首页 > 最新文献

Color Research and Application最新文献

英文 中文
Color blindness and semantic knowledge: Cognition of color terms from elicited lists in dichromats and normal observers 色盲与语义知识:色盲和正常观察者从诱导列表中认知颜色术语
IF 1.2 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1002/col.22925
Humberto Moreira, Leticia Álvaro, Julio Lillo

Color blindness (color vision deficiency) affects ~8% of males and ~ 0.4% females worldwide. Here we use the elicited lists method to investigate their semantic knowledge regarding “basic color terms” and their relationships. Lists were obtained from color vision deficient and normal observers. 32 dichromats (15 protanopes, 17 deuteranopes) and 32 normal trichromats (17 females, 15 males) diagnosed by a battery of color tests (Ishihara, City University Test, anomaloscope) wrote monolexemic lists of colors. Psychological salience of terms (ln(CSI)), adjacency between pairs of terms (ADJ; MDS) and the presence of clusters of terms defined on the basis of the Universals and evolution hypothesis were analyzed. All four groups of participants showed the same semantic memory structure: lists started with the cardinal primaries cluster (blue, red, yellow, green), followed by the achromatic primaries cluster (black and white), or the derived cluster (brown, orange, violet, pink, purple, and gray). After the clusters (cardinals, achromatics, and derived), a highly variable number of non-basic terms appeared. This number was higher in normal trichromats. Non-basic terms were not part of any cognitive cluster. The similarity in the lists of trichromats and dichromats suggest that both may acquire similar semantic knowledge about color terms. Several potential explanations are considered.

全球约有 8% 的男性和约 0.4% 的女性患有色盲(色觉缺陷)。在这里,我们使用诱发列表法来研究他们对 "基本颜色术语 "及其关系的语义知识。我们从色觉障碍者和正常观察者那里获得了清单。通过一系列颜色测试(石原测试、城市大学测试、异常镜)确诊的 32 名色弱者(15 名原色弱者,17 名去色弱者)和 32 名正常色弱者(17 名女性,15 名男性)写出了单复色表。我们对术语的心理显著性(ln(CSI))、术语对之间的邻接性(ADJ;MDS)以及根据普遍性和进化假说定义的术语群的存在进行了分析。所有四组受试者都表现出相同的语义记忆结构:列表以红基色基团(蓝、红、黄、绿)开始,然后是消色基团(黑、白)或派生基团(棕、橙、紫、粉、紫、灰)。在这些词组(红心、消色差和派生词组)之后,出现了数量变化很大的非基本词汇。正常三色者的非基本词汇数量较多。非基本词汇不属于任何认知集群。三色 "和 "二色 "的词汇表具有相似性,这表明二者可能获得了类似的颜色词汇语义知识。我们考虑了几种可能的解释。
{"title":"Color blindness and semantic knowledge: Cognition of color terms from elicited lists in dichromats and normal observers","authors":"Humberto Moreira,&nbsp;Leticia Álvaro,&nbsp;Julio Lillo","doi":"10.1002/col.22925","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22925","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Color blindness (color vision deficiency) affects ~8% of males and ~ 0.4% females worldwide. Here we use the elicited lists method to investigate their semantic knowledge regarding “basic color terms” and their relationships. Lists were obtained from color vision deficient and normal observers. 32 dichromats (15 protanopes, 17 deuteranopes) and 32 normal trichromats (17 females, 15 males) diagnosed by a battery of color tests (Ishihara, City University Test, anomaloscope) wrote monolexemic lists of colors. Psychological salience of terms (ln(CSI)), adjacency between pairs of terms (ADJ; MDS) and the presence of clusters of terms defined on the basis of the Universals and evolution hypothesis were analyzed. All four groups of participants showed the same semantic memory structure: lists started with the cardinal primaries cluster (blue, red, yellow, green), followed by the achromatic primaries cluster (black and white), or the derived cluster (brown, orange, violet, pink, purple, and gray). After the clusters (cardinals, achromatics, and derived), a highly variable number of non-basic terms appeared. This number was higher in normal trichromats. Non-basic terms were not part of any cognitive cluster. The similarity in the lists of trichromats and dichromats suggest that both may acquire similar semantic knowledge about color terms. Several potential explanations are considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 5","pages":"420-432"},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/col.22925","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140181783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Study of color degradation in package prints: Analyzing kinetics with principal component analysis 包装印刷品色彩降解研究:用主成分分析法分析动力学
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1002/col.22924
Anirban Das, Mahasweta Mandal

