性的更细致入微的讨论。我把这两个术语的讨论作为跳板,提出一个更广泛的观点:作为研究人员,我们应该灵活而有意地使用关于性别/性的语言。作为心理学家,我们受过训练,要对我们研究的概念使用清晰的操作化,然而,当作者谈到“女性”和“男性”时,他们通常不清楚是指生理性别、性别认同、社会感知还是社会化。我鼓励研究人员在使用语言时更加注意,并使用性别学者(包括Jackson和Bussey)在讨论性别/性时提出的微妙术语——无论是在跨性别身份和经历的背景下,还是在讨论顺性别身份和经历的背景下。
{"title":"Language about gender/sex should be used intentionally and flexibly.","authors":"Thekla Morgenroth","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12769","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12769","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In their paper, 'Conceptualizing transgender experiences in psychology: Do we have a 'true' gender?' Jackson and Bussey (British Journal of Psychology, 115, 723) critique the idea of having a 'true' gender and propose that the term 'transgender experience' may be more appropriate than 'transgender identity'. In this commentary, I reflect on the usefulness of the terms transgender identity and transgender experience and argue that both hold value and can contribute to a more nuanced discussion of gender/sex. I use the discussion of these two terms as a springboard to make a broader point: As researchers, we should use language about gender/sex flexibly and intentionally. As psychologists, we are trained to use clear operationalizations for the constructs we study, yet it is often unclear whether authors refer to biological sex, gender identity, social perception, or socialization when they speak of 'women' and 'men'. I encourage researchers to be more mindful in their language use and to engage with the nuanced terms that gender scholars (including Jackson and Bussey) have put forward when discussing gender/sex - both in the context of transgender identities and experiences and when discussing cisgender identities and experiences.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143000545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nuance and specificity always matter-Especially for all the meanings of 'gender': A reply to Jackson and Bussey (2024). 细微差别和特殊性总是很重要——尤其是对于“性别”的所有含义:对杰克逊和布西(2024)的回应。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-14 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12771
Charlotte Chucky Tate

Jackson and Bussey (The British Journal of Psychology, 2024, 115, 723) present the provocative argument that the concept of a "true gender" is not a useful conceptual object for psychology. While the article does make 2 valid and worthwhile points, the remainder of that article lacks necessary nuance in developing its general conclusion.

Jackson和Bussey (The British Journal of Psychology, 2024, 115,723)提出了一个具有挑衅性的论点,即“真实性别”的概念对心理学来说不是一个有用的概念对象。虽然这篇文章确实提出了两个有效且有价值的观点,但文章的其余部分在发展其总体结论方面缺乏必要的细微差别。
{"title":"Nuance and specificity always matter-Especially for all the meanings of 'gender': A reply to Jackson and Bussey (2024).","authors":"Charlotte Chucky Tate","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12771","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12771","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Jackson and Bussey (The British Journal of Psychology, 2024, 115, 723) present the provocative argument that the concept of a \"true gender\" is not a useful conceptual object for psychology. While the article does make 2 valid and worthwhile points, the remainder of that article lacks necessary nuance in developing its general conclusion.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142982884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
New evidence finds young people in Mainland China are now bicultural. 新的证据表明,中国大陆的年轻人现在是双文化的。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12767
Yi-Meng Wang, Feng-Yan Wang, Thomas Talhelm, Yi-Qun Chen

This study reports new evidence that young people in Mainland China are now bicultural. We followed the established method of testing biculturalism by priming participants with images from two different cultures and measuring whether those images activate different thought styles. First, we replicated findings from 25 years ago that college students in Hong Kong are bicultural (Study 1). Next, we found that priming Mainland Chinese college students with Chinese culture increased external attributions (which are more common in China), whereas priming American culture increased internal attributions (which are more common in the US; Study 2). Next, we tested a "negative control" group that we expected should not respond to bicultural primes. Older adults who were born before China's Reform and Opening policy in 1978 showed no evidence of biculturalism (Study 3). This new evidence extends biculturalism to Mainland China, and it provides a crucial negative control test for biculturalism research.

