首页 > 最新文献

Child development最新文献

英文 中文
“She's so pretty”: The development of valuing personal attractiveness among young children "她真漂亮幼儿重视个人魅力的发展过程
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14104
May Ling D. Halim, Lyric N. Russo, Kaelyn N. Echave, Sachiko Tawa, Dylan J. Sakamoto, Miguel A. Portillo

The current study sought to understand gender differences in how much children value personal attractiveness, whether age is associated with valuing personal attractiveness, and the role of gender identity development. Three- to five-year-olds (N = 170; 89 girls, 81 boys, 0 other genders; primarily Latiné, multiethnic, and non-Hispanic White American) were recruited from child centers across the Los Angeles and Orange County metropolitan areas. Across several indicators (e.g., self-report, preference for appearance-related female-typed occupations and fancy gender-typed outfits, memory for fancy gender-typed clothing, and spontaneous reasons for liking a media character), girls highly valued personal attractiveness. Girls also valued personal attractiveness and tied their gender to personal attractiveness to a greater extent than boys. We discuss implications for later well-being and health.

本研究旨在了解儿童在重视个人吸引力方面的性别差异、年龄是否与重视个人吸引力有关以及性别认同发展的作用。研究人员从洛杉矶和橘郡都会区的儿童中心招募了三到五岁的儿童(人数=170;89 名女孩,81 名男孩,0 名其他性别儿童;主要为拉丁裔、多种族和非西班牙裔美国白人)。从几项指标(如自我报告、对与外表相关的女性职业和花哨的性别服装的偏好、对花哨的性别服装的记忆、喜欢媒体角色的自发原因)来看,女孩非常重视个人吸引力。与男孩相比,女孩也更看重个人魅力,并在更大程度上将自己的性别与个人魅力联系在一起。我们讨论了这对日后幸福和健康的影响。
{"title":"“She's so pretty”: The development of valuing personal attractiveness among young children","authors":"May Ling D. Halim,&nbsp;Lyric N. Russo,&nbsp;Kaelyn N. Echave,&nbsp;Sachiko Tawa,&nbsp;Dylan J. Sakamoto,&nbsp;Miguel A. Portillo","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14104","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14104","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study sought to understand gender differences in how much children value personal attractiveness, whether age is associated with valuing personal attractiveness, and the role of gender identity development. Three- to five-year-olds (<i>N</i> = 170; 89 girls, 81 boys, 0 other genders; primarily Latiné, multiethnic, and non-Hispanic White American) were recruited from child centers across the Los Angeles and Orange County metropolitan areas. Across several indicators (e.g., self-report, preference for appearance-related female-typed occupations and fancy gender-typed outfits, memory for fancy gender-typed clothing, and spontaneous reasons for liking a media character), girls highly valued personal attractiveness. Girls also valued personal attractiveness and tied their gender to personal attractiveness to a greater extent than boys. We discuss implications for later well-being and health.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140651854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal depression, parenting, and child psychological outcomes in the context of maternal pain 产妇疼痛背景下的产妇抑郁、养育子女和儿童心理结果
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14106
Jacqueline R. O'Brien, Angela H. Lee, Amanda L. Stone, Nathan F. Dieckmann, Maureen Zalewski, Anna C. Wilson

Parental chronic pain is associated with adverse outcomes in children, but the mechanisms of transmission are largely untested. Mothers with chronic pain (N = 400, Mage = 40.3 years, 90.5% White) and their children (Mage = 10.33 years, 83.3% White, 50.2% female) were recruited in 2016–2018 to test longitudinal pathways of risk transmission from maternal chronic pain to children's psychological symptoms, examining roles of parenting, maternal depression, and child distress tolerance. Maternal pain was associated with positive (β = .28) and pain-specific (β = .10) parenting behaviors. Maternal depression was associated with lower child distress tolerance (β = −.03), which was associated with greater child psychological symptoms (β = −.62). Parenting and maternal pain were not prospectively associated with child outcomes. When considering the dual-generational impacts of chronic pain, physical and psychological functioning should be examined.