Color degradation is a pressing issue in various fields like food and medicine packaging, as well as printed materials, leading to a loss or distortion of vital information. Therefore, understanding how the color of package prints evolves over time is essential for product quality assessment and effective product life cycle management. This study focuses on investigating color changes in prints on blister foils, widely used in the medicine and packaging industries. To assess fading characteristics, samples printed via the gravure process have been exposed to a Xenon Test Chamber (B-SUN). Spectrophotometric measurements of the prints have been conducted using a spectroradiometer, and the data have been analyzed using principal component analysis (PCA). The fading nature of cyan and black prints aligns with Type IV, while magenta and yellow prints correspond to Type II, as described by Giles. Notably, cyan and black inks demonstrated resistance toward fading at the end of the exposure, with a minor exception. The investigation involved curve fitting, revealing a consistent first-order kinetic model governing fading rates throughout the exposure time. High R2 values (0.96–0.99) and low root means square error values (0.02–0.08) are observed across all cases. While color fading involves diverse kinetics, the fading rate consistently adheres to a singular first-order kinetic path. The study highlights that yellow ink is most sensitive to light, followed by magenta ink. These findings offer valuable insights for optimizing packaging materials and ensuring product quality in industries reliant on printed materials. By mitigating color degradation, manufacturers can enhance the longevity and appeal of their products, reinforcing consumer trust and satisfaction. Furthermore, the effective utilization of PCA as a data analysis tool offers a valuable approach for future investigations in color science and related fields.

色彩退化是食品和药品包装以及印刷材料等各个领域亟待解决的问题,它会导致重要信息的丢失或失真。因此,了解包装印刷品的颜色如何随时间变化,对于产品质量评估和有效的产品生命周期管理至关重要。本研究的重点是调查广泛应用于医药和包装行业的吸塑箔上印刷品的颜色变化。为了评估褪色特性,通过凹版印刷工艺印刷的样品被置于氙气测试室(B-SUN)中。使用分光辐射计对印刷品进行了分光光度测量,并使用主成分分析法(PCA)对数据进行了分析。根据 Giles 的描述,青色和黑色印刷品的褪色性质符合类型 IV,而品红色和黄色印刷品则符合类型 II。值得注意的是,青色和黑色油墨在曝光结束时表现出抗褪色性,但有一个小例外。调查涉及曲线拟合,揭示了在整个曝光时间内控制褪色率的一致的一阶动力学模型。在所有情况下都能观察到较高的 R2 值(0.96-0.99)和较低的均方根误差值(0.02-0.08)。虽然褪色涉及不同的动力学原理,但褪色率始终遵循单一的一阶动力学路径。研究强调,黄色油墨对光最敏感,其次是品红色油墨。这些发现为依赖印刷材料的行业优化包装材料和确保产品质量提供了宝贵的见解。通过减少颜色退化,制造商可以提高产品的使用寿命和吸引力,增强消费者的信任度和满意度。此外,有效利用 PCA 作为数据分析工具,也为色彩科学及相关领域的未来研究提供了宝贵的方法。
{"title":"Study of color degradation in package prints: Analyzing kinetics with principal component analysis","authors":"Anirban Das,&nbsp;Mahasweta Mandal","doi":"10.1002/col.22924","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22924","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Color degradation is a pressing issue in various fields like food and medicine packaging, as well as printed materials, leading to a loss or distortion of vital information. Therefore, understanding how the color of package prints evolves over time is essential for product quality assessment and effective product life cycle management. This study focuses on investigating color changes in prints on blister foils, widely used in the medicine and packaging industries. To assess fading characteristics, samples printed via the gravure process have been exposed to a Xenon Test Chamber (B-SUN). Spectrophotometric measurements of the prints have been conducted using a spectroradiometer, and the data have been analyzed using principal component analysis (PCA). The fading nature of cyan and black prints aligns with Type IV, while magenta and yellow prints correspond to Type II, as described by Giles. Notably, cyan and black inks demonstrated resistance toward fading at the end of the exposure, with a minor exception. The investigation involved curve fitting, revealing a consistent first-order kinetic model governing fading rates throughout the exposure time. High <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> values (0.96–0.99) and low root means square error values (0.02–0.08) are observed across all cases. While color fading involves diverse kinetics, the fading rate consistently adheres to a singular first-order kinetic path. The study highlights that yellow ink is most sensitive to light, followed by magenta ink. These findings offer valuable insights for optimizing packaging materials and ensuring product quality in industries reliant on printed materials. By mitigating color degradation, manufacturers can enhance the longevity and appeal of their products, reinforcing consumer trust and satisfaction. Furthermore, the effective utilization of PCA as a data analysis tool offers a valuable approach for future investigations in color science and related fields.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 4","pages":"401-415"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140168442","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connecting the new to the past: The Color-Material-Finish design approach applied to the Fortezza da Basso 连接新与旧:应用于巴索广场的色彩-材料-饰面设计方法
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1002/col.22923
Francesca Valan, Pietro Paglierani