本研究报告提供了新的证据,证明中国大陆的年轻人现在已经具有两种文化。我们按照测试双文化的既定方法,向参与者提供两种不同文化的图像,并测量这些图像是否激活了不同的思维方式。首先,我们复制了 25 年前的研究结果,即香港大学生具有双文化(研究 1)。接着,我们发现,以中国文化为引子的中国大陆大学生会增加外部归因(这在中国更为常见),而以美国文化为引子的中国大陆大学生会增加内部归因(这在美国更为常见;研究 2)。接下来,我们测试了一个 "阴性对照 "组,我们预计该组不会对双文化引物做出反应。1978年中国改革开放前出生的老年人没有表现出双文化倾向(研究3)。这一新证据将双文化延伸到了中国大陆,并为双文化研究提供了重要的阴性对照测试。
{"title":"New evidence finds young people in Mainland China are now bicultural.","authors":"Yi-Meng Wang, Feng-Yan Wang, Thomas Talhelm, Yi-Qun Chen","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12767","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12767","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study reports new evidence that young people in Mainland China are now bicultural. We followed the established method of testing biculturalism by priming participants with images from two different cultures and measuring whether those images activate different thought styles. First, we replicated findings from 25 years ago that college students in Hong Kong are bicultural (Study 1). Next, we found that priming Mainland Chinese college students with Chinese culture increased external attributions (which are more common in China), whereas priming American culture increased internal attributions (which are more common in the US; Study 2). Next, we tested a \"negative control\" group that we expected should not respond to bicultural primes. Older adults who were born before China's Reform and Opening policy in 1978 showed no evidence of biculturalism (Study 3). This new evidence extends biculturalism to Mainland China, and it provides a crucial negative control test for biculturalism research.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142969694","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Leading by example: Experimental evidence that therapist lived experience disclosures can model the path to recovery for clients. 以身作则:实验证据表明,治疗师的生活经历披露可以为客户提供康复之路的模型。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-11 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12759
Alysia M Robertson, Tegan Cruwys, Mark Stevens, Michael J Platow

A common guideline for self-disclosure is that therapists should only share recovered personal experiences with clients (i.e., no longer distressing). However, theoretical rationale and empirical support for this claim is limited. Drawing on identity leadership theorizing, we investigated whether recovery disclosures are beneficial to the extent that they signal a therapist's aspirational prototypicality (i.e., embodiment of an aspirational identity for clients). Across two experimental studies (N = 545), we recruited clients, therapists and general population adults. Participants read a group therapy for depression vignette in which the therapist disclosed: nothing, professional experience with depression, current depression, recovered depression or recovered anxiety. Participants rated the prototypicality of the therapist, the extent to which they perceived the therapist positively, the therapist's expertness and the expected prognosis for therapy. Contrary to our hypotheses, the type of disclosure did not significantly affect positive perceptions, expertness or expected prognosis ratings. However, the therapist disclosing a recovered and relevant condition (recovered depression) was rated as significantly more aspirationally prototypical than the other therapists. Given prior evidence that group therapists are more effective when viewed as aspirationally prototypical, our findings suggest that recovery disclosures may represent one way therapists can signal their prototypicality and enhance their effectiveness.

