首页 > 最新文献

British journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Listen to yourself! Prioritization of self-associated and own voice cues 倾听自己的声音优先考虑自我关联和自己的声音提示。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12741
Neil W. Kirk, Sheila J. Cunningham

Self-cues such as one's own name or face attract attention, reflecting a bias for stimuli connected to self to be prioritized in cognition. Recent evidence suggests that even external voices can elicit this self-prioritization effect; in a voice-label matching task, external voices assigned to the Self-identity label ‘you’ elicited faster responses than those assigned to ‘friend’ or ‘stranger’ (Payne et al., Br. J. Psychology, 112, 585-610). However, it is not clear whether external voices assigned to Self are prioritized over participants' own voices. We explore this issue in two experiments. In Exp 1 (N = 35), a voice-label matching task comprising three external voices confirmed that reaction time and accuracy are improved when an external voice cue is assigned to Self rather than Friend or Stranger. In Exp 2 (N = 90), one of the voice cues was replaced with a recording of the participant's own voice. Reaction time and accuracy showed a consistent advantage for the participant's own-voice, even when it was assigned to the ‘friend’ or ‘stranger’ identity. These findings show that external voices can elicit self-prioritization effects if associated with Self, but they are not prioritized above individuals' own voices. This has implications for external voice production technology, suggesting own-voice imitation may be beneficial.

自己的名字或面孔等自我线索会吸引注意力,这反映出在认知过程中与自我有关的刺激会被优先考虑。最近的证据表明,即使是外部声音也能引起这种自我优先效应;在一项声音标签匹配任务中,分配给自我身份标签 "你 "的外部声音比分配给 "朋友 "或 "陌生人 "的声音反应更快(Payne 等人,Br. J. Psychology, 112, 585-610)。然而,目前还不清楚分配给 "自我 "的外部声音是否优先于参与者自己的声音。我们在两个实验中探讨了这个问题。在实验 1(N = 35)中,一个由三个外部声音组成的声音标签匹配任务证实,当外部声音线索分配给 "自我 "而不是 "朋友 "或 "陌生人 "时,反应时间和准确性都会提高。在实验 2(N = 90)中,其中一个声音线索被替换为被试者自己的声音录音。结果表明,即使被试的声音被指定为 "朋友 "或 "陌生人 "身份,被试自己的声音在反应时间和准确性上也始终具有优势。这些研究结果表明,如果外部声音与 "自我 "相关联,就会产生自我优先效应,但其优先级不会高于个人自己的声音。这对外界声音制作技术有一定的影响,表明模仿自己的声音可能是有益的。
{"title":"Listen to yourself! Prioritization of self-associated and own voice cues","authors":"Neil W. Kirk,&nbsp;Sheila J. Cunningham","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12741","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12741","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Self-cues such as one's own name or face attract attention, reflecting a bias for stimuli connected to self to be prioritized in cognition. Recent evidence suggests that even external voices can elicit this self-prioritization effect; in a voice-label matching task, external voices assigned to the Self-identity label ‘you’ elicited faster responses than those assigned to ‘friend’ or ‘stranger’ (Payne et al., Br. J. Psychology, 112, 585-610). However, it is not clear whether external voices assigned to Self are prioritized over participants' own voices. We explore this issue in two experiments. In Exp 1 (<i>N</i> = 35), a voice-label matching task comprising three external voices confirmed that reaction time and accuracy are improved when an external voice cue is assigned to Self rather than Friend or Stranger. In Exp 2 (<i>N</i> = 90), one of the voice cues was replaced with a recording of the participant's own voice. Reaction time and accuracy showed a consistent advantage for the participant's own-voice, even when it was assigned to the ‘friend’ or ‘stranger’ identity. These findings show that external voices can elicit self-prioritization effects if associated with Self, but they are not prioritized above individuals' own voices. This has implications for external voice production technology, suggesting own-voice imitation may be beneficial.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"131-148"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11724686/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142370973","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial basis of stereotypes: Judgements of warmth and competence based on cross-group typicality/distinctiveness of faces 刻板印象的面部基础:基于面孔的跨群体典型性/独特性对温暖和能力的判断。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12738
S. Adil Saribay, Šimon Pokorný, Petr Tureček, Karel Kleisner