父母的慢性疼痛与儿童的不良后果有关,但其传播机制在很大程度上尚未得到验证。研究人员于2016-2018年招募了患有慢性疼痛的母亲(人数=400,年龄=40.3岁,90.5%为白人)及其子女(年龄=10.33岁,83.3%为白人,50.2%为女性),以测试从母亲慢性疼痛到子女心理症状的纵向风险传播途径,考察养育子女、母亲抑郁和儿童痛苦耐受性的作用。母亲疼痛与积极的(β = .28)和特定于疼痛的(β = .10)养育行为相关。母亲抑郁与儿童较低的痛苦容忍度(β = -.03)相关,而儿童较低的痛苦容忍度与儿童较高的心理症状(β = -.62)相关。养育子女和母亲的痛苦与儿童的结果没有前瞻性关联。在考虑慢性疼痛对两代人的影响时,应检查生理和心理功能。
{"title":"Maternal depression, parenting, and child psychological outcomes in the context of maternal pain","authors":"Jacqueline R. O'Brien,&nbsp;Angela H. Lee,&nbsp;Amanda L. Stone,&nbsp;Nathan F. Dieckmann,&nbsp;Maureen Zalewski,&nbsp;Anna C. Wilson","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14106","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14106","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Parental chronic pain is associated with adverse outcomes in children, but the mechanisms of transmission are largely untested. Mothers with chronic pain (<i>N</i> = 400, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 40.3 years, 90.5% White) and their children (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 10.33 years, 83.3% White, 50.2% female) were recruited in 2016–2018 to test longitudinal pathways of risk transmission from maternal chronic pain to children's psychological symptoms, examining roles of parenting, maternal depression, and child distress tolerance. Maternal pain was associated with positive (<i>β</i> = .28) and pain-specific (<i>β</i> = .10) parenting behaviors. Maternal depression was associated with lower child distress tolerance (<i>β</i> = −.03), which was associated with greater child psychological symptoms (<i>β</i> = −.62). Parenting and maternal pain were not prospectively associated with child outcomes. When considering the dual-generational impacts of chronic pain, physical and psychological functioning should be examined.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140651828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Leveraging an intensive time series of young children's movement to capture impulsive and inattentive behaviors in a preschool setting 利用幼儿运动的密集时间序列捕捉学前环境中的冲动和注意力不集中行为
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14100
Andrew E. Koepp, Elizabeth T. Gershoff

Studying within-person variability in children's behavior is frequently hindered by challenges collecting repeated observations. This study used wearable accelerometers to collect an intensive time series (2.7 million observations) of young children's movement at school (N = 62, Mage = 4.5 years, 54% male, 74% Non-Hispanic White) in 2021. Machine learning analyses indicated that children's typical forward acceleration was strongly correlated with lower teacher-reported inhibitory control and attention (r = −.69). Using forward movement intensity as a proxy for impulsivity, we partitioned the intensive time series and found that (1) children modulated their behavior across periods of the school day, (2) children's impulsivity increased across the school week, and (3) children with greater impulsivity showed greater variability in behavior across days.