自我表露的一个共同准则是,治疗师应该只与来访者分享恢复后的个人经历(即不再痛苦)。然而,这一主张的理论基础和实证支持是有限的。利用身份领导理论,我们调查了恢复披露是否在一定程度上是有益的,因为它们表明了治疗师的理想原型(即,客户的理想身份的具体化)。在两项实验研究中(N = 545),我们招募了客户、治疗师和普通成年人。参与者阅读了一篇抑郁症团体治疗的小短文,其中治疗师透露了:什么都没有,抑郁症的专业经历,目前的抑郁症,抑郁症恢复或焦虑恢复。参与者对治疗师的原型性、他们对治疗师的积极感知程度、治疗师的专业程度和对治疗的预期预后进行了评分。与我们的假设相反,披露的类型并没有显著影响积极的感知、熟练程度或预期预后评分。然而,治疗师透露一个恢复和相关的条件(恢复抑郁症)被评为明显比其他治疗师更理想的原型。鉴于先前的证据表明,当被视为理想原型时,团体治疗师更有效,我们的研究结果表明,恢复披露可能是治疗师表明其原型性并提高其有效性的一种方式。
{"title":"Leading by example: Experimental evidence that therapist lived experience disclosures can model the path to recovery for clients.","authors":"Alysia M Robertson, Tegan Cruwys, Mark Stevens, Michael J Platow","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12759","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12759","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A common guideline for self-disclosure is that therapists should only share recovered personal experiences with clients (i.e., no longer distressing). However, theoretical rationale and empirical support for this claim is limited. Drawing on identity leadership theorizing, we investigated whether recovery disclosures are beneficial to the extent that they signal a therapist's aspirational prototypicality (i.e., embodiment of an aspirational identity for clients). Across two experimental studies (N = 545), we recruited clients, therapists and general population adults. Participants read a group therapy for depression vignette in which the therapist disclosed: nothing, professional experience with depression, current depression, recovered depression or recovered anxiety. Participants rated the prototypicality of the therapist, the extent to which they perceived the therapist positively, the therapist's expertness and the expected prognosis for therapy. Contrary to our hypotheses, the type of disclosure did not significantly affect positive perceptions, expertness or expected prognosis ratings. However, the therapist disclosing a recovered and relevant condition (recovered depression) was rated as significantly more aspirationally prototypical than the other therapists. Given prior evidence that group therapists are more effective when viewed as aspirationally prototypical, our findings suggest that recovery disclosures may represent one way therapists can signal their prototypicality and enhance their effectiveness.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142963901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the screen: Dissecting the nexus of victimization and cyberhate among adolescents through excessive internet use, online interactions with strangers and parental restrictions. 屏幕之外:剖析青少年因过度使用互联网、与陌生人在线互动和父母限制而造成的受害和网络仇恨之间的联系。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12766
Liliia Korol, Catherine Blaya

Prior research has established that being a target of offline and online victimization might function as a significant risk factor that increases the likelihood of adolescents' involvement in cyberhate. Yet, relatively little is known about the mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying this relationship. To fill this important gap in knowledge, the present study aims to examine (1) whether excessive Internet use and contact with unknown people online act as sequential mediators in the relationship between overall victimization and youth's involvement in cyberhate; and (2) whether restrictive parental mediation has any role to play in moderating this relationship. The findings suggest that adolescents who experience victimization are more likely to turn to using the Internet excessively, and consequently interact with strangers online, which in turn makes them more prone to becoming victim to cyberhate or spreading hateful content online themselves. Moreover, restrictive parental mediation was shown to exacerbate the link between excessive Internet use and adolescents' contacts with unknown people online, thereby putting them at higher risk of cyberhate involvement. The current study emphasizes the need for a balanced approach to parental mediation - one that fosters open communication, trust and the development of digital literacy skills.

先前的研究已经确定,成为离线和在线受害者的目标可能是增加青少年参与网络仇恨的可能性的一个重要风险因素。然而,对这种关系的中介和调节机制的了解相对较少。为了填补这一重要的知识空白,本研究旨在检验(1)过度使用互联网和在网上与陌生人接触是否在总体受害与青少年参与网络仇恨之间的关系中起序贯中介作用;(2)限制性父母调解是否对这种关系有调节作用。研究结果表明,遭受过伤害的青少年更有可能过度使用互联网,从而在网上与陌生人互动,这反过来又使他们更容易成为网络仇恨的受害者,或者自己在网上传播仇恨内容。此外,限制性父母调解被证明会加剧过度使用互联网与青少年在网上与陌生人接触之间的联系,从而使他们面临更高的网络仇恨参与风险。目前的研究强调需要一种平衡的父母调解方法——一种促进开放沟通、信任和数字素养技能发展的方法。
{"title":"Beyond the screen: Dissecting the nexus of victimization and cyberhate among adolescents through excessive internet use, online interactions with strangers and parental restrictions.","authors":"Liliia Korol, Catherine Blaya","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12766","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12766","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has established that being a target of offline and online victimization might function as a significant risk factor that increases the likelihood of adolescents' involvement in cyberhate. Yet, relatively little is known about the mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying this relationship. To fill this important gap in knowledge, the present study aims to examine (1) whether excessive Internet use and contact with unknown people online act as sequential mediators in the relationship between overall victimization and youth's involvement in cyberhate; and (2) whether restrictive parental mediation has any role to play in moderating this relationship. The findings suggest that adolescents who experience victimization are more likely to turn to using the Internet excessively, and consequently interact with strangers online, which in turn makes them more prone to becoming victim to cyberhate or spreading hateful content online themselves. Moreover, restrictive parental mediation was shown to exacerbate the link between excessive Internet use and adolescents' contacts with unknown people online, thereby putting them at higher risk of cyberhate involvement. The current study emphasizes the need for a balanced approach to parental mediation - one that fosters open communication, trust and the development of digital literacy skills.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142926539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Artificial intelligence chatbots mimic human collective behaviour. 人工智能聊天机器人模仿人类的集体行为。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-31 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12764
James K He, Felix P S Wallis, Andrés Gvirtz, Steve Rathje