Human migration is an increasingly common phenomenon and migrants are at risk of disadvantageous treatment. We reasoned that migrants may receive differential treatment by locals based on the closeness of their facial features to the host average. Residents of Türkiye, the country with the largest number of refugees currently, served as participants. Because many of these refugees are of Arabic origin, we created target facial stimuli varying along the axis connecting Turkish and Arabic morphological prototypes (excluding skin colour) computed using geometric morphometrics and available databases. Participants made judgements of two universal dimensions of social perception–warmth and competence–on these faces. We predicted that participants judging faces manipulated towards the Turkish average would provide higher warmth and competence ratings compared to judging the same faces manipulated towards the Arabic average. Bayesian statistical tools were employed to estimate parameter values in multilevel models with intercorrelated varying effects. The findings did not support the prediction and revealed raters (as well as target faces) to be an important source of variation in social judgements. In the absence of simple cues (e.g. skin colour, group labels), the effect of facial morphology on social judgements may be much more complex than previously assumed.

人口迁移是一种日益普遍的现象,移民有可能受到不利待遇。我们推断,移民可能会根据其面部特征与东道国平均水平的接近程度而受到当地人的区别对待。目前难民人数最多的国家--土耳其的居民是研究的参与者。由于这些难民中有许多人来自阿拉伯,因此我们制作了目标面部刺激物,这些刺激物沿着连接土耳其和阿拉伯形态原型(不包括肤色)的轴线变化,这些原型是通过几何形态计量学和现有数据库计算得出的。受试者对这些面孔的两个普遍的社会感知维度--温暖和能力--进行判断。我们预测,与评判阿拉伯语平均值的相同面孔相比,评判土耳其语平均值的面孔的参与者会提供更高的温暖度和能力评级。我们使用贝叶斯统计工具来估计具有相互关联的不同效应的多层次模型的参数值。研究结果并不支持这一预测,并揭示了评分者(以及目标面孔)是社交判断差异的一个重要来源。在没有简单线索(如肤色、群体标签)的情况下,面部形态对社交判断的影响可能比之前假设的要复杂得多。
{"title":"Facial basis of stereotypes: Judgements of warmth and competence based on cross-group typicality/distinctiveness of faces","authors":"S. Adil Saribay,&nbsp;Šimon Pokorný,&nbsp;Petr Tureček,&nbsp;Karel Kleisner","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12738","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12738","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Human migration is an increasingly common phenomenon and migrants are at risk of disadvantageous treatment. We reasoned that migrants may receive differential treatment by locals based on the closeness of their facial features to the host average. Residents of Türkiye, the country with the largest number of refugees currently, served as participants. Because many of these refugees are of Arabic origin, we created target facial stimuli varying along the axis connecting Turkish and Arabic morphological prototypes (excluding skin colour) computed using geometric morphometrics and available databases. Participants made judgements of two universal dimensions of social perception–warmth and competence–on these faces. We predicted that participants judging faces manipulated towards the Turkish average would provide higher warmth and competence ratings compared to judging the same faces manipulated towards the Arabic average. Bayesian statistical tools were employed to estimate parameter values in multilevel models with intercorrelated varying effects. The findings did not support the prediction and revealed raters (as well as target faces) to be an important source of variation in social judgements. In the absence of simple cues (e.g. skin colour, group labels), the effect of facial morphology on social judgements may be much more complex than previously assumed.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"269-286"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11724689/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142342011","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who tends to be a perfectionistic adolescent? Distinguishing perfectionism from excellencism and investigating the links with the Big Five and self-esteem 哪些青少年倾向于完美主义?区分完美主义与优秀主义,研究完美主义与五大特征和自尊之间的联系。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12739
Kristina Bien, Jenny Wagner, Naemi D. Brandt

Striving towards perfection is an important concept of study, given its heterogenous associations with both positive and negative outcomes. To address this matter, recent work has emphasized the need to differentiate between striving towards perfection (perfectionism) and excellence (excellencism). However, the applicability of this differentiation in adolescence remains largely unexplored, despite this life phase being particularly sensitive for the development of perfectionism. To better understand striving towards perfection in adolescence, we examined the psychometric properties of the German Scale of Perfectionism and Excellencism (SCOPE) and evaluated the nomological net with the Big Five and self-esteem in 788 German adolescents (Mage = 15.49 years; 50% female). The results underscored the distinctiveness of the different strivings in adolescents but pointed to mixed evidence regarding convergent and discriminant validities. Notably, striving towards perfection was related to lower levels of openness and self-esteem but higher levels of neuroticism, whereas striving towards excellence was related to higher levels of every trait except neuroticism. Finally, most results remained consistent across genders and school types. We discuss how the differentiation between perfectionism and excellencism deepens our understanding of adolescents' perfectionistic strivings and how it might inform future research across different psychological fields.