This article presents the Color-Material-Finish (CMF) project developed for the renovation of Fortezza da Basso in Florence, Italy. Fortezza da Basso—a large, fortified complex consisting of old and new buildings—is the main venue in Tuscany for international exhibitions and cultural activities. In 2010, the Municipality of Florence started its restoration, which is still ongoing. The goal is to renovate the Renaissance parts of the fortress and replace the 20th-century pavilions with environmentally sustainable buildings, harmonically integrated in the context. The renovation started with a project aimed at assessing the preservation status of the fortress. In 2016, the Municipality of Florence developed a body of design rules, the “Fortezza System,” dedicated to the design of new buildings within the fortress. A key role was assigned to CMF design: at first, to identify the fortress CMF identity; then, to develop CMF prescriptions to harmonically integrate the new constructions with the historic parts. The CMF approach required for such an ambitious plan also had to be flexible and with wide design margins to allow architects experiment innovative CMF languages. Such conflicting needs were met by designing a CMF approach based on Chromatic Areas, that is, usable continuous areas in the color space rather than on color charts based on lists of allowed historical colors. The article shows how the developed CMF approach can contribute to the full preservation of the Fortezza historical identity and at the same time increase the autonomy of architects in their material and color choices.

本文介绍了为翻新意大利佛罗伦萨巴索广场(Fortezza da Basso)而开发的色彩-材料-饰面(CMF)项目。巴索广场(Fortezza da Basso)是一个由新旧建筑组成的大型防御建筑群,是托斯卡纳区举办国际展览和文化活动的主要场所。2010 年,佛罗伦萨市政府开始对其进行修复,目前修复工作仍在进行中。目标是翻新要塞文艺复兴时期的部分,用环境可持续建筑取代 20 世纪的展馆,使其与周围环境和谐地融为一体。翻新工作始于一个旨在评估要塞保护状况的项目。2016 年,佛罗伦萨市政府制定了一套设计规则,即 "Fortezza 系统",专门用于设计要塞内的新建筑。CMF设计被委以重任:首先,确定要塞CMF的特性;然后,制定CMF规定,将新建筑与历史部分和谐地融合在一起。这样一个雄心勃勃的计划所需的 CMF 方法还必须具有灵活性和较大的设计余地,以便建筑师尝试创新的 CMF 语言。为了满足这些相互冲突的需求,我们设计了一种基于 "色域 "的 CMF 方法,即色彩空间中可用的连续区域,而不是基于允许使用的历史色彩列表的色彩图表。文章展示了所开发的 CMF 方法如何有助于全面保护堡垒广场的历史特征,同时提高建筑师在材料和色彩选择方面的自主性。
{"title":"Connecting the new to the past: The Color-Material-Finish design approach applied to the Fortezza da Basso","authors":"Francesca Valan,&nbsp;Pietro Paglierani","doi":"10.1002/col.22923","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22923","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This article presents the Color-Material-Finish (CMF) project developed for the renovation of Fortezza da Basso in Florence, Italy. Fortezza da Basso—a large, fortified complex consisting of old and new buildings—is the main venue in Tuscany for international exhibitions and cultural activities. In 2010, the Municipality of Florence started its restoration, which is still ongoing. The goal is to renovate the Renaissance parts of the fortress and replace the 20th-century pavilions with environmentally sustainable buildings, harmonically integrated in the context. The renovation started with a project aimed at assessing the preservation status of the fortress. In 2016, the Municipality of Florence developed a body of design rules, the “Fortezza System,” dedicated to the design of new buildings within the fortress. A key role was assigned to CMF design: at first, to identify the fortress CMF identity; then, to develop CMF prescriptions to harmonically integrate the new constructions with the historic parts. The CMF approach required for such an ambitious plan also had to be flexible and with wide design margins to allow architects experiment innovative CMF languages. Such conflicting needs were met by designing a CMF approach based on Chromatic Areas, that is, usable continuous areas in the color space rather than on color charts based on lists of allowed historical colors. The article shows how the developed CMF approach can contribute to the full preservation of the Fortezza historical identity and at the same time increase the autonomy of architects in their material and color choices.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 4","pages":"384-400"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140115222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relationship between brightness perception and skin color influenced by experimental method 受实验方法影响的亮度感知与肤色之间的关系
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1002/col.22921
Yuanyuan He, Hiromi Sato, Chanprapha Phuangsuwan, Kitirochna Rattanakasamsuk, Yoko Mizokami