Artificial Intelligence (AI) chatbots, such as ChatGPT, have been shown to mimic individual human behaviour in a wide range of psychological and economic tasks. Do groups of AI chatbots also mimic collective behaviour? If so, artificial societies of AI chatbots may aid social scientific research by simulating human collectives. To investigate this theoretical possibility, we focus on whether AI chatbots natively mimic one commonly observed collective behaviour: homophily, people's tendency to form communities with similar others. In a large simulated online society of AI chatbots powered by large language models (N = 33,299), we find that communities form over time around bots using a common language. In addition, among chatbots that predominantly use English (N = 17,746), communities emerge around bots that post similar content. These initial empirical findings suggest that AI chatbots mimic homophily, a key aspect of human collective behaviour. Thus, in addition to simulating individual human behaviour, AI-powered artificial societies may advance social science research by allowing researchers to simulate nuanced aspects of collective behaviour.

人工智能(AI)聊天机器人,如ChatGPT,已经被证明可以在广泛的心理和经济任务中模仿人类的个体行为。人工智能聊天机器人群也会模仿集体行为吗?如果是这样,由人工智能聊天机器人组成的人工社会可能会通过模拟人类集体来帮助社会科学研究。为了研究这种理论上的可能性,我们关注人工智能聊天机器人是否天生模仿一种常见的集体行为:同质性,即人们倾向于与相似的人组成社区。在一个由大型语言模型(N = 33,299)驱动的人工智能聊天机器人的大型模拟在线社会中,我们发现随着时间的推移,使用共同语言的机器人形成了社区。此外,在主要使用英语的聊天机器人中(N = 17,746),围绕发布类似内容的机器人出现了社区。这些初步的实证发现表明,人工智能聊天机器人模仿了同质性,这是人类集体行为的一个关键方面。因此,除了模拟个人行为外,人工智能驱动的人工社会还可以通过允许研究人员模拟集体行为的细微方面来推进社会科学研究。
{"title":"Artificial intelligence chatbots mimic human collective behaviour.","authors":"James K He, Felix P S Wallis, Andrés Gvirtz, Steve Rathje","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12764","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12764","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Artificial Intelligence (AI) chatbots, such as ChatGPT, have been shown to mimic individual human behaviour in a wide range of psychological and economic tasks. Do groups of AI chatbots also mimic collective behaviour? If so, artificial societies of AI chatbots may aid social scientific research by simulating human collectives. To investigate this theoretical possibility, we focus on whether AI chatbots natively mimic one commonly observed collective behaviour: homophily, people's tendency to form communities with similar others. In a large simulated online society of AI chatbots powered by large language models (N = 33,299), we find that communities form over time around bots using a common language. In addition, among chatbots that predominantly use English (N = 17,746), communities emerge around bots that post similar content. These initial empirical findings suggest that AI chatbots mimic homophily, a key aspect of human collective behaviour. Thus, in addition to simulating individual human behaviour, AI-powered artificial societies may advance social science research by allowing researchers to simulate nuanced aspects of collective behaviour.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142909342","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bridging minds and machines: Unmasking the limits in text-based automatic personality recognition for enhanced psychology-AI synergy. 连接思想与机器:揭开基于文本的自动人格识别的局限,增强心理学与人工智能的协同作用。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-18 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12755
Avanti Bhandarkar, Ronald Wilson, Anushka Swarup, Gregory D Webster, Damon Woodard

Text-based automatic personality recognition (APR) operates at the intersection of artificial intelligence (AI) and psychology to determine the personality of an individual from their text sample. This covert form of personality assessment is key for a variety of online applications that contribute to individual convenience and well-being such as that of chatbots and personal assistants. Despite the availability of good quality data utilizing state-of-the-art AI methods, the reported performance of these recognition systems remains below expectations in comparable areas. Consequently, this work investigates and identifies the source of this performance limit and attributes it to the flawed assumptions of text-based APR. These insights are obtained via a large-scale comprehensive benchmark and analysis of text data from five corpora with diverse characteristics and complementary personality models (Big Five and Dark Triad) applied to an assortment of AI methods ranging from hand-crafted linguistic features to data-driven transformers. Finally, the work concludes by identifying the open problems that can help navigate the limitations in text-based automatic personality recognition to a great extent.

基于文本的自动人格识别(APR)在人工智能(AI)和心理学的交叉点上运行,从他们的文本样本中确定个人的个性。这种隐蔽的人格评估形式是各种在线应用程序的关键,这些应用程序有助于个人的便利和福祉,如聊天机器人和个人助理。尽管利用最先进的人工智能方法可以获得高质量的数据,但这些识别系统的报告性能仍然低于可比领域的预期。因此,这项工作调查并确定了这种性能限制的来源,并将其归因于基于文本的apr的有缺陷的假设。这些见解是通过对具有不同特征和互补人格模型(Big five和Dark Triad)的五个语料库的文本数据进行大规模综合基准测试和分析获得的,这些语料库应用于各种人工智能方法,从手工制作的语言特征到数据驱动的转换器。最后,本文总结了一些开放的问题,这些问题可以在很大程度上帮助解决基于文本的自动人格识别的局限性。
{"title":"Bridging minds and machines: Unmasking the limits in text-based automatic personality recognition for enhanced psychology-AI synergy.","authors":"Avanti Bhandarkar, Ronald Wilson, Anushka Swarup, Gregory D Webster, Damon Woodard","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12755","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12755","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Text-based automatic personality recognition (APR) operates at the intersection of artificial intelligence (AI) and psychology to determine the personality of an individual from their text sample. This covert form of personality assessment is key for a variety of online applications that contribute to individual convenience and well-being such as that of chatbots and personal assistants. Despite the availability of good quality data utilizing state-of-the-art AI methods, the reported performance of these recognition systems remains below expectations in comparable areas. Consequently, this work investigates and identifies the source of this performance limit and attributes it to the flawed assumptions of text-based APR. These insights are obtained via a large-scale comprehensive benchmark and analysis of text data from five corpora with diverse characteristics and complementary personality models (Big Five and Dark Triad) applied to an assortment of AI methods ranging from hand-crafted linguistic features to data-driven transformers. Finally, the work concludes by identifying the open problems that can help navigate the limitations in text-based automatic personality recognition to a great extent.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142845874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face and voice identity matching accuracy is not improved by multimodal identity information. 多模态身份信息并不能提高人脸和声音身份匹配的准确性。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-17 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12757
Harriet M J Smith, Kay L Ritchie, Thom S Baguley, Nadine Lavan

Identity verification from both faces and voices can be error-prone. Previous research has shown that faces and voices signal concordant information and cross-modal unfamiliar face-to-voice matching is possible, albeit often with low accuracy. In the current study, we ask whether performance on a face or voice identity matching task can be improved by using multimodal stimuli which add a second modality (voice or face). We find that overall accuracy is higher for face matching than for voice matching. However, contrary to predictions, presenting one unimodal and one multimodal stimulus within a matching task did not improve face or voice matching compared to presenting two unimodal stimuli. Additionally, we find that presenting two multimodal stimuli does not improve accuracy compared to presenting two unimodal face stimuli. Thus, multimodal information does not improve accuracy. However, intriguingly, we find that cross-modal face-voice matching accuracy predicts voice matching accuracy but not face matching accuracy. This suggests cross-modal information can nonetheless play a role in identity matching, and face and voice information combine to inform matching decisions. We discuss our findings in light of current models of person perception, and consider the implications for identity verification in security and forensic settings.

通过面部和声音进行身份验证很容易出错。先前的研究表明,面孔和声音发出一致的信息,跨模态的不熟悉的声音与声音匹配是可能的,尽管通常精度较低。在当前的研究中,我们询问是否可以通过使用添加第二模态(声音或面孔)的多模态刺激来改善面部或声音身份匹配任务的表现。我们发现,人脸匹配的总体准确率高于语音匹配。然而,与预测相反,与呈现两个单峰刺激相比,在匹配任务中呈现一个单峰刺激和一个多峰刺激并没有改善面部或声音匹配。此外,我们发现呈现两个多模态刺激并不比呈现两个单模态面部刺激提高准确率。因此,多模态信息不能提高准确性。然而,有趣的是,我们发现跨模态人脸-语音匹配精度预测语音匹配精度,而不是人脸匹配精度。这表明,尽管如此,跨模态信息仍然可以在身份匹配中发挥作用,面部和语音信息结合起来,为匹配决策提供信息。我们根据当前的人感知模型讨论了我们的发现,并考虑了安全和法医环境中身份验证的含义。
{"title":"Face and voice identity matching accuracy is not improved by multimodal identity information.","authors":"Harriet M J Smith, Kay L Ritchie, Thom S Baguley, Nadine Lavan","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12757","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12757","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Identity verification from both faces and voices can be error-prone. Previous research has shown that faces and voices signal concordant information and cross-modal unfamiliar face-to-voice matching is possible, albeit often with low accuracy. In the current study, we ask whether performance on a face or voice identity matching task can be improved by using multimodal stimuli which add a second modality (voice or face). We find that overall accuracy is higher for face matching than for voice matching. However, contrary to predictions, presenting one unimodal and one multimodal stimulus within a matching task did not improve face or voice matching compared to presenting two unimodal stimuli. Additionally, we find that presenting two multimodal stimuli does not improve accuracy compared to presenting two unimodal face stimuli. Thus, multimodal information does not improve accuracy. However, intriguingly, we find that cross-modal face-voice matching accuracy predicts voice matching accuracy but not face matching accuracy. This suggests cross-modal information can nonetheless play a role in identity matching, and face and voice information combine to inform matching decisions. We discuss our findings in light of current models of person perception, and consider the implications for identity verification in security and forensic settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142845958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The token undermining effect: When and why adding a small reward to a dated outcome makes it less preferred. 象征性破坏效应:何时以及为什么在过时的结果中添加小奖励会使其不那么受欢迎。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12758
Cheng-Ming Jiang, Li-Na Chen, Qian Luo, Wen Wang, Jing Zhou, Jia-Tao Ma

The mere token strategy, which adds a small reward (token) to an option to increase attractiveness, is widely used in the consumer field. However, we conducted six studies that seek to confirm the 'token undermining effect', where adding a small token to a sooner and smaller reward (SS) paired with a later and larger reward (LL) decreases the preference for the SS. The results showed that the effect persists across various choice sets, participant populations, reward amounts, delays, outcome properties and regardless of whether the scenarios are incentivized. However, an important boundary condition was that the token must share the same nature as the original option. Furthermore, we used mouse cursor tracking methods to examine the underlying process of attention allocation and demonstrated that adding a small token to the SS leads individuals to allocate more attention to the magnitude dimension than to the delay dimension, ultimately decreasing their preference for the SS. Therefore, managers and policymakers should use the mere token strategy with caution as it could backfire.