追求完美是一个重要的研究概念,因为它与积极和消极结果都有不同的关联。为了解决这个问题,最近的研究强调了区分追求完美(完美主义)和追求卓越(卓越主义)的必要性。然而,这种区分在青少年时期的适用性在很大程度上仍未得到探讨,尽管这一人生阶段对完美主义的发展尤为敏感。为了更好地了解青少年时期对完美的追求,我们研究了德国完美主义和优秀主义量表(SCOPE)的心理测量学特性,并评估了 788 名德国青少年(年龄 = 15.49 岁;50% 为女性)与大五项和自尊之间的理论联系。结果表明,青少年的不同追求各具特色,但在收敛性和鉴别性方面证据不一。值得注意的是,追求完美与较低水平的开放性和自尊有关,但与较高水平的神经质有关,而追求卓越则与除神经质以外的所有特质的较高水平有关。最后,大多数结果在不同性别和学校类型之间保持一致。我们讨论了完美主义和卓越主义之间的区别如何加深了我们对青少年完美主义追求的理解,以及如何为未来不同心理学领域的研究提供参考。
{"title":"Who tends to be a perfectionistic adolescent? Distinguishing perfectionism from excellencism and investigating the links with the Big Five and self-esteem","authors":"Kristina Bien,&nbsp;Jenny Wagner,&nbsp;Naemi D. Brandt","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12739","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12739","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Striving towards perfection is an important concept of study, given its heterogenous associations with both positive and negative outcomes. To address this matter, recent work has emphasized the need to differentiate between striving towards perfection (perfectionism) and excellence (excellencism). However, the applicability of this differentiation in adolescence remains largely unexplored, despite this life phase being particularly sensitive for the development of perfectionism. To better understand striving towards perfection in adolescence, we examined the psychometric properties of the German Scale of Perfectionism and Excellencism (SCOPE) and evaluated the nomological net with the Big Five and self-esteem in 788 German adolescents (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 15.49 years; 50% female). The results underscored the distinctiveness of the different strivings in adolescents but pointed to mixed evidence regarding convergent and discriminant validities. Notably, striving towards perfection was related to lower levels of openness and self-esteem but higher levels of neuroticism, whereas striving towards excellence was related to higher levels of every trait except neuroticism. Finally, most results remained consistent across genders and school types. We discuss how the differentiation between perfectionism and excellencism deepens our understanding of adolescents' perfectionistic strivings and how it might inform future research across different psychological fields.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"108-130"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11724684/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142342012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Generative neural networks for experimental manipulation: Examining dominance-trustworthiness face impressions with data-efficient models. 用于实验操作的生成神经网络:用数据高效模型检验支配力-可信度面部印象。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12732
Adam Sobieszek, Maciej Siemiątkowski, Kamil K Imbir

An important development in the study of face impressions was the introduction of dominance and trustworthiness as the primary and potentially orthogonal traits judged from faces. We test competing predictions of recent accounts that address evidence against the independence of these judgements. To this end we develop a version of recent 'deep models of face impressions' better suited for data-efficient experimental manipulation. In Study 1 (N = 128) we build impression models using 15 times less ratings per dimension than previously assumed necessary. In Study 2 (N = 234) we show how our method can precisely manipulate dominance and trustworthiness impressions of face photographs and observe how the effects' pattern of the cues of one trait on impressions of the other differs from previous accounts. We propose an altered account that stresses how a successful execution of the two judgements' functional roles requires impressions of trustworthiness and dominance to be based on cues of both traits. Finally we show our manipulation resulted in larger effect sizes using a broader array of features than previous methods. Our approach lets researchers manipulate face stimuli for various face perception studies and investigate new dimensions with minimal data collection.

