首页 > 最新文献

Child development最新文献

英文 中文
“Some people will tell jokes to you; some people be racist:” A mixed-method examination of racist jokes and adolescents’ well-being "有些人会对你讲笑话;有些人会有种族歧视:"对种族主义笑话和青少年福祉的混合方法研究。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14095
Aprile D. Benner, Francheska Alers-Rojas, Briana A. López, Shanting Chen

This study examined how adolescents make meaning of racist jokes and their impact on daily well-being using a sequential mixed-methods research design with interview (N = 20; 60% girls, 5% gender-nonconforming; 45% Asian American, 40% Latina/o/x, 10% Black, 5% biracial/multiethnic) and daily diary data (N = 168; 54% girls; 57% Latina/o/x, 21% biracial/multiethnic, 10% Asian American, 9% White, 4% Black). Qualitative results revealed that racist jokes were common, distinct from other overt forms of discrimination, and perceived as harmless when perpetrated by friends. Quantitatively, approximately half of adolescents reported hearing at least one racist joke during the study period, and racist jokes by friends were associated with higher daily angry, anxious, and depressed moods and stress. Racist jokes by known others and strangers were also significantly associated with poorer well-being, although less consistently. Findings highlight the hidden harmful effects of racist jokes on adolescents’ daily mood and stress.

本研究通过访谈(N = 20;60%为女孩,5%为性别不符者;45%为亚裔美国人,40%为拉丁裔/有色人种,10%为黑人,5%为双种族/多种族)和日常日记数据(N = 168;54%为女孩;57%为拉丁裔/有色人种,21%为双种族/多种族,10%为亚裔美国人,9%为白人,4%为黑人),采用顺序混合方法研究设计,考察了青少年如何理解种族主义笑话及其对日常幸福生活的影响。定性结果显示,种族主义笑话很常见,有别于其他公开形式的歧视,如果是朋友开的,被认为是无害的。从数量上看,约有一半的青少年表示在研究期间至少听到过一个种族主义笑话,而朋友开的种族主义笑话与较高的日常愤怒、焦虑、抑郁情绪和压力有关。已知他人和陌生人的种族主义笑话也与较差的幸福感显著相关,但一致性较低。研究结果凸显了种族主义笑话对青少年日常情绪和压力的隐性有害影响。
{"title":"“Some people will tell jokes to you; some people be racist:” A mixed-method examination of racist jokes and adolescents’ well-being","authors":"Aprile D. Benner,&nbsp;Francheska Alers-Rojas,&nbsp;Briana A. López,&nbsp;Shanting Chen","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14095","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14095","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examined how adolescents make meaning of racist jokes and their impact on daily well-being using a sequential mixed-methods research design with interview (<i>N</i> = 20; 60% girls, 5% gender-nonconforming; 45% Asian American, 40% Latina/o/x, 10% Black, 5% biracial/multiethnic) and daily diary data (<i>N</i> = 168; 54% girls; 57% Latina/o/x, 21% biracial/multiethnic, 10% Asian American, 9% White, 4% Black). Qualitative results revealed that racist jokes were common, distinct from other overt forms of discrimination, and perceived as harmless when perpetrated by friends. Quantitatively, approximately half of adolescents reported hearing at least one racist joke during the study period, and racist jokes by friends were associated with higher daily angry, anxious, and depressed moods and stress. Racist jokes by known others and strangers were also significantly associated with poorer well-being, although less consistently. Findings highlight the hidden harmful effects of racist jokes on adolescents’ daily mood and stress.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140292976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“You gotta tell the camera”: Advancing children's engineering learning opportunities through tinkering and digital storytelling "你得告诉相机":通过修补和讲数字故事,增加儿童学习工程学的机会。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14094
Lauren C. Pagano, Riley E. George, David H. Uttal, Catherine A. Haden

This study addressed whether combining tinkering with digital storytelling (i.e., narrating and reflecting about experiences to an imagined audience) can engender engineering learning opportunities. Eighty-four families with 5- to 10-year-old (M = 7.69) children (48% female children; 57% White, 11% Asian, 6% Black) watched a video introducing a tinkering activity and were randomly assigned either to a digital storytelling condition or a no digital storytelling condition during tinkering. After tinkering, families reflected on their tinkering experience and were randomly assigned to either engage in digital storytelling or not. Children in the digital storytelling condition during tinkering spoke most to an imagined audience during tinkering, talked most about engineering at reflection, and remembered the most information about the experience weeks later.

本研究探讨了将修补活动与讲数字故事(即向想象中的观众讲述和反映经历)相结合是否能带来工程学学习机会。84个有5至10岁儿童(中=7.69)的家庭(48%为女性儿童;57%为白人,11%为亚裔,6%为黑人)观看了介绍修补活动的视频,并在修补过程中被随机分配到数字故事讲述或无数字故事讲述的环境中。修补活动结束后,家庭对修补活动的体验进行反思,并随机分配参与或不参与数字故事讲述活动。在修补活动中参与数字故事讲述条件的儿童在修补活动中与想象中的观众交谈最多,在反思时谈论最多的是工程学,并且在几周后记住了有关这次体验的最多信息。
{"title":"“You gotta tell the camera”: Advancing children's engineering learning opportunities through tinkering and digital storytelling","authors":"Lauren C. Pagano,&nbsp;Riley E. George,&nbsp;David H. Uttal,&nbsp;Catherine A. Haden","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14094","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14094","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study addressed whether combining tinkering with digital storytelling (i.e., narrating and reflecting about experiences to an imagined audience) can engender engineering learning opportunities. Eighty-four families with 5- to 10-year-old (<i>M</i> = 7.69) children (48% female children; 57% White, 11% Asian, 6% Black) watched a video introducing a tinkering activity and were randomly assigned either to a digital storytelling condition or a no digital storytelling condition during tinkering. After tinkering, families reflected on their tinkering experience and were randomly assigned to either engage in digital storytelling or not. Children in the digital storytelling condition during tinkering spoke most to an imagined audience during tinkering, talked most about engineering at reflection, and remembered the most information about the experience weeks later.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14094","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140305093","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's moral evaluations of and behaviors toward people who are curious about religion and science 儿童对宗教和科学好奇者的道德评价和行为。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14088
Ariel J. Mosley, Cindel J. M. White, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon

Although children exhibit curiosity regarding science, questions remain regarding how children evaluate others' curiosity and whether evaluations differ across domains that prioritize faith (e.g., religion) versus those that value questioning (e.g., science). In Study 1 (n = 115 5- to 8-year-olds; 49% female; 66% White), children evaluated actors who were curious, ignorant and non-curious, or knowledgeable about religion or science; curiosity elicited relatively favorable moral evaluations (ds > .40). Study 2 (n = 62 7- to 8-year-olds; 48% female; 63% White) found that these evaluations generalized to behaviors, as children acted more pro-socially and less punitively toward curious, versus not curious, individuals (ηp2 = .37). These findings (data collected 2020–2022) demonstrate children's positive moral evaluations of curiosity and contribute to debates regarding overlap between scientific and religious cognition.

尽管儿童对科学表现出好奇心,但对于儿童如何评价他人的好奇心,以及在以信仰为优先的领域(如宗教)和以质疑为优先的领域(如科学)中,评价是否有所不同,仍然存在疑问。在研究 1(n = 115 名 5 至 8 岁儿童;49% 为女性;66% 为白人)中,儿童对宗教或科学方面好奇、无知、不好奇或有知识的行为者进行了评价;好奇心引起了相对有利的道德评价(ds > .40)。研究 2(n = 62 名 7 至 8 岁儿童;48% 为女性;63% 为白人)发现,这些评价会扩展到行为上,因为儿童对好奇心强的人和不好奇的人表现出更多的社会支持和更少的惩罚(η p 2 $$ {eta}_mathrm{p}}^2 $$ = .37)。这些发现(数据收集于2020-2022年)表明儿童对好奇心有着积极的道德评价,并有助于有关科学认知与宗教认知重叠的辩论。
{"title":"Children's moral evaluations of and behaviors toward people who are curious about religion and science","authors":"Ariel J. Mosley,&nbsp;Cindel J. M. White,&nbsp;Larisa Heiphetz Solomon","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14088","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14088","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although children exhibit curiosity regarding science, questions remain regarding how children evaluate others' curiosity and whether evaluations differ across domains that prioritize faith (e.g., religion) versus those that value questioning (e.g., science). In Study 1 (<i>n</i> = 115 5- to 8-year-olds; 49% female; 66% White), children evaluated actors who were curious, ignorant and non-curious, or knowledgeable about religion or science; curiosity elicited relatively favorable moral evaluations (<i>d</i>s &gt; .40). Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 62 7- to 8-year-olds; 48% female; 63% White) found that these evaluations generalized to behaviors, as children acted more pro-socially and less punitively toward curious, versus not curious, individuals (<span></span><math>\u0000 <semantics>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msubsup>\u0000 <mi>η</mi>\u0000 <mi>p</mi>\u0000 <mn>2</mn>\u0000 </msubsup>\u0000 </mrow>\u0000 </semantics></math> = .37). These findings (data collected 2020–2022) demonstrate children's positive moral evaluations of curiosity and contribute to debates regarding overlap between scientific and religious cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140292977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ownership-attributing intuitions are cross-culturally shared 所有权归属的直觉是跨文化共享的。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14092
Michał Białek, Michal Mikolaj Stefanczyk, Marta Kowal, Piotr Sorokowski

This study tested intuitions about ownership in children of Dani people, an indigenous Papuan society (N = 79, Mage = 7, 49.4% females). The results show that similar to studies with children from Western societies, children infer ownership from (1) control of permission, (2) ownership of the territory the object is located in, and (3) manmade versus natural origins of the object. By contrast, they did not (4) infer ownership from the first observed possession of an object. Additionally, Papuan children showed (5) an absolute first possession heuristic, whereby they assigned ownership to a person who achieved a goal, in contrast to a person who was first to pursue this goal but failed to be the first to claim it.

本研究测试了巴布亚原住民达尼族儿童对所有权的直觉(N = 79,Mage = 7,49.4% 为女性)。结果显示,与对西方社会儿童的研究类似,儿童从以下方面推断所有权:(1) 对许可的控制;(2) 对物品所在领土的所有权;(3) 物品的人工来源与自然来源。相比之下,他们不会(4) 从首次观察到的物品拥有情况中推断出所有权。此外,巴布亚儿童还表现出(5)绝对的首先占有启发式,即他们将所有权归属于实现了目标的人,而不是首先追求这一目标但未能首先拥有它的人。
{"title":"Ownership-attributing intuitions are cross-culturally shared","authors":"Michał Białek,&nbsp;Michal Mikolaj Stefanczyk,&nbsp;Marta Kowal,&nbsp;Piotr Sorokowski","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14092","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14092","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study tested intuitions about ownership in children of Dani people, an indigenous Papuan society (<i>N</i> = 79, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 7, 49.4% females). The results show that similar to studies with children from Western societies, children infer ownership from (1) control of permission, (2) ownership of the territory the object is located in, and (3) manmade versus natural origins of the object. By contrast, they did not (4) infer ownership from the first observed possession of an object. Additionally, Papuan children showed (5) an absolute first possession heuristic, whereby they assigned ownership to a person who achieved a goal, in contrast to a person who was first to pursue this goal but failed to be the first to claim it.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140206401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age differences in generalization, memory specificity, and their overnight fate in childhood 概括能力、记忆特异性的年龄差异及其在童年时期的隔夜命运。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-22 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14089
Elisa S. Buchberger, Ann-Kathrin Joechner, Chi T. Ngo, Ulman Lindenberger, Markus Werkle-Bergner

Memory enables generalization to new situations, and memory specificity that preserves individual episodes. This study investigated generalization, memory specificity, and their overnight fate in 141 4- to 8-year-olds (computerized memory game; 71 females, tested 2020–2021 in Germany). The results replicated age effects in generalization and memory specificity, and a contingency of generalization on object conceptual properties and interobject semantic proximity. Age effects were stronger in generalization than in memory specificity, and generalization was more closely linked to the explicit regularity knowledge in older than in younger children. After an overnight delay, older children retained more generalized and specific memories and showed greater gains but only in generalization. These findings reveal distinct age differences in generalization and memory specificity across childhood.

记忆可以使记忆泛化到新的情境中,也可以使记忆特异性保留个别情节。本研究调查了 141 名 4 至 8 岁儿童(电脑记忆游戏;71 名女性,2020-2021 年在德国接受测试)的记忆泛化、记忆特异性及其一夜之间的命运。结果重复了泛化和记忆特异性的年龄效应,以及泛化对对象概念属性和对象间语义接近性的或然性。年龄效应对概括能力的影响比对记忆特异性的影响更大,而且大龄儿童的概括能力与显性规律性知识的联系比小龄儿童更紧密。经过一夜的延迟后,年龄较大的儿童保留了更多的泛化记忆和特定记忆,并取得了更大的进步,但仅限于泛化记忆。这些发现揭示了儿童期在记忆的概括性和特异性方面存在明显的年龄差异。
{"title":"Age differences in generalization, memory specificity, and their overnight fate in childhood","authors":"Elisa S. Buchberger,&nbsp;Ann-Kathrin Joechner,&nbsp;Chi T. Ngo,&nbsp;Ulman Lindenberger,&nbsp;Markus Werkle-Bergner","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14089","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14089","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Memory enables generalization to new situations, and memory specificity that preserves individual episodes. This study investigated generalization, memory specificity, and their overnight fate in 141 4- to 8-year-olds (computerized memory game; 71 females, tested 2020–2021 in Germany). The results replicated age effects in generalization and memory specificity, and a contingency of generalization on object conceptual properties and interobject semantic proximity. Age effects were stronger in generalization than in memory specificity, and generalization was more closely linked to the explicit regularity knowledge in older than in younger children. After an overnight delay, older children retained more generalized and specific memories and showed greater gains but only in generalization. These findings reveal distinct age differences in generalization and memory specificity across childhood.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14089","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140183907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do adolescents use choice to learn about their preferences? Development of value refinement and its associations with depressive symptoms in adolescence 青少年是否通过选择来了解自己的偏好?价值细化的发展及其与青春期抑郁症状的关联。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14084
M. E. Moses-Payne, D. G. Lee, J. P. Roiser

Independent decision making requires forming stable estimates of one's preferences. We assessed whether adolescents learn about their preferences through choice deliberation and whether depressive symptoms disrupt this process. Adolescents aged 11–18 (N = 214; participated 2021–22; Female: 53.9%; White/Black/Asian/Mixed/Arab or Latin American: 26/21/19/9/8%) rated multiple activities, chose between pairs of activities and re-rated those activities. As expected, overall, participants uprated chosen and downrated unchosen activities (dz = .20). This value refinement through choice was not evident in younger participants but emerged across adolescence. Contrary to our predictions, depressive symptoms were associated with greater value refinement. Despite this, more depressed adolescents reported lower value certainty and choice confidence. The cognitive processes through which choice deliberation shapes preference develop over adolescence, and are disrupted in depression.

独立决策需要对自己的偏好形成稳定的估计。我们评估了青少年是否通过选择审议来了解自己的偏好,以及抑郁症状是否会干扰这一过程。11-18 岁的青少年(N = 214;参与人数:2021-22;女性:53.9%;白人/黑人/亚洲人/混血人/阿拉伯人或拉丁美洲人:26/21/19/9/8%)对多项活动进行了评分,在成对的活动中进行了选择,并对这些活动进行了重新评分。不出所料,总体而言,参与者对选择的活动评分较高,而对未选择的活动评分较低(dz = 0.20)。这种通过选择来完善价值的现象在年龄较小的参与者身上并不明显,但在整个青春期都出现了。与我们的预测相反,抑郁症状与更高的价值完善度有关。尽管如此,抑郁程度较高的青少年的价值确定性和选择信心却较低。选择审议形成偏好的认知过程是在青春期发展起来的,并在抑郁时受到干扰。
{"title":"Do adolescents use choice to learn about their preferences? Development of value refinement and its associations with depressive symptoms in adolescence","authors":"M. E. Moses-Payne,&nbsp;D. G. Lee,&nbsp;J. P. Roiser","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14084","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14084","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Independent decision making requires forming stable estimates of one's preferences. We assessed whether adolescents learn about their preferences through choice deliberation and whether depressive symptoms disrupt this process. Adolescents aged 11–18 (<i>N</i> = 214; participated 2021–22; Female: 53.9%; White/Black/Asian/Mixed/Arab or Latin American: 26/21/19/9/8%) rated multiple activities, chose between pairs of activities and re-rated those activities. As expected, overall, participants uprated chosen and downrated unchosen activities (<i>dz</i> = .20). This value refinement through choice was not evident in younger participants but emerged across adolescence. Contrary to our predictions, depressive symptoms were associated with greater value refinement. Despite this, more depressed adolescents reported lower value certainty and choice confidence. The cognitive processes through which choice deliberation shapes preference develop over adolescence, and are disrupted in depression.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14084","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140058788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents' and classmates' influences on adolescents' ethnic prejudice: A longitudinal multi-informant study 父母和同学对青少年种族偏见的影响:多信息纵向研究。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14087
Beatrice Bobba, Susan Branje, Elisabetta Crocetti

The family and classroom are important contexts that can contribute to the socialization of ethnic prejudice. However, less is known about their unique, relative, and synergic contributions in influencing youth's affective and cognitive prejudice. The current longitudinal study examined these processes and possible moderators among 688 Italian youth (49.13% girls; Mage = 15.61 years), their parents (nmothers = 603, nfathers = 471; Mage = 49.51 years), and classmates between January/February 2022 and January/February 2023. Cross-lagged panel models highlighted that parents and classmates exert unique and relative influences on different dimensions of adolescents' prejudice. Additionally, different interaction effects also emerged for affective (i.e., adverse compensatory effect) and cognitive (i.e., amplifying effect) prejudice. Thus, adolescents draw from the multiple contexts of development to orient themselves in the social world.

家庭和课堂是造成种族偏见社会化的重要环境。然而,人们对它们在影响青少年情感和认知偏见方面的独特、相对和协同作用却知之甚少。本纵向研究在2022年1月/2月至2023年1月/2月期间,对688名意大利青少年(49.13%为女孩;年龄=15.61岁)、他们的父母(母亲人数=603人,父亲人数=471人;年龄=49.51岁)和同学进行了研究。交叉滞后面板模型突出表明,父母和同学对青少年偏见的不同维度产生了独特和相对的影响。此外,情感偏见(即不利补偿效应)和认知偏见(即放大效应)也出现了不同的交互效应。因此,青少年可以从多种发展背景中汲取营养,以确定自己在社会世界中的方向。
{"title":"Parents' and classmates' influences on adolescents' ethnic prejudice: A longitudinal multi-informant study","authors":"Beatrice Bobba,&nbsp;Susan Branje,&nbsp;Elisabetta Crocetti","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14087","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14087","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The family and classroom are important contexts that can contribute to the socialization of ethnic prejudice. However, less is known about their unique, relative, and synergic contributions in influencing youth's affective and cognitive prejudice. The current longitudinal study examined these processes and possible moderators among 688 Italian youth (49.13% girls; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 15.61 years), their parents (<i>n</i><sub>mothers</sub> = 603, <i>n</i><sub>fathers</sub> = 471; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 49.51 years), and classmates between January/February 2022 and January/February 2023. Cross-lagged panel models highlighted that parents and classmates exert unique and relative influences on different dimensions of adolescents' prejudice. Additionally, different interaction effects also emerged for affective (i.e., adverse compensatory effect) and cognitive (i.e., amplifying effect) prejudice. Thus, adolescents draw from the multiple contexts of development to orient themselves in the social world.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14087","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140058789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential psychophysiological responses associated with decision-making in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds 不同社会经济背景儿童与决策相关的心理生理反应差异。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14082
Hernán Delgado, Sebastián Lipina, M. Carmen Pastor, Graciela Muniz-Terrera, Ñeranei Menéndez, Richard Rodríguez, Alejandra Carboni

This study examined how socioeconomic status (SES) influences on decision-making processing. The roles of anticipatory/outcome-related cardiac activity and awareness of task contingencies were also assessed. One hundred twelve children (Mage = 5.83, SDage = 0.32; 52.7% female, 51.8% low-SES; data collected October–December 2018 and April–December 2019) performed the Children's Gambling Task, while heart rate activity was recorded. Awareness of gain/loss contingencies was assessed after completing the task. Distinct decision-making strategies emerged among low and middle/high-SES children. Despite similar awareness levels between SES groups, future-oriented decision-making was linked solely to the middle/high-SES group. Somatic markers did not manifest unequivocally. However, contrasting cardiac patterns were evident concerning feedback processing and the association between anticipatory activity and awareness (low: acceleration vs. middle/high: deceleration). Results are interpreted from an evolutionary-developmental perspective.

本研究探讨了社会经济地位(SES)如何影响决策过程。此外,还评估了与预期/结果相关的心脏活动和对任务意外情况的认识的作用。112 名儿童(Mage = 5.83,SDage = 0.32;52.7% 为女性,51.8% 为低社会经济地位;数据收集时间为 2018 年 10 月至 12 月和 2019 年 4 月至 12 月)进行了儿童赌博任务,同时记录了心率活动。完成任务后,对收益/损失应急意识进行了评估。低等和中等/高等收入儿童的决策策略各不相同。尽管社会经济地位组之间的意识水平相似,但面向未来的决策仅与中/高社会经济地位组有关。躯体标记没有明确表现出来。然而,在反馈处理和预期活动与意识之间的关联(低:加速与中、高:减速)方面,对比明显的心脏模式。研究结果从进化-发展的角度进行了解释。
{"title":"Differential psychophysiological responses associated with decision-making in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds","authors":"Hernán Delgado,&nbsp;Sebastián Lipina,&nbsp;M. Carmen Pastor,&nbsp;Graciela Muniz-Terrera,&nbsp;Ñeranei Menéndez,&nbsp;Richard Rodríguez,&nbsp;Alejandra Carboni","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14082","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14082","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examined how socioeconomic status (SES) influences on decision-making processing. The roles of anticipatory/outcome-related cardiac activity and awareness of task contingencies were also assessed. One hundred twelve children (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 5.83, SD<sub>age</sub> = 0.32; 52.7% female, 51.8% low-SES; data collected October–December 2018 and April–December 2019) performed the Children's Gambling Task, while heart rate activity was recorded. Awareness of gain/loss contingencies was assessed after completing the task. Distinct decision-making strategies emerged among low and middle/high-SES children. Despite similar awareness levels between SES groups, future-oriented decision-making was linked solely to the middle/high-SES group. Somatic markers did not manifest unequivocally. However, contrasting cardiac patterns were evident concerning feedback processing and the association between anticipatory activity and awareness (low: acceleration vs. middle/high: deceleration). Results are interpreted from an evolutionary-developmental perspective.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140021057","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental differential treatment of siblings linked with internalizing and externalizing behavior: A meta-analysis 父母对兄弟姐妹的区别对待与内化和外化行为有关:荟萃分析。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14091
Alexander C. Jensen, Alexandra E. Thomsen

This meta-analysis linked relative and absolute parental differential treatment (PDT) with internalizing and externalizing behavior of children and adolescents. Multilevel meta-analysis data represented 26,451 participants based on 2890 effect sizes coming from 88 sources, nested within 43 samples. Participants were between 3.18 and 18.99 years of age (Mage = 12.64, SD = 3.89; 51.31% female; 82.23% White; 54.68% from the United States). Less-favored treatment (relative PDT) was linked to more internalizing and externalizing behavior. Additionally, greater differences in parenting between siblings (absolute PDT) were linked to more internalizing and externalizing behavior. Some links were moderated by other factors. For example, some effects were stronger when PDT was reported by children, and others, when siblings were closer in age.

这项荟萃分析将父母的相对和绝对差别待遇(PDT)与儿童和青少年的内化和外化行为联系起来。多层次荟萃分析数据代表了 26451 名参与者,基于来自 88 个来源的 2890 个效应大小,嵌套在 43 个样本中。参与者的年龄介于 3.18 岁至 18.99 岁之间(平均年龄 = 12.64 岁,标准差 = 3.89 岁;51.31% 为女性;82.23% 为白人;54.68% 来自美国)。较差的待遇(相对 PDT)与更多的内化和外化行为有关。此外,兄弟姐妹之间的养育差异(绝对 PDT)与更多的内化和外化行为有关。有些联系受到其他因素的调节。例如,当孩子们报告 PDT 时,有些影响会更强,而当兄弟姐妹的年龄更接近时,另一些影响会更强。
{"title":"Parental differential treatment of siblings linked with internalizing and externalizing behavior: A meta-analysis","authors":"Alexander C. Jensen,&nbsp;Alexandra E. Thomsen","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14091","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14091","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This meta-analysis linked relative and absolute parental differential treatment (PDT) with internalizing and externalizing behavior of children and adolescents. Multilevel meta-analysis data represented 26,451 participants based on 2890 effect sizes coming from 88 sources, nested within 43 samples. Participants were between 3.18 and 18.99 years of age (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 12.64, SD = 3.89; 51.31% female; 82.23% White; 54.68% from the United States). Less-favored treatment (relative PDT) was linked to more internalizing and externalizing behavior. Additionally, greater differences in parenting between siblings (absolute PDT) were linked to more internalizing and externalizing behavior. Some links were moderated by other factors. For example, some effects were stronger when PDT was reported by children, and others, when siblings were closer in age.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140027522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
DNA methylation variation after a parenting program for child conduct problems: Findings from a randomized controlled trial 针对儿童行为问题的养育计划后的 DNA 甲基化变异:随机对照试验的结果。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14090
Nicole Creasey, Patty Leijten, Marieke S. Tollenaar, Marco P. Boks, Geertjan Overbeek

This study investigated associations of the Incredible Years (IY) parenting program with children's DNA methylation. Participants were 289 Dutch children aged 3–9 years (75% European ancestry, 48% female) with above-average conduct problems. Saliva was collected 2.5 years after families were randomized to IY or care as usual (CAU). Using an intention-to-treat approach, confirmatory multiple-regression analyses revealed no significant differences between the IY and CAU groups in children's methylation levels at the NR3C1 and FKBP5 genes. However, exploratory epigenome-wide analyses revealed nine differentially methylated regions between groups, coinciding with SLAMF1, MITF, FAM200B, PSD3, SNX31, and CELSR1. The study provides preliminary evidence for associations of IY with children's salivary methylation levels and highlights the need for further research into biological outcomes of parenting programs.

本研究调查了 "不可思议的岁月"(IY)育儿计划与儿童 DNA 甲基化之间的关系。参与者为 289 名 3-9 岁的荷兰儿童(75% 为欧洲血统,48% 为女性),他们的行为问题高于平均水平。在家庭被随机分配到 IY 或照常护理 (CAU) 2.5 年后收集唾液。采用意向治疗方法进行的确认性多元回归分析表明,在 NR3C1 和 FKBP5 基因的甲基化水平上,益智组和照常护理组之间没有显著差异。然而,探索性的全表观基因组分析显示,组间存在九个不同的甲基化区域,分别是 SLAMF1、MITF、FAM200B、PSD3、SNX31 和 CELSR1。该研究提供了国际养育年与儿童唾液甲基化水平相关的初步证据,并强调了进一步研究养育计划生物学结果的必要性。
{"title":"DNA methylation variation after a parenting program for child conduct problems: Findings from a randomized controlled trial","authors":"Nicole Creasey,&nbsp;Patty Leijten,&nbsp;Marieke S. Tollenaar,&nbsp;Marco P. Boks,&nbsp;Geertjan Overbeek","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14090","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14090","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study investigated associations of the Incredible Years (IY) parenting program with children's DNA methylation. Participants were 289 Dutch children aged 3–9 years (75% European ancestry, 48% female) with above-average conduct problems. Saliva was collected 2.5 years after families were randomized to IY or care as usual (CAU). Using an intention-to-treat approach, confirmatory multiple-regression analyses revealed no significant differences between the IY and CAU groups in children's methylation levels at the NR3C1 and FKBP5 genes. However, exploratory epigenome-wide analyses revealed nine differentially methylated regions between groups, coinciding with <i>SLAMF1</i>, <i>MITF</i>, <i>FAM200B</i>, <i>PSD3</i>, <i>SNX31</i>, and <i>CELSR1</i>. The study provides preliminary evidence for associations of IY with children's salivary methylation levels and highlights the need for further research into biological outcomes of parenting programs.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14090","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140021058","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Child development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1