研究儿童行为的人内变异性经常会受到重复观察数据收集工作的阻碍。本研究使用可穿戴加速度计收集了 2021 年幼儿在校运动的密集时间序列(270 万次观察)(N = 62,年龄 = 4.5 岁,54% 为男性,74% 为非西班牙裔白人)。机器学习分析表明,儿童的典型前向加速度与教师报告的较低抑制控制和注意力密切相关(r = -.69)。我们使用前向运动强度作为冲动性的替代指标,对密集时间序列进行了分区,结果发现:(1)儿童在上学日的不同时段对自己的行为进行了调节;(2)儿童的冲动性在整个上学周内都在增加;(3)冲动性较强的儿童在不同天内的行为变化较大。
{"title":"Leveraging an intensive time series of young children's movement to capture impulsive and inattentive behaviors in a preschool setting","authors":"Andrew E. Koepp,&nbsp;Elizabeth T. Gershoff","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14100","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14100","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studying within-person variability in children's behavior is frequently hindered by challenges collecting repeated observations. This study used wearable accelerometers to collect an intensive time series (2.7 million observations) of young children's movement at school (<i>N</i> = 62, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 4.5 years, 54% male, 74% Non-Hispanic White) in 2021. Machine learning analyses indicated that children's typical forward acceleration was strongly correlated with lower teacher-reported inhibitory control and attention (<i>r</i> = −.69). Using forward movement intensity as a proxy for impulsivity, we partitioned the intensive time series and found that (1) children modulated their behavior across periods of the school day, (2) children's impulsivity increased across the school week, and (3) children with greater impulsivity showed greater variability in behavior across days.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14100","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140643001","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How retributive motives shape the emergence of third-party punishment across intergroup contexts 报复性动机如何影响第三方惩罚在不同群体间的出现
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14097
Julia Marshall, Katherine McAuliffe

This study examines how retributive motives—the desire to punish for the purpose of inflicting harm in the absence of future benefits—shape third-party punishment behavior across intergroup contexts. Six- to nine-year-olds (N = 151, Mage = 8.00, SDage = 1.15; 54% White, 18% mixed ethnicities, 17% Asian American; 46% female; from the USA) could punish ingroup, outgroup, or non-group transgressors by removing positive resources and allocating negative ones. Both punishments were described as retributive, yet allocating negative resources was perceived as more retributive than removing positive ones. We predicted that children would punish outgroups more so than ingroups and that this effect would be especially pronounced when punishment is perceived as particularly retributive. The results did not align with this prediction; instead, children similarly punished all agents.

本研究探讨了报应动机--在没有未来利益的情况下为造成伤害而进行惩罚的欲望--如何在不同群体间形成第三方惩罚行为。来自美国的六到九岁儿童(N = 151,Mage = 8.00,SDage = 1.15;54%为白人,18%为混血儿,17%为亚裔美国人;46%为女性)可以通过剥夺积极资源和分配消极资源的方式来惩罚本群体、外群体或非本群体的违规者。这两种惩罚都被描述为报复性的,但分配消极资源比剥夺积极资源更具有报复性。我们预测,儿童对外部群体的惩罚会比对内部群体的惩罚更强烈,而且当惩罚被认为具有特别的报复性时,这种效应会尤为明显。结果与这一预测不符;相反,儿童对所有代理人的惩罚都差不多。
{"title":"How retributive motives shape the emergence of third-party punishment across intergroup contexts","authors":"Julia Marshall,&nbsp;Katherine McAuliffe","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14097","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14097","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examines how retributive motives—the desire to punish for the purpose of inflicting harm in the absence of future benefits—shape third-party punishment behavior across intergroup contexts. Six- to nine-year-olds (<i>N</i> = 151, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 8.00, SD<sub>age</sub> = 1.15; 54% White, 18% mixed ethnicities, 17% Asian American; 46% female; from the USA) could punish ingroup, outgroup, or non-group transgressors by removing positive resources and allocating negative ones. Both punishments were described as retributive, yet allocating negative resources was perceived as more retributive than removing positive ones. We predicted that children would punish outgroups more so than ingroups and that this effect would be especially pronounced when punishment is perceived as particularly retributive. The results did not align with this prediction; instead, children similarly punished all agents.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14097","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140551918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trajectories of digital flourishing in adolescence: The predictive roles of developmental changes and digital divide factors 青少年数字化发展的轨迹:发展变化和数字鸿沟因素的预测作用
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14101
Jasmina Rosič, Lara Schreurs, Sophie H. Janicke-Bowles, Laura Vandenbosch

Digital flourishing refers to the positive perceptions of digital communication use in five dimensions: connectedness, positive social comparison, authentic self-presentation, civil participation, and self-control. This three-wave panel study among 1081 Slovenian adolescents (Mage = 15.34 years, 53.8% boys, 80.7% ethnic majority) explored the trajectories of their digital flourishing dimensions over 1 year (2021–2022). Latent class growth analysis identified two classes. Adolescents in the first class reported high levels of digital flourishing, which remained stable over time, whereas those in the second class reported low levels of digital flourishing with decreased self-control over time. Autonomy-supportive restrictive, autonomy-supportive active, and controlling active parental mediation styles, together with high parental digital skills, predicted adolescents' belongingness to the (more digitally flourishing) first class.

数字繁荣指的是在以下五个方面对数字通信使用的积极看法:联系性、积极的社会比较、真实的自我展示、公民参与和自我控制。这项三波小组研究对 1081 名斯洛文尼亚青少年(年龄 = 15.34 岁,53.8% 为男孩,80.7% 为多数民族)进行了调查,探讨了他们在一年(2021-2022 年)内数字繁荣维度的发展轨迹。潜类增长分析确定了两个等级。第一类青少年的数字繁荣程度较高,且随着时间的推移保持稳定;而第二类青少年的数字繁荣程度较低,且随着时间的推移自我控制能力下降。父母的自主支持限制型、自主支持主动型和控制主动型调解风格,以及父母的高数字技能,都预示着青少年属于(数字繁荣程度较高的)第一类。
{"title":"Trajectories of digital flourishing in adolescence: The predictive roles of developmental changes and digital divide factors","authors":"Jasmina Rosič,&nbsp;Lara Schreurs,&nbsp;Sophie H. Janicke-Bowles,&nbsp;Laura Vandenbosch","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14101","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14101","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Digital flourishing refers to the positive perceptions of digital communication use in five dimensions: connectedness, positive social comparison, authentic self-presentation, civil participation, and self-control. This three-wave panel study among 1081 Slovenian adolescents (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 15.34 years, 53.8% boys, 80.7% ethnic majority) explored the trajectories of their digital flourishing dimensions over 1 year (2021–2022). Latent class growth analysis identified two classes. Adolescents in the first class reported high levels of digital flourishing, which remained stable over time, whereas those in the second class reported low levels of digital flourishing with decreased self-control over time. Autonomy-supportive restrictive, autonomy-supportive active, and controlling active parental mediation styles, together with high parental digital skills, predicted adolescents' belongingness to the (more digitally flourishing) first class.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140551920","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
This is me! Neural correlates of self-recognition in 6- to 8-month-old infants 这就是我6 至 8 个月大婴儿自我认知的神经相关性
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14102
Silvia Rigato, Rita De Sepulveda, Eleanor Richardson, Maria Laura Filippetti

Historically, evidence of self-recognition in development has been associated with the “rouge test”; however, this has been often criticized for providing a reductionist picture of self-conscious behavior. With two event-related potential (ERP) experiments, this study investigated the origin of self-recognition. Six- to eight-month-old infants (42 males and 35 females, predominately White, tested in the UK in 2022–2023) were presented with images of their face, another peer's face, and their mother's face (N = 38, Exp.1), and images of their own face morphed into another peer's face (N = 39, Exp.2). Results showed an enhanced P100 in infants' ERP response to their own face compared to others' faces (Exp.1 only), suggesting the presence of an enhanced attentional mechanism to one own's face as early as 6 months.