面孔印象研究的一个重要发展是引入了支配力和可信度作为从面孔中判断出的主要和潜在的正交特征。我们检验了最近一些观点的竞争性预测,这些观点针对这些判断的独立性提出了证据。为此,我们开发了一种新的 "人脸印象深度模型",这种模型更适合于数据效率高的实验操作。在研究 1(N = 128)中,我们建立了印象模型,每个维度使用的评分比以前假设的少 15 倍。在研究 2(N = 234)中,我们展示了我们的方法如何精确地操作人脸照片中的优势和可信度印象,并观察一种特质的线索对另一种特质印象的影响模式与之前的说法有何不同。我们提出了一种新的解释,强调要成功执行这两种判断的功能作用,就必须根据这两种特质的线索来建立可信度和优势度的印象。最后,我们表明,与以前的方法相比,我们的操作方法使用了更多的特征,从而产生了更大的效应量。我们的方法可以让研究人员在各种人脸感知研究中操纵人脸刺激,并以最少的数据收集来研究新的维度。
{"title":"Generative neural networks for experimental manipulation: Examining dominance-trustworthiness face impressions with data-efficient models.","authors":"Adam Sobieszek, Maciej Siemiątkowski, Kamil K Imbir","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12732","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12732","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An important development in the study of face impressions was the introduction of dominance and trustworthiness as the primary and potentially orthogonal traits judged from faces. We test competing predictions of recent accounts that address evidence against the independence of these judgements. To this end we develop a version of recent 'deep models of face impressions' better suited for data-efficient experimental manipulation. In Study 1 (N = 128) we build impression models using 15 times less ratings per dimension than previously assumed necessary. In Study 2 (N = 234) we show how our method can precisely manipulate dominance and trustworthiness impressions of face photographs and observe how the effects' pattern of the cues of one trait on impressions of the other differs from previous accounts. We propose an altered account that stresses how a successful execution of the two judgements' functional roles requires impressions of trustworthiness and dominance to be based on cues of both traits. Finally we show our manipulation resulted in larger effect sizes using a broader array of features than previous methods. Our approach lets researchers manipulate face stimuli for various face perception studies and investigate new dimensions with minimal data collection.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142280540","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders, complex post-traumatic stress disorder and emotionally unstable personality disorder: A Delphi study 自闭症、依恋障碍、复杂创伤后应激障碍和情绪不稳定型人格障碍的鉴别诊断:德尔菲研究
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12731
Rachel Sarr, Debbie Spain, Alice M. G. Quinton, Francesca Happé, Chris R. Brewin, Jonathan Radcliffe, Sally Jowett, Sarah Miles, Rafael A. González, Idit Albert, Alix Scholwin, Marguerite Stirling, Sarah Markham, Sally Strange, Freya Rumball

Individuals diagnosed with autism, attachment disorders, emotionally unstable personality disorder (EUPD) or complex post-traumatic stress disorder (CPTSD) can present with similar features. This renders differential and accurate diagnosis of these conditions difficult, leading to diagnostic overshadowing and misdiagnosis. The purpose of this study was to explore professionals' perspectives on the differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders and CPTSD in young people; and of autism, CPTSD and EUPD in adults. A co-produced three-round Delphi study gathered information through a series of questionnaires from 106 international professionals with expertise in assessing and/or diagnosing at least one of these conditions. To provide specialist guidance and data triangulation, working groups of experts by experience, clinicians and researchers were consulted. Delphi statements were considered to have reached consensus if at least 80% of participants were in agreement. Two hundred and seventy-five Delphi statements reached consensus. Overlapping and differentiating features, methods of assessment, difficulties encountered during differential diagnosis and suggestions for improvements were identified. The findings highlight current practices for differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders, CPTSD and EUPD in young people and adults. Areas for future research, clinical and service provision implications, were also identified.