Facial brightness perception is affected by skin color. It was reported that reddish skin was matched with a higher lightness scale stimulus than yellowish skin among Japanese observers. However, this trend is inconsistent among Thai, Korean, Chinese, and European observers, implying the influence factors, such as ethnicity, environment, and judgment criteria. In this study, we investigated the influence of the experimental method on facial brightness perception using test stimulus images with constant lightness and different hue angles. We examined two instructions (“Appearance match” vs. “Brightness match”) and the scale stimulus for matching (a uniform color patch vs. a face image). We further conducted an impression evaluation experiment of the whole face. The matching results from the Japanese showed a similar trend in both instructions and scale stimuli under most conditions, suggesting that matched brightness is not influenced by instruction and the complexity of scale stimuli. The impression evaluation results were similar for Thai, Japanese, and Chinese observers, with reddish faces being evaluated with higher scores in brightness. Our findings suggest the influence of experimental methods on facial brightness judgment. The holistic judgment of facial brightness would be universal among different East Asian groups, at least for the range of variation in skin color we used.

面部亮度感知受肤色影响。据报道,在日本观察者中,偏红的皮肤比偏黄的皮肤匹配更高的亮度量表刺激。然而,这一趋势在泰国、韩国、中国和欧洲的观察者中并不一致,这意味着存在种族、环境和判断标准等影响因素。在本研究中,我们使用具有恒定亮度和不同色调角度的测试刺激图像,研究了实验方法对面部亮度感知的影响。我们考察了两种指令("外观匹配 "与 "亮度匹配")和匹配的尺度刺激(统一色块与面部图像)。我们还对整个面部进行了印象评估实验。日本人的匹配结果显示,在大多数条件下,指令和比例刺激的趋势相似,这表明匹配亮度不受指令和比例刺激复杂程度的影响。泰国、日本和中国观察者的印象评价结果相似,红色人脸的亮度得分较高。我们的研究结果表明了实验方法对面部亮度判断的影响。至少在我们使用的肤色变化范围内,面部亮度的整体判断在不同的东亚群体中是通用的。
{"title":"Relationship between brightness perception and skin color influenced by experimental method","authors":"Yuanyuan He,&nbsp;Hiromi Sato,&nbsp;Chanprapha Phuangsuwan,&nbsp;Kitirochna Rattanakasamsuk,&nbsp;Yoko Mizokami","doi":"10.1002/col.22921","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22921","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Facial brightness perception is affected by skin color. It was reported that reddish skin was matched with a higher lightness scale stimulus than yellowish skin among Japanese observers. However, this trend is inconsistent among Thai, Korean, Chinese, and European observers, implying the influence factors, such as ethnicity, environment, and judgment criteria. In this study, we investigated the influence of the experimental method on facial brightness perception using test stimulus images with constant lightness and different hue angles. We examined two instructions (“Appearance match” vs. “Brightness match”) and the scale stimulus for matching (a uniform color patch vs. a face image). We further conducted an impression evaluation experiment of the whole face. The matching results from the Japanese showed a similar trend in both instructions and scale stimuli under most conditions, suggesting that matched brightness is not influenced by instruction and the complexity of scale stimuli. The impression evaluation results were similar for Thai, Japanese, and Chinese observers, with reddish faces being evaluated with higher scores in brightness. Our findings suggest the influence of experimental methods on facial brightness judgment. The holistic judgment of facial brightness would be universal among different East Asian groups, at least for the range of variation in skin color we used.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 4","pages":"360-373"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139956842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A study on the impressions induced by lipstick colors 关于口红颜色所引起的印象的研究
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1002/col.22922
Yu-An Wu, Shi-Min Gong, Wen-Yuan Lee

Owing to the need for beauty in modern society, cosmetics are essential to enhance femininity and attractiveness. