首页 > 最新文献

Child development最新文献

英文 中文
Mother–child collaboration in an Indigenous community: Changing and enduring across generations 土著社区的母子合作:跨代的变化与持久
IF 4.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14181
Barbara Rogoff, Itzel Aceves-Azuara
Changes in family life related to globalization may include reduction in the collaborativeness observed in many Indigenous American communities. The present study examined longitudinal changes and continuities in collaboration in a Guatemalan Maya community experiencing rapid globalization. Fluid collaboration was widespread 3 decades ago among triads of mothers and 1- to 6-year-olds in 24 Mayan families exploring novel objects during home visits (Dayton et al., 2022). However, in the “same” situation 30 years later, 22 mother–child triads of their relatives spent half as much time in collaboration among all three people. This aligns with globalizing changes and with the pattern of Dayton et al.'s middle-class European American families. Nonetheless, the Mayan families maintained harmonious interactions, in line with preserving important cultural values.
与全球化相关的家庭生活的变化可能包括在许多美国土著社区观察到的协作性的减少。本研究考察了一个经历快速全球化的危地马拉玛雅社区中协作的纵向变化和连续性。三十年前,在 24 个玛雅家庭中,母亲和 1-6 岁儿童在家访期间探索新奇物品的三人小组中,流畅协作非常普遍(Dayton 等人,2022 年)。然而,30 年后,在 "同样 "的情况下,22 个母子三人组的亲属在三人协作中所花费的时间只有 30 年前的一半。这与全球化的变化以及代顿等人研究的欧洲中产阶级家庭的模式是一致的。尽管如此,玛雅家庭仍然保持着和谐的互动关系,这与保留重要的文化价值观是一致的。
{"title":"Mother–child collaboration in an Indigenous community: Changing and enduring across generations","authors":"Barbara Rogoff, Itzel Aceves-Azuara","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14181","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14181","url":null,"abstract":"Changes in family life related to globalization may include reduction in the collaborativeness observed in many Indigenous American communities. The present study examined longitudinal changes and continuities in collaboration in a Guatemalan Maya community experiencing rapid globalization. Fluid collaboration was widespread 3 decades ago among triads of mothers and 1- to 6-year-olds in 24 Mayan families exploring novel objects during home visits (Dayton et al., 2022). However, in the “same” situation 30 years later, 22 mother–child triads of their relatives spent half as much time in collaboration among all three people. This aligns with globalizing changes and with the pattern of Dayton et al.'s middle-class European American families. Nonetheless, the Mayan families maintained harmonious interactions, in line with preserving important cultural values.","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142610733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Determinants of socioemotional and behavioral well-being among First Nations children living off-reserve in Canada: A cross-sectional study 生活在加拿大保留地以外的原住民儿童社会情感和行为福祉的决定因素:横断面研究
IF 4.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14192
Sawayra Owais, Maria B. Ospina, Camron D. Ford, Troy Hill, Jessica Lai, John Krzeczkowski, Jacob A. Burack, Ryan J. Van Lieshout
Few studies have focused on off-reserve Indigenous children and families. This nationally representative, cross-sectional study (data collected from 2006 to 2007) examined Indigenous- and non-Indigenous-specific determinants associated with positive socioemotional and behavioral well-being among First Nations children living off-reserve in Canada. The parents or other caregivers of 2990 two-to-five-year-old children (M = 3.65; 50.6% male) reported on their children's socioemotional and behavioral well-being and a range of child, parent, and housing characteristics. Being taught an Indigenous culture, greater community cohesion, caregiver nurturance, good parental/other caregiver health, and fewer household members were associated with better socioemotional and behavioral well-being. These results highlight the importance of leveraging Indigenous-specific determinants and acknowledging non-Indigenous-specific factors, to promote the well-being of First Nations children living off-reserve.
很少有研究关注非保留地土著儿童和家庭。这项具有全国代表性的横断面研究(数据收集于 2006 年至 2007 年)考察了与加拿大非保留地原住民儿童积极的社会情感和行为福祉相关的原住民和非原住民特定决定因素。2 990 名 2 至 5 岁儿童(M = 3.65;50.6% 为男性)的父母或其他照顾者报告了他们孩子的社会情感和行为健康状况,以及一系列儿童、父母和住房特征。接受土著文化教育、更强的社区凝聚力、照顾者的养育、父母/其他照顾者健康状况良好以及家庭成员较少与更好的社会情感和行为幸福感有关。这些结果凸显了利用原住民特有的决定因素并承认非原住民特有因素的重要性,以促进生活在保留地以外的原住民儿童的福祉。
{"title":"Determinants of socioemotional and behavioral well-being among First Nations children living off-reserve in Canada: A cross-sectional study","authors":"Sawayra Owais, Maria B. Ospina, Camron D. Ford, Troy Hill, Jessica Lai, John Krzeczkowski, Jacob A. Burack, Ryan J. Van Lieshout","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14192","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14192","url":null,"abstract":"Few studies have focused on off-reserve Indigenous children and families. This nationally representative, cross-sectional study (data collected from 2006 to 2007) examined Indigenous- and non-Indigenous-specific determinants associated with positive socioemotional and behavioral well-being among First Nations children living off-reserve in Canada. The parents or other caregivers of 2990 two-to-five-year-old children (<i>M</i> = 3.65; 50.6% male) reported on their children's socioemotional and behavioral well-being and a range of child, parent, and housing characteristics. Being taught an Indigenous culture, greater community cohesion, caregiver nurturance, good parental/other caregiver health, and fewer household members were associated with better socioemotional and behavioral well-being. These results highlight the importance of leveraging Indigenous-specific determinants and acknowledging non-Indigenous-specific factors, to promote the well-being of First Nations children living off-reserve.","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142601957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Needing to shout to be heard? Caregiver under‐responsivity and disconnection between vocal signaling and autonomic arousal in infants from chaotic households 需要大声喊叫才能被听到?混乱家庭婴儿的照料者反应不足以及发声信号与自律神经唤醒之间的脱节
IF 4.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-08 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14183
S. V. Wass, C. S. Smith, F. U. Mirza, E. M. G. Greenwood, L. Goupil
Children raised in chaotic households show affect dysregulation during later childhood. To understand why, we took day‐long home recordings using microphones and autonomic monitors from 74 12‐month‐old infant–caregiver dyads (40% male, 60% white, data collected between 2018 and 2021). Caregivers in low‐Confusion Hubbub And Order Scale (chaos) households responded to negative affect infant vocalizations by changing their own arousal and vocalizing in response; but high‐chaos caregivers did not, whereas infants in low‐chaos households consistently produced clusters of negative vocalizations around peaks in their own arousal, high‐chaos infants did not. Their negative vocalizations were less tied to their own underlying arousal. Our data indicate that, in chaotic households, both communicating and responding are atypical: infants are not expressing their levels of arousal, and caregivers are under‐responsive to their infants' behavioral signals.
在混乱家庭中长大的儿童在日后的童年时期会出现情感失调。为了了解原因,我们使用麦克风和自律神经监测器对74个12个月大的婴儿-照顾者二人组(40%为男性,60%为白人,数据收集于2018年至2021年)进行了长达一天的家庭录音。低混乱喧闹与秩序量表(混乱)家庭中的照顾者通过改变自己的唤醒状态和发声来回应婴儿的负面情绪发声;但高混乱家庭中的照顾者没有这样做,而低混乱家庭中的婴儿总是在自己唤醒状态的峰值周围产生负面发声集群,高混乱家庭中的婴儿则没有。他们的负面发声与自身的潜在唤醒联系较少。我们的数据表明,在混乱的家庭中,交流和反应都是不典型的:婴儿没有表达出自己的唤醒水平,而照料者对婴儿的行为信号反应不足。
{"title":"Needing to shout to be heard? Caregiver under‐responsivity and disconnection between vocal signaling and autonomic arousal in infants from chaotic households","authors":"S. V. Wass, C. S. Smith, F. U. Mirza, E. M. G. Greenwood, L. Goupil","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14183","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14183","url":null,"abstract":"Children raised in chaotic households show affect dysregulation during later childhood. To understand why, we took day‐long home recordings using microphones and autonomic monitors from 74 12‐month‐old infant–caregiver dyads (40% male, 60% white, data collected between 2018 and 2021). Caregivers in low‐Confusion Hubbub And Order Scale (chaos) households responded to negative affect infant vocalizations by changing their own arousal and vocalizing in response; but high‐chaos caregivers did not, whereas infants in low‐chaos households consistently produced clusters of negative vocalizations around peaks in their own arousal, high‐chaos infants did not. Their negative vocalizations were less tied to their own underlying arousal. Our data indicate that, in chaotic households, both communicating and responding are atypical: infants are not expressing their levels of arousal, and caregivers are under‐responsive to their infants' behavioral signals.","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142596909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To heal, grow, and thrive: Engaging Indigenous paradigms and perspectives in developmental science. 治愈、成长和茁壮:让土著范式和观点参与发展科学。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-04 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14194
Monica Tsethlikai, Ashley Cole, Adam J Hoffman, Megan Bang, Florrie Fei-Yin Ng

A focus on positive child development among Indigenous children has largely been absent from developmental science. In this special section of Child Development, we sought to address continuing inequity in representation and valuing Indigenous knowledge and voices by soliciting articles that identified cultural and strengths-based factors Indigenous children, youth, and families cultivate and leverage to promote positive development. In this introduction to the special section, we provide an overview of the four empirical articles included, with attention to the ways these articles advance Indigenous paradigms and methodologies by focusing on the unique histories and strengths of four distinct Indigenous communities. We end with a discussion of how to promote continued growth and inclusiveness in developmental science with Indigenous communities.

在发展科学中,对土著儿童积极儿童发展的关注在很大程度上是缺失的。在《儿童发展》的这一专栏中,我们试图通过征集文章,确定土著儿童、青年和家庭为促进积极发展而培养和利用的文化和优势因素,从而解决在代表和重视土著知识和声音方面持续存在的不平等问题。在本专栏的导言中,我们将概述收录的四篇实证文章,并关注这些文章如何通过关注四个不同土著社区的独特历史和优势来推进土著范式和方法。最后,我们将讨论如何促进土著社区发展科学的持续发展和包容性。
{"title":"To heal, grow, and thrive: Engaging Indigenous paradigms and perspectives in developmental science.","authors":"Monica Tsethlikai, Ashley Cole, Adam J Hoffman, Megan Bang, Florrie Fei-Yin Ng","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14194","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14194","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A focus on positive child development among Indigenous children has largely been absent from developmental science. In this special section of Child Development, we sought to address continuing inequity in representation and valuing Indigenous knowledge and voices by soliciting articles that identified cultural and strengths-based factors Indigenous children, youth, and families cultivate and leverage to promote positive development. In this introduction to the special section, we provide an overview of the four empirical articles included, with attention to the ways these articles advance Indigenous paradigms and methodologies by focusing on the unique histories and strengths of four distinct Indigenous communities. We end with a discussion of how to promote continued growth and inclusiveness in developmental science with Indigenous communities.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142567533","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bigger versus smaller: Children's understanding of size comparison words becomes more precise with age. 大与小随着年龄的增长,儿童对大小比较词的理解会越来越精确。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14182
Alissa L Ferry, Mia G Corcoran, Emily Williams, Sheila M Curtis, Cathryn J Gale, Katherine E Twomey

The ability to compare plays a key role in how humans learn, but words that describe relations between objects, like comparisons, are difficult to learn. We examined how children learn size comparison words, and how their interpretations of these change across development. One-hundred-and-forty children in England (36-107 months; 68 girls; majority White) were asked to build block structures that were bigger, longer, smaller, shorter, or taller than an experimenter's. Children were most successful with words that refer to size increases. Younger children were less accurate with smaller and shorter, often building bigger structures. The dimensional aspect of taller emerged gradually. These findings suggest that children's interpretation of the meaning of size comparison words changes and becomes more precise across development.

比较能力在人类的学习过程中起着关键作用,但描述物体之间关系的词,如比较词,却很难学习。我们研究了儿童是如何学习大小比较词的,以及他们对这些词的解释在整个成长过程中是如何变化的。我们要求英格兰的 140 名儿童(36-107 个月;68 名女孩;大多数为白人)搭建比实验者的积木更大、更长、更小、更短或更高的积木结构。儿童在使用指尺寸增加的词语时最为成功。年龄较小的儿童对 "更小 "和 "更短 "的准确度较低,他们往往搭建出更大的结构。而 "高 "则是逐渐出现的。这些研究结果表明,儿童对大小比较词语含义的解释会随着发育的变化而改变,并变得更加精确。
{"title":"Bigger versus smaller: Children's understanding of size comparison words becomes more precise with age.","authors":"Alissa L Ferry, Mia G Corcoran, Emily Williams, Sheila M Curtis, Cathryn J Gale, Katherine E Twomey","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14182","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14182","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to compare plays a key role in how humans learn, but words that describe relations between objects, like comparisons, are difficult to learn. We examined how children learn size comparison words, and how their interpretations of these change across development. One-hundred-and-forty children in England (36-107 months; 68 girls; majority White) were asked to build block structures that were bigger, longer, smaller, shorter, or taller than an experimenter's. Children were most successful with words that refer to size increases. Younger children were less accurate with smaller and shorter, often building bigger structures. The dimensional aspect of taller emerged gradually. These findings suggest that children's interpretation of the meaning of size comparison words changes and becomes more precise across development.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142557291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Overcoming human exceptionalism: The role of ethical nature-culture relations in the developmental contexts of indigenous children 克服人类例外论:自然-文化伦理关系在土著儿童成长环境中的作用
IF 4.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-30 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14195
Emma Elliott, Jillian Fish
Indigenous populations, including American Indians, Alaska Natives, First Nations, and other first peoples worldwide, have been largely overlooked in child development research. This commentary examines how Indigenous relationality intersects with developmental science, advocating for a shift from human exceptionalism to an interconnected relationality among people, land, and more-than-human beings. Drawing from diverse Indigenous knowledge and practices, The Six Pillars to Advance Indigenous Relationality among Children provides frameworks for integrating Indigenous worldviews emphasizing interconnected responsibilities and sustainability. Embracing Indigenous relationality—grounded in respect and reciprocity—dismantles inequitable systems, enhances socioecological well-being, and supports healthy Indigenous child development, fostering responsible relationships with the land and ensuring a sustainable future for generations.
土著居民,包括美国印第安人、阿拉斯加原住民、原住民和世界各地的其他原住民,在儿童发展研究中大多被忽视。这篇评论探讨了原住民的关系性如何与发展科学相交融,倡导从人类例外论转向人、土地和超人类之间相互关联的关系性。促进土著儿童关系性的六大支柱》从不同的土著知识和实践中汲取营养,提供了整合土著世界观的框架,强调相互关联的责任和可持续性。建立在尊重和互惠基础上的原住民关系,可以消除不公平的制度,提高社会生态福祉,支持原住民儿童的健康发展,促进与土地之间负责任的关系,确保世世代代拥有可持续的未来。
{"title":"Overcoming human exceptionalism: The role of ethical nature-culture relations in the developmental contexts of indigenous children","authors":"Emma Elliott, Jillian Fish","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14195","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14195","url":null,"abstract":"Indigenous populations, including American Indians, Alaska Natives, First Nations, and other first peoples worldwide, have been largely overlooked in child development research. This commentary examines how Indigenous relationality intersects with developmental science, advocating for a shift from human exceptionalism to an interconnected relationality among people, land, and more-than-human beings. Drawing from diverse Indigenous knowledge and practices, <i>The Six Pillars to Advance Indigenous Relationality among Children</i> provides frameworks for integrating Indigenous worldviews emphasizing interconnected responsibilities and sustainability. Embracing Indigenous relationality—grounded in respect and reciprocity—dismantles inequitable systems, enhances socioecological well-being, and supports healthy Indigenous child development, fostering responsible relationships with the land and ensuring a sustainable future for generations.","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142541558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“Oh, the places you'll go”: The psychological consequences of omission and misrepresentation for Native children "哦,你要去的地方":遗漏和失实陈述给土著儿童带来的心理后果
IF 4.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-30 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14193
Stephanie A. Fryberg, Arianne E. Eason
All children navigate the world by searching for information in their sociocultural contexts (e.g., schools, media, laws) to make sense of their experiences and potential futures. In doing so, Native children, however, must contend with the legacy and ongoing oppression of their Peoples, communities, and ways of being. In this manuscript, we highlight how sociocultural contexts stemming from settler colonialism undermine Native futures. We detail how settler colonialism, specifically its manifestation in acts of omission and misrepresentation, impacts children's self and identity development, and intergroup relations. We close by acknowledging the importance of understanding development in context, and the power that the representational landscape can have for promoting paths toward positive child development and greater equity.
所有儿童都通过在其社会文化背景(如学校、媒体、法律)中搜索信息来了解自己的经历和潜在的未来,从而在世界上游刃有余。然而,在这样做的过程中,土著儿童必须与他们的民族、社区和生存方式遗留下来的和持续的压迫作斗争。在本手稿中,我们强调了源自定居者殖民主义的社会文化背景如何破坏原住民的未来。我们详细介绍了定居者殖民主义,特别是其在遗漏和歪曲行为中的表现,是如何影响儿童的自我和身份发展以及群体间关系的。最后,我们承认在背景中理解发展的重要性,以及表象景观在促进儿童积极发展和更大公平方面的力量。
{"title":"“Oh, the places you'll go”: The psychological consequences of omission and misrepresentation for Native children","authors":"Stephanie A. Fryberg, Arianne E. Eason","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14193","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14193","url":null,"abstract":"All children navigate the world by searching for information in their sociocultural contexts (e.g., schools, media, laws) to make sense of their experiences and potential futures. In doing so, Native children, however, must contend with the legacy and ongoing oppression of their Peoples, communities, and ways of being. In this manuscript, we highlight how sociocultural contexts stemming from settler colonialism undermine Native futures. We detail how settler colonialism, specifically its manifestation in acts of omission and misrepresentation, impacts children's self and identity development, and intergroup relations. We close by acknowledging the importance of understanding development in context, and the power that the representational landscape can have for promoting paths toward positive child development and greater equity.","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142541556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Experiences of discrimination and snacking behavior in Black and Latinx children. 黑人和拉丁裔儿童的歧视经历与零食行为。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14191
Katherine B Ehrlich, Julie M Brisson, Elizabeth R Wiggins, Sarah M Lyle, Manuela Celia-Sanchez, Daisy Gallegos, Anna Langer, Kharah M Ross, Mary A Gerend

Little is known about how discrimination contributes to health behaviors in childhood. We examined the association between children's exposure to discrimination and their snacking behavior in a sample of youth of color (N = 164, Mage = 11.5 years, 49% female, 60% Black, 40% Hispanic/Latinx). We also explored whether children's body mass index (BMI) or sleepiness moderated the association between discrimination and calorie consumption. The significant link between discrimination and calorie consumption was moderated by children's BMI, such that discrimination was associated with calorie consumption for children with BMI percentiles above 79%. Children's sleepiness did not serve as an additional moderator. Efforts to promote health should consider children's broader socio-contextual experiences, including discrimination, as factors that may shape eating patterns.

人们对歧视如何影响儿童期健康行为知之甚少。我们以有色人种青少年为样本(样本数=164,年龄=11.5岁,49%为女性,60%为黑人,40%为西班牙裔/拉丁裔),研究了儿童遭受歧视与他们吃零食行为之间的关系。我们还探讨了儿童的体重指数(BMI)或嗜睡程度是否会调节歧视与卡路里消耗量之间的关系。儿童的体重指数调节了歧视与卡路里消耗量之间的重要联系,因此体重指数高于 79% 的儿童的歧视与卡路里消耗量有关。儿童的嗜睡程度没有起到额外的调节作用。促进健康的工作应考虑儿童更广泛的社会背景经历,包括歧视,因为这些因素可能会影响饮食模式。
{"title":"Experiences of discrimination and snacking behavior in Black and Latinx children.","authors":"Katherine B Ehrlich, Julie M Brisson, Elizabeth R Wiggins, Sarah M Lyle, Manuela Celia-Sanchez, Daisy Gallegos, Anna Langer, Kharah M Ross, Mary A Gerend","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14191","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14191","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Little is known about how discrimination contributes to health behaviors in childhood. We examined the association between children's exposure to discrimination and their snacking behavior in a sample of youth of color (N = 164, M<sub>age</sub> = 11.5 years, 49% female, 60% Black, 40% Hispanic/Latinx). We also explored whether children's body mass index (BMI) or sleepiness moderated the association between discrimination and calorie consumption. The significant link between discrimination and calorie consumption was moderated by children's BMI, such that discrimination was associated with calorie consumption for children with BMI percentiles above 79%. Children's sleepiness did not serve as an additional moderator. Efforts to promote health should consider children's broader socio-contextual experiences, including discrimination, as factors that may shape eating patterns.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142521139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers use probabilistic evidence to flexibly change or maintain expectations on an active search task. 学龄前儿童在主动搜索任务中利用概率证据灵活改变或维持预期。
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14190
Brooke C Hilton, Mark A Sabbagh

This study investigated 3- to 5-year-olds' (N = 64, 37 girls, 62.5% White, data collected between 2021-2022) ability to use probabilistic information gleaned through active search to appropriately change or maintain expectations. In an online fishing game, children first learned that one of two ponds was good for catching fish. During a subsequent testing phase, children searched the ponds for fish. Half saw outcomes that were probabilistically consistent with training, and the other half saw outcomes that were probabilistically inconsistent. Children in the Inconsistent condition adapted their search strategies, showing evidence of changing their expectations. Those in the Consistent condition maintained their initial search strategy. Trial-by-trial analyses suggested that children used a combination of heuristic and information integration strategies to guide their search behavior.

本研究调查了 3 至 5 岁儿童(人数 = 64,37 名女孩,62.5% 为白人,数据收集于 2021-2022 年)利用通过主动搜索获得的概率信息来适当改变或维持预期的能力。在一款在线捕鱼游戏中,儿童首先了解到两个池塘中的一个适合捕鱼。在随后的测试阶段,儿童在池塘中寻找鱼。一半儿童看到的结果在概率上与训练一致,另一半儿童看到的结果在概率上不一致。不一致条件下的儿童调整了他们的搜索策略,显示出他们的期望发生了变化。而处于 "一致 "条件下的儿童则保持了最初的搜索策略。逐次试验分析表明,儿童使用启发式策略和信息整合策略来指导他们的搜索行为。
{"title":"Preschoolers use probabilistic evidence to flexibly change or maintain expectations on an active search task.","authors":"Brooke C Hilton, Mark A Sabbagh","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14190","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14190","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated 3- to 5-year-olds' (N = 64, 37 girls, 62.5% White, data collected between 2021-2022) ability to use probabilistic information gleaned through active search to appropriately change or maintain expectations. In an online fishing game, children first learned that one of two ponds was good for catching fish. During a subsequent testing phase, children searched the ponds for fish. Half saw outcomes that were probabilistically consistent with training, and the other half saw outcomes that were probabilistically inconsistent. Children in the Inconsistent condition adapted their search strategies, showing evidence of changing their expectations. Those in the Consistent condition maintained their initial search strategy. Trial-by-trial analyses suggested that children used a combination of heuristic and information integration strategies to guide their search behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142521140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early-life threat and deprivation: Are children similarly affected by exposure to each? 早期生活的威胁和匮乏:儿童受到的影响是否相似?
IF 3.9 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-28 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14188
Kristina Sayler, Katie A McLaughlin, Jay Belsky

Extensive evidence documents negative consequences of adversity for children's development. Here, we extend such work by looking beyond average effects to consider variation in susceptibility to both threat and deprivation in terms of cognitive and social-emotional development, using an influence-statistic methodology. Data come from the ongoing Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (N = 14,541, 49.1% female, age range: 6 month to 12 year, race and ethnicity: 97.8% white, 0.4% black, and 0.6% other). With respect to anticipated associations of threat with problem behavior and of deprivation with cognition, results of this pre-registered research revealed that a roughly equal proportion of children proved to be susceptible in a domain-general manner (similarly influenced) and a domain-specific one (dissimilarly influenced). Implications for intervention are considered.

大量证据表明,逆境会对儿童的发展造成负面影响。在此,我们采用影响统计方法,超越平均效应,从认知和社会情感发展的角度考虑易受威胁和匮乏影响的差异,从而扩展这些研究。数据来自正在进行的雅芳父母与子女纵向研究(N = 14,541,49.1% 为女性,年龄范围为 6 个月至 12 岁,种族和性别不限):6个月至12岁,种族和民族:97.8%为白人,0.4%为黑人,0.6%为其他种族)。关于威胁与问题行为以及贫困与认知之间的预期联系,这项预先登记的研究结果表明,大致相同比例的儿童易受一般领域(类似影响)和特定领域(不同影响)的影响。研究考虑了干预措施的意义。
{"title":"Early-life threat and deprivation: Are children similarly affected by exposure to each?","authors":"Kristina Sayler, Katie A McLaughlin, Jay Belsky","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14188","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14188","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extensive evidence documents negative consequences of adversity for children's development. Here, we extend such work by looking beyond average effects to consider variation in susceptibility to both threat and deprivation in terms of cognitive and social-emotional development, using an influence-statistic methodology. Data come from the ongoing Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (N = 14,541, 49.1% female, age range: 6 month to 12 year, race and ethnicity: 97.8% white, 0.4% black, and 0.6% other). With respect to anticipated associations of threat with problem behavior and of deprivation with cognition, results of this pre-registered research revealed that a roughly equal proportion of children proved to be susceptible in a domain-general manner (similarly influenced) and a domain-specific one (dissimilarly influenced). Implications for intervention are considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142521138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Child development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1