被诊断出患有自闭症、依恋障碍、情绪不稳定型人格障碍(EUPD)或复杂创伤后应激障碍(CPTSD)的患者可能表现出相似的特征。这就给这些疾病的鉴别和准确诊断带来了困难,导致诊断上的阴影和误诊。本研究旨在探讨专业人士对青少年自闭症、依恋障碍和 CPTSD 以及成人自闭症、CPTSD 和 EUPD 的鉴别诊断的看法。一项共同编制的三轮德尔菲研究通过一系列调查问卷,从 106 名在评估和/或诊断至少一种自闭症方面具有专长的国际专业人士那里收集信息。为了提供专家指导和数据三角测量,还咨询了由经验丰富的专家、临床医生和研究人员组成的工作组。如果至少 80% 的参与者意见一致,德尔菲声明即被视为已达成共识。有 275 份德尔菲声明达成了共识。研究确定了重叠和区分特征、评估方法、鉴别诊断过程中遇到的困难以及改进建议。研究结果强调了目前对青少年和成年人的自闭症、依恋障碍、CPTSD 和 EUPD 进行鉴别诊断的做法。此外,还确定了今后的研究领域、临床和服务提供方面的影响。
{"title":"Differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders, complex post-traumatic stress disorder and emotionally unstable personality disorder: A Delphi study","authors":"Rachel Sarr,&nbsp;Debbie Spain,&nbsp;Alice M. G. Quinton,&nbsp;Francesca Happé,&nbsp;Chris R. Brewin,&nbsp;Jonathan Radcliffe,&nbsp;Sally Jowett,&nbsp;Sarah Miles,&nbsp;Rafael A. González,&nbsp;Idit Albert,&nbsp;Alix Scholwin,&nbsp;Marguerite Stirling,&nbsp;Sarah Markham,&nbsp;Sally Strange,&nbsp;Freya Rumball","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12731","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12731","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Individuals diagnosed with autism, attachment disorders, emotionally unstable personality disorder (EUPD) or complex post-traumatic stress disorder (CPTSD) can present with similar features. This renders differential and accurate diagnosis of these conditions difficult, leading to diagnostic overshadowing and misdiagnosis. The purpose of this study was to explore professionals' perspectives on the differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders and CPTSD in young people; and of autism, CPTSD and EUPD in adults. A co-produced three-round Delphi study gathered information through a series of questionnaires from 106 international professionals with expertise in assessing and/or diagnosing at least one of these conditions. To provide specialist guidance and data triangulation, working groups of experts by experience, clinicians and researchers were consulted. Delphi statements were considered to have reached consensus if at least 80% of participants were in agreement. Two hundred and seventy-five Delphi statements reached consensus. Overlapping and differentiating features, methods of assessment, difficulties encountered during differential diagnosis and suggestions for improvements were identified. The findings highlight current practices for differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders, CPTSD and EUPD in young people and adults. Areas for future research, clinical and service provision implications, were also identified.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"1-33"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12731","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142255379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial: Introducing the British Psychological Society journal's landmark special issue on the United Nation's Sustainability Goals. 社论:介绍英国心理学会期刊关于联合国可持续发展目标的标志性特刊。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12737
Katherine Berry
{"title":"Editorial: Introducing the British Psychological Society journal's landmark special issue on the United Nation's Sustainability Goals.","authors":"Katherine Berry","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12737","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12737","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142280539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the spatial interference effects elicited by social and non-social targets: A conditional accuracy function approach 探索社交目标和非社交目标引发的空间干扰效应:条件准确度函数法
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12735
Renato Ponce, Juan Lupiáñez, Carlos González-García, Maria Casagrande, Andrea Marotta

Recent studies employing the spatial interference paradigm reveal qualitative differences in congruency effects between gaze and arrow targets. Typically, arrows produce a standard congruency effect (SCE), with faster responses when target direction aligns with its location. Conversely, gaze targets often lead to a reversed congruency effect (RCE), where responses are slower in similar conditions. We explored this dissociation using the Conditional Accuracy Function (CAF) to assess accuracy across reaction time bins. Using a hierarchical linear mixed modelling approach to compare cropped eyes, and full faces as social stimuli, and arrows as non-social stimuli, we synthesized findings from 11 studies, which led to three distinct models. The results showed that with non-social targets, incongruent trials exhibited lower accuracy rates in the first bin than in subsequent bins, while congruent trials maintained stable accuracy throughout the distribution. Conversely, social targets revealed a dissociation within the fastest responses; alongside a general reduction in accuracy for both congruency conditions, congruent trials resulted in even lower accuracy rates than incongruent ones. These results suggest with gaze targets that additional information, perhaps social, in addition to the automatic capture by the irrelevant target location, is being processed during the earlier stages of processing.

最近采用空间干扰范式进行的研究显示,凝视目标和箭头目标之间的一致性效应存在质的差异。通常情况下,箭头会产生标准一致效应(SCE),当目标方向与其位置一致时,反应速度更快。相反,凝视目标通常会导致反向一致性效应(RCE),即在类似条件下反应较慢。我们使用条件准确度函数(CAF)来评估不同反应时间区间的准确度,从而探索这种分离现象。我们采用分层线性混合建模法,将裁剪过的眼睛和完整的面孔作为社交刺激物与箭头作为非社交刺激物进行比较,综合了 11 项研究的结果,得出了三种不同的模型。结果表明,对于非社会性目标,不一致的试验在第一个分区的准确率低于随后的分区,而一致的试验在整个分布中保持稳定的准确率。相反,社交目标则显示出最快反应中的不一致;在两种一致性条件下,除了准确率普遍降低外,一致性试验的准确率甚至低于不一致试验。这些结果表明,对于凝视目标,除了自动捕捉无关的目标位置外,在处理的早期阶段还需要处理额外的信息,也许是社会信息。
{"title":"Exploring the spatial interference effects elicited by social and non-social targets: A conditional accuracy function approach","authors":"Renato Ponce,&nbsp;Juan Lupiáñez,&nbsp;Carlos González-García,&nbsp;Maria Casagrande,&nbsp;Andrea Marotta","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12735","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12735","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent studies employing the spatial interference paradigm reveal qualitative differences in congruency effects between gaze and arrow targets. Typically, arrows produce a standard congruency effect (SCE), with faster responses when target direction aligns with its location. Conversely, gaze targets often lead to a reversed congruency effect (RCE), where responses are slower in similar conditions. We explored this dissociation using the Conditional Accuracy Function (CAF) to assess accuracy across reaction time bins. Using a hierarchical linear mixed modelling approach to compare cropped eyes, and full faces as social stimuli, and arrows as non-social stimuli, we synthesized findings from 11 studies, which led to three distinct models. The results showed that with non-social targets, incongruent trials exhibited lower accuracy rates in the first bin than in subsequent bins, while congruent trials maintained stable accuracy throughout the distribution. Conversely, social targets revealed a dissociation within the fastest responses; alongside a general reduction in accuracy for both congruency conditions, congruent trials resulted in even lower accuracy rates than incongruent ones. These results suggest with gaze targets that additional information, perhaps social, in addition to the automatic capture by the irrelevant target location, is being processed during the earlier stages of processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"69-88"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjop.12735","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142255380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unpacking interplays between competitiveness, cooperativeness, and social comparison orientation: A network psychometric approach and replication 解读竞争性、合作性和社会比较取向之间的相互作用:网络心理测量方法与复制
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12734
Guangteng Meng, Lingxiao Wang, Bowei Zhong, Shujing Wang, Qi Li, Xun Liu

Emerging evidence highlights the role of social comparison in competition and cooperation, yet the dynamics among competitiveness, cooperativeness, and social comparison orientation (SCO) remain underexplored. In the present study, we sought to unravel these complex interplays employing graphical Gaussian models (GGMs) and characterize the network structures utilizing cross-sectional data derived from 1073 Chinese participants. A pre-registered independent sample (n = 1348) served to examine the replicability and robustness of our initial findings. Moreover, we explored the Bayesian networks (i.e., directed acyclic graphs) to delve into their probabilistic directed dependencies. Our analysis identified an interpersonal co-opetition network where SCO bridges competitiveness and cooperativeness, emphasizing the key influence of the desire to surpass others. We also found a significant gender effect on the levels of these orientations but not on the network patterns. Additionally, the replication sample offered validation for the resultant network structure and its key nodes. In sum, these findings provide a new insight into the symbiotic relationship between competitiveness and cooperativeness within the framework of social comparison, offering significant theoretical and practical implications.

新出现的证据凸显了社会比较在竞争与合作中的作用,然而竞争力、合作性和社会比较取向(SCO)之间的动态关系仍未得到充分探讨。在本研究中,我们试图利用图形高斯模型(GGMs)来揭示这些复杂的相互作用,并利用来自 1073 名中国参与者的横截面数据来描述网络结构的特征。预先登记的独立样本(n = 1348)检验了我们初步发现的可复制性和稳健性。此外,我们还探索了贝叶斯网络(即有向无环图),以深入研究其概率有向依赖关系。我们的分析发现了一个人际合作竞争网络,在这个网络中,SCO 在竞争性与合作性之间架起了一座桥梁,强调了超越他人的愿望所产生的关键影响。我们还发现,性别对这些取向的水平有明显影响,但对网络模式没有影响。此外,复制样本还验证了由此产生的网络结构及其关键节点。总之,这些研究结果为在社会比较框架内研究竞争性与合作性之间的共生关系提供了新的视角,具有重要的理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Unpacking interplays between competitiveness, cooperativeness, and social comparison orientation: A network psychometric approach and replication","authors":"Guangteng Meng,&nbsp;Lingxiao Wang,&nbsp;Bowei Zhong,&nbsp;Shujing Wang,&nbsp;Qi Li,&nbsp;Xun Liu","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12734","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12734","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Emerging evidence highlights the role of social comparison in competition and cooperation, yet the dynamics among competitiveness, cooperativeness, and social comparison orientation (SCO) remain underexplored. In the present study, we sought to unravel these complex interplays employing graphical Gaussian models (GGMs) and characterize the network structures utilizing cross-sectional data derived from 1073 Chinese participants. A pre-registered independent sample (<i>n</i> = 1348) served to examine the replicability and robustness of our initial findings. Moreover, we explored the Bayesian networks (i.e., directed acyclic graphs) to delve into their probabilistic directed dependencies. Our analysis identified an interpersonal co-opetition network where SCO bridges competitiveness and cooperativeness, emphasizing the key influence of the desire to surpass others. We also found a significant gender effect on the levels of these orientations but not on the network patterns. Additionally, the replication sample offered validation for the resultant network structure and its key nodes. In sum, these findings provide a new insight into the symbiotic relationship between competitiveness and cooperativeness within the framework of social comparison, offering significant theoretical and practical implications.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"52-68"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Positive and negative touch differentially modulate metacognitive memory judgements for emotional stimuli 正面和负面触觉对情绪刺激的元认知记忆判断有不同的调节作用
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12733
Gianmarco Convertino, Jessica Talbot, Mara Stockner, Daniele Gatti, Michela Marchetti, Danilo Mitaritonna, Giuliana Mazzoni

Touch plays a crucial role in providing humans with information from the external environment and can be perceived by humans as positively or negatively valenced. It is well documented that touch can differentially influence social functions, but very little is known about how touch can modulate (meta)cognition. Utilizing a within-subject design, participants were exposed to (a) positive, (b) negative, and (c) no touch, alongside encoding of emotionally valenced (positive and negative) images. After a 20-minute delay, participants completed a Yes/No recognition task to investigate how touch influenced memory-related decision components (e.g. criterion, confidence). Results showed that, compared to the control condition, both positive and negative touch were associated with overall lower confidence ratings, a less liberal response bias and slower response times. Interestingly, for correct recognitions, only negative touch (vs. no touch) led to inappropriately lower confidence and slower response times while both confidence and response time remained unaltered in positive touch. Our findings provide the first evidence that positive and negative touch differentially interact with metacognitive memory-related decisions. Altered metacognitive judgements after being touched could have significant consequences in applied settings, such as situations of eyewitness testimony.

触觉在为人类提供来自外部环境的信息方面起着至关重要的作用,人类可以将触觉感知为积极或消极的情绪。触觉可以对社会功能产生不同的影响,这一点已经有很多文献记载,但人们对触觉如何调节(元)认知却知之甚少。采用受试者内设计,让受试者接触(a)积极、(b)消极和(c)无触摸,同时对有情绪价值(积极和消极)的图像进行编码。延迟20分钟后,参与者完成 "是/否 "识别任务,以研究触摸如何影响与记忆相关的决策成分(如标准、信心)。结果表明,与对照组相比,正面和负面触觉都与总体较低的信心评级、较宽松的反应偏差和较慢的反应时间有关。有趣的是,在正确识别方面,只有负面触摸(与无触摸相比)会导致不适当的信心降低和反应时间变慢,而在正面触摸中,信心和反应时间都保持不变。我们的研究结果首次证明了正性触摸和负性触摸会对元认知记忆相关决策产生不同程度的影响。被触摸后元认知判断的改变可能会在应用环境中产生重大影响,例如在目击证人作证的情况下。
{"title":"Positive and negative touch differentially modulate metacognitive memory judgements for emotional stimuli","authors":"Gianmarco Convertino,&nbsp;Jessica Talbot,&nbsp;Mara Stockner,&nbsp;Daniele Gatti,&nbsp;Michela Marchetti,&nbsp;Danilo Mitaritonna,&nbsp;Giuliana Mazzoni","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12733","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjop.12733","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Touch plays a crucial role in providing humans with information from the external environment and can be perceived by humans as positively or negatively valenced. It is well documented that touch can differentially influence social functions, but very little is known about how touch can modulate (meta)cognition. Utilizing a within-subject design, participants were exposed to (a) positive, (b) negative, and (c) no touch, alongside encoding of emotionally valenced (positive and negative) images. After a 20-minute delay, participants completed a Yes/No recognition task to investigate how touch influenced memory-related decision components (e.g. criterion, confidence). Results showed that, compared to the control condition, both positive and negative touch were associated with overall lower confidence ratings, a less liberal response bias and slower response times. Interestingly, for correct recognitions, only negative touch (vs. no touch) led to inappropriately lower confidence and slower response times while both confidence and response time remained unaltered in positive touch. Our findings provide the first evidence that positive and negative touch differentially interact with metacognitive memory-related decisions. Altered metacognitive judgements after being touched could have significant consequences in applied settings, such as situations of eyewitness testimony.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"34-51"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People have different expectations for their own versus others' use of AI-mediated communication tools. 人们对自己和他人使用以人工智能为媒介的通信工具有着不同的期望。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12727
Zoe A Purcell, Mengchen Dong, Anne-Marie Nussberger, Nils Köbis, Maurice Jakesch

Artificial intelligence (AI) can enhance human communication, for example, by improving the quality of our writing, voice or appearance. However, AI mediated communication also has risks-it may increase deception, compromise authenticity or yield widespread mistrust. As a result, both policymakers and technology firms are developing approaches to prevent and reduce potentially unacceptable uses of AI communication technologies. However, we do not yet know what people believe is acceptable or what their expectations are regarding usage. Drawing on normative psychology theories, we examine people's judgements of the acceptability of open and secret AI use, as well as people's expectations of their own and others' use. In two studies with representative samples (Study 1: N = 477; Study 2: N = 765), we find that people are less accepting of secret than open AI use in communication, but only when directly compared. Our results also suggest that people believe others will use AI communication tools more than they would themselves and that people do not expect others' use to align with their expectations of what is acceptable. While much attention has been focused on transparency measures, our results suggest that self-other differences are a central factor for understanding people's attitudes and expectations for AI-mediated communication.

人工智能(AI)可以提高人类交流的质量,例如,提高我们的写作、语音或外表的质量。然而,以人工智能为媒介的交流也有风险--它可能会增加欺骗、损害真实性或产生广泛的不信任。因此,政策制定者和技术公司都在开发各种方法,以防止和减少人工智能通信技术潜在的不可接受的用途。然而,我们还不知道人们认为什么是可接受的,也不知道他们对使用的期望是什么。借鉴规范心理学理论,我们研究了人们对公开和秘密使用人工智能的可接受性的判断,以及人们对自己和他人使用人工智能的期望。在两项具有代表性的样本研究(研究 1:N = 477;研究 2:N = 765)中,我们发现人们在交流中对秘密使用人工智能的接受程度低于对公开使用人工智能的接受程度,但仅限于直接比较时。我们的研究结果还表明,人们相信他人会比自己更多使用人工智能交流工具,而且人们并不期望他人的使用符合他们对可接受程度的期望。虽然人们的注意力主要集中在透明度测量上,但我们的结果表明,自我与他人的差异是理解人们对以人工智能为媒介的交流的态度和期望的核心因素。
{"title":"People have different expectations for their own versus others' use of AI-mediated communication tools.","authors":"Zoe A Purcell, Mengchen Dong, Anne-Marie Nussberger, Nils Köbis, Maurice Jakesch","doi":"10.1111/bjop.12727","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12727","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Artificial intelligence (AI) can enhance human communication, for example, by improving the quality of our writing, voice or appearance. However, AI mediated communication also has risks-it may increase deception, compromise authenticity or yield widespread mistrust. As a result, both policymakers and technology firms are developing approaches to prevent and reduce potentially unacceptable uses of AI communication technologies. However, we do not yet know what people believe is acceptable or what their expectations are regarding usage. Drawing on normative psychology theories, we examine people's judgements of the acceptability of open and secret AI use, as well as people's expectations of their own and others' use. In two studies with representative samples (Study 1: N = 477; Study 2: N = 765), we find that people are less accepting of secret than open AI use in communication, but only when directly compared. Our results also suggest that people believe others will use AI communication tools more than they would themselves and that people do not expect others' use to align with their expectations of what is acceptable. While much attention has been focused on transparency measures, our results suggest that self-other differences are a central factor for understanding people's attitudes and expectations for AI-mediated communication.</p>","PeriodicalId":9300,"journal":{"name":"British journal of psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142124883","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1