单纯的代币策略,在一个选项中增加一个小的奖励(代币)来增加吸引力,被广泛应用于消费领域。然而,我们进行了六项研究,试图证实“代币破坏效应”,其中将小代币添加到更快更小的奖励(SS)中,再加上更晚更大的奖励(LL),会降低对SS的偏好。结果表明,这种效应持续存在于各种选择集、参与者群体、奖励金额、延迟、结果属性中,无论场景是否受到激励。然而,一个重要的边界条件是令牌必须与原始选项具有相同的性质。此外,我们使用鼠标光标跟踪方法来检查注意力分配的潜在过程,并证明向SS添加一个小令牌会导致个体将更多的注意力分配给量级维度而不是延迟维度,最终降低他们对SS的偏好。因此,管理者和决策者应谨慎使用仅仅是令牌策略,因为它可能适得其反。
{"title":"The token undermining effect: When and why adding a small reward to a dated outcome makes it less preferred.","authors":"Cheng-Ming Jiang, Li-Na Chen, Qian Luo, Wen Wang, Jing Zhou, Jia-Tao Ma","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12758","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12758","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The mere token strategy, which adds a small reward (token) to an option to increase attractiveness, is widely used in the consumer field. However, we conducted six studies that seek to confirm the 'token undermining effect', where adding a small token to a sooner and smaller reward (SS) paired with a later and larger reward (LL) decreases the preference for the SS. The results showed that the effect persists across various choice sets, participant populations, reward amounts, delays, outcome properties and regardless of whether the scenarios are incentivized. However, an important boundary condition was that the token must share the same nature as the original option. Furthermore, we used mouse cursor tracking methods to examine the underlying process of attention allocation and demonstrated that adding a small token to the SS leads individuals to allocate more attention to the magnitude dimension than to the delay dimension, ultimately decreasing their preference for the SS. Therefore, managers and policymakers should use the mere token strategy with caution as it could backfire.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142812191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mapping the maze: A network analysis of social–emotional skills among children and adolescents with social–emotional difficulties 绘制迷宫:有社交情绪困难的儿童和青少年社交情绪技能的网络分析。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-10 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12751
Ming Huo, Bo Ning

Developing social–emotional skills is crucial for all children and adolescents, particularly those experiencing social and emotional difficulties. This study used network analysis to identify the central skills and network association of different social–emotional skills and investigated how these networks differ between childhood and adolescence. Data were obtained from the 2019 Survey on Social and Emotional Skills by the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. Our study focused on the bottom quartile of participants aged 10 and 15 years, including 7737 and 7439 individuals from each age group. Optimism and cooperation consistently emerged as the central skills of social–emotional competence across both age groups. When comparing network structures, there was a significant difference between children and adolescents. The connectivity of social–emotional networks was stronger among adolescents, indicating closer skill associations. Understanding these developmental differences is important for educators and practitioners to more effectively support the social–emotional development of children and adolescents experiencing social–emotional difficulties.

发展社交情感技能对所有儿童和青少年都至关重要,尤其是那些经历社交和情感困难的儿童和青少年。本研究采用网络分析方法确定了不同社会情感技能的中心技能和网络关联,并探讨了这些网络在儿童期和青春期之间的差异。数据来自经济合作与发展组织2019年的社交和情感技能调查。我们的研究集中在10岁和15岁的最低四分之一的参与者,包括每个年龄组的7737和7439个人。乐观和合作一直是两个年龄组的社会情感能力的核心技能。当比较网络结构时,儿童和青少年之间存在显著差异。社会情感网络的连通性在青少年中更强,表明更紧密的技能关联。了解这些发展差异对于教育者和从业者更有效地支持经历社会情感困难的儿童和青少年的社会情感发展非常重要。
{"title":"Mapping the maze: A network analysis of social–emotional skills among children and adolescents with social–emotional difficulties","authors":"Ming Huo,&nbsp;Bo Ning","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12751","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12751","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Developing social–emotional skills is crucial for all children and adolescents, particularly those experiencing social and emotional difficulties. This study used network analysis to identify the central skills and network association of different social–emotional skills and investigated how these networks differ between childhood and adolescence. Data were obtained from the 2019 Survey on Social and Emotional Skills by the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. Our study focused on the bottom quartile of participants aged 10 and 15 years, including 7737 and 7439 individuals from each age group. Optimism and cooperation consistently emerged as the central skills of social–emotional competence across both age groups. When comparing network structures, there was a significant difference between children and adolescents. The connectivity of social–emotional networks was stronger among adolescents, indicating closer skill associations. Understanding these developmental differences is important for educators and practitioners to more effectively support the social–emotional development of children and adolescents experiencing social–emotional difficulties.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"233-249"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142799490","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1