一直以来,发育过程中自我认知的证据都与 "胭脂测试 "有关;然而,这种测试常常被批评为提供了自我意识行为的还原图景。本研究通过两个事件相关电位(ERP)实验,对自我认知的起源进行了研究。研究人员向六到八个月大的婴儿(42 名男性和 35 名女性,主要为白人,2022-2023 年在英国接受测试)展示了他们自己的脸、另一个同伴的脸和他们母亲的脸(N = 38,Exp.1),以及他们自己的脸变形为另一个同伴的脸的图像(N = 39,Exp.2)。结果显示,与其他人的脸相比,婴儿对自己的脸的ERP反应中的P100增强了(仅实验1),这表明早在6个月大时,婴儿对自己的脸的注意机制就增强了。
{"title":"This is me! Neural correlates of self-recognition in 6- to 8-month-old infants","authors":"Silvia Rigato,&nbsp;Rita De Sepulveda,&nbsp;Eleanor Richardson,&nbsp;Maria Laura Filippetti","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14102","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14102","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Historically, evidence of self-recognition in development has been associated with the “rouge test”; however, this has been often criticized for providing a reductionist picture of self-conscious behavior. With two event-related potential (ERP) experiments, this study investigated the origin of self-recognition. Six- to eight-month-old infants (42 males and 35 females, predominately White, tested in the UK in 2022–2023) were presented with images of their face, another peer's face, and their mother's face (<i>N</i> = 38, Exp.1), and images of their own face morphed into another peer's face (<i>N</i> = 39, Exp.2). Results showed an enhanced P100 in infants' ERP response to their own face compared to others' faces (Exp.1 only), suggesting the presence of an enhanced attentional mechanism to one own's face as early as 6 months.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14102","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140551930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotions or cognitions first? Longitudinal relations between executive functions and emotion regulation in childhood 先有情绪还是先有认知?儿童时期执行功能与情绪调节之间的纵向关系
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14096
Marte Halse, Silje Steinsbekk, Oda Bjørklund, Åsa Hammar, Lars Wichstrøm

Executive functions and emotion regulation develop from early childhood to adolescence and are predictive of important psychosocial outcomes. However, despite the correlation between the two regulatory capacities, whether they are prospectively related in school-aged children remains unknown, and the direction of effects is uncertain. In this study, a sample drawn from two birth cohorts in Norway was biennially examined between the ages of 6 and 14 (n = 852, 50.1% girls, 93% Norwegian). Parents completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist, and teachers completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. A random intercept cross-lagged panel model revealed that improved emotion regulation predicted increased executive functioning to the same extent throughout development, whereas enhanced executive functioning was unrelated to future changes in emotion regulation.

执行功能和情绪调节能力是从幼儿期发展到青春期的,对重要的社会心理结果具有预测作用。然而,尽管这两种调节能力之间存在相关性,但它们在学龄儿童中是否存在前瞻性关联仍是未知数,其影响方向也不确定。在这项研究中,我们每两年对挪威两个出生组群中6至14岁的儿童(n = 852,50.1%为女孩,93%为挪威人)进行一次抽样调查。家长填写了情绪调节核对表,教师填写了执行功能行为评级量表。随机截距交叉滞后面板模型显示,情绪调节能力的提高在整个成长过程中对执行功能提高的预测程度相同,而执行功能的提高与情绪调节能力的未来变化无关。
{"title":"Emotions or cognitions first? Longitudinal relations between executive functions and emotion regulation in childhood","authors":"Marte Halse,&nbsp;Silje Steinsbekk,&nbsp;Oda Bjørklund,&nbsp;Åsa Hammar,&nbsp;Lars Wichstrøm","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14096","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14096","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Executive functions and emotion regulation develop from early childhood to adolescence and are predictive of important psychosocial outcomes. However, despite the correlation between the two regulatory capacities, whether they are prospectively related in school-aged children remains unknown, and the direction of effects is uncertain. In this study, a sample drawn from two birth cohorts in Norway was biennially examined between the ages of 6 and 14 (<i>n</i> = 852, 50.1% girls, 93% Norwegian). Parents completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist, and teachers completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. A random intercept cross-lagged panel model revealed that improved emotion regulation predicted increased executive functioning to the same extent throughout development, whereas enhanced executive functioning was unrelated to future changes in emotion regulation.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14096","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140542124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disagreement reduces overconfidence and prompts exploration in young children 意见分歧会降低幼儿的过度自信,并促使他们进行探索
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14098
Antonia F. Langenhoff, Mahesh Srinivasan, Jan M. Engelmann

Can the experience of disagreement lead young children to reason in more sophisticated ways? Across two preregistered studies, four- to six-year-old US children (N = 136, 50% female, mixed ethnicities, data collected 2020–2022) experienced either a disagreement or an agreement with a confederate about a causal mechanism after being presented with ambiguous evidence. We measured (1) children's confidence in their belief before and after the (dis)agreement, and (2) how long children searched for information about the correct answer. Disagreement, especially with an expert (Experiment 2), reduced overconfidence and prompted children to search longer for information, compared to agreement. Together, our findings suggest possibilities for interventions aimed at fostering humility and learning across the lifespan.

不同意见的经历能否引导幼儿以更复杂的方式进行推理?在两项预先登记的研究中,四到六岁的美国儿童(N = 136,50% 为女性,种族混杂,数据收集于 2020-2022 年)在获得模棱两可的证据后,与同伴就因果机制产生了分歧或达成了一致。我们测量了(1)儿童在(不)同意之前和之后对其信念的信心,以及(2)儿童搜索正确答案信息的时间。与同意相比,不同意,尤其是与专家的不同意(实验 2),会降低过度自信,并促使儿童花更长时间搜索信息。总之,我们的研究结果表明,我们有可能采取干预措施,在儿童的一生中培养他们的谦逊和学习能力。
{"title":"Disagreement reduces overconfidence and prompts exploration in young children","authors":"Antonia F. Langenhoff,&nbsp;Mahesh Srinivasan,&nbsp;Jan M. Engelmann","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14098","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14098","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Can the experience of disagreement lead young children to reason in more sophisticated ways? Across two preregistered studies, four- to six-year-old US children (<i>N</i> = 136, 50% female, mixed ethnicities, data collected 2020–2022) experienced either a disagreement or an agreement with a confederate about a causal mechanism after being presented with ambiguous evidence. We measured (1) children's confidence in their belief before and after the (dis)agreement, and (2) how long children searched for information about the correct answer. Disagreement, especially with an expert (Experiment 2), reduced overconfidence and prompted children to search longer for information, compared to agreement. Together, our findings suggest possibilities for interventions aimed at fostering humility and learning across the lifespan.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14098","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140538939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
She made it with her friend: How social object history influences children's thinking about the value of digital objects 这是她和朋友一起制作的:社会物品史如何影响儿童对数字物品价值的思考。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14093
Keiana Price, Jasmine M. DeJesus, Shaylene E. Nancekivell

Two studies examine how social object histories from collaborative experiences influenced North American children (N = 160, 5–10 years) thinking about the value of digital objects (48% male/51% female; 51% White/24% Black/11% Asian). With forced-choice judgments, Study 1 found (moderate–large effects) that children viewed digital and physical objects with social histories as more special than objects without such histories. On a 10-point scale, Study 2 found (large effects) that children rated digital objects with positive social histories as more special than objects with negative ones. Overall, the studies found that children's tendencies to use object history to understand object value extends into digital contexts. They also reveal how an unexplored kind of history—social history—affects judgments.

两项研究考察了合作经历中的社会物品历史如何影响北美儿童(160 人,5-10 岁)对数字物品价值的思考(48% 为男性/51% 为女性;51% 为白人/24% 为黑人/11% 为亚裔)。通过强制选择判断,研究 1 发现(中-大影响),儿童认为有社会历史的数字物品和实物比没有社会历史的物品更特别。在 10 分制中,研究 2 发现(较大影响),儿童认为有正面社会历史的数字物品比没有负面社会历史的物品更特别。总之,这些研究发现,儿童利用物品历史来理解物品价值的倾向在数字环境中也有体现。这些研究还揭示了一种未被探索的历史--社会历史--是如何影响判断的。
{"title":"She made it with her friend: How social object history influences children's thinking about the value of digital objects","authors":"Keiana Price,&nbsp;Jasmine M. DeJesus,&nbsp;Shaylene E. Nancekivell","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14093","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14093","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Two studies examine how social object histories from collaborative experiences influenced North American children (<i>N</i> = 160, 5–10 years) thinking about the value of digital objects (48% male/51% female; 51% White/24% Black/11% Asian). With forced-choice judgments, Study 1 found (moderate–large effects) that children viewed digital and physical objects with social histories as more special than objects without such histories. On a 10-point scale, Study 2 found (large effects) that children rated digital objects with positive social histories as more special than objects with negative ones. Overall, the studies found that children's tendencies to use object history to understand object value extends into digital contexts. They also reveal how an unexplored kind of history—social history—affects judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14093","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334875","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language development beyond the here-and-now: Iconicity and displacement in child-directed communication 超越此时此地的语言发展:儿童自主交流中的象征性和移位。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14099
Yasamin Motamedi, Margherita Murgiano, Beata Grzyb, Yan Gu, Viktor Kewenig, Ricarda Brieke, Ed Donnellan, Chloe Marshall, Elizabeth Wonnacott, Pamela Perniss, Gabriella Vigliocco

Most language use is displaced, referring to past, future, or hypothetical events, posing the challenge of how children learn what words refer to when the referent is not physically available. One possibility is that iconic cues that imagistically evoke properties of absent referents support learning when referents are displaced. In an audio-visual corpus of caregiver–child dyads, English-speaking caregivers interacted with their children (N = 71, 24–58 months) in contexts in which the objects talked about were either familiar or unfamiliar to the child, and either physically present or displaced. The analysis of the range of vocal, manual, and looking behaviors caregivers produced suggests that caregivers used iconic cues especially in displaced contexts and for unfamiliar objects, using other cues when objects were present.

大多数语言的使用都是指代过去、未来或假设的事件,这就给儿童带来了一个挑战,即当指代对象不存在时,儿童如何学习词语的指代对象。一种可能性是,当指代物被移位时,通过想象唤起不在场指代物属性的标志性线索有助于学习。在照顾者-儿童二人组的视听语料库中,讲英语的照顾者与他们的孩子(人数=71,24-58 个月)在孩子熟悉或不熟悉的情境中互动,所谈论的对象要么是实际存在的,要么是移位的。对照料者所做的一系列发声、手动和注视行为的分析表明,照料者尤其在移位情境中和对不熟悉的物体使用标志性线索,而在物体存在时则使用其他线索。
{"title":"Language development beyond the here-and-now: Iconicity and displacement in child-directed communication","authors":"Yasamin Motamedi,&nbsp;Margherita Murgiano,&nbsp;Beata Grzyb,&nbsp;Yan Gu,&nbsp;Viktor Kewenig,&nbsp;Ricarda Brieke,&nbsp;Ed Donnellan,&nbsp;Chloe Marshall,&nbsp;Elizabeth Wonnacott,&nbsp;Pamela Perniss,&nbsp;Gabriella Vigliocco","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14099","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14099","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Most language use is displaced, referring to past, future, or hypothetical events, posing the challenge of how children learn what words refer to when the referent is not physically available. One possibility is that iconic cues that imagistically evoke properties of absent referents support learning when referents are displaced. In an audio-visual corpus of caregiver–child dyads, English-speaking caregivers interacted with their children (<i>N</i> = 71, 24–58 months) in contexts in which the objects talked about were either familiar or unfamiliar to the child, and either physically present or displaced. The analysis of the range of vocal, manual, and looking behaviors caregivers produced suggests that caregivers used iconic cues especially in displaced contexts and for unfamiliar objects, using other cues when objects were present.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14099","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334866","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Child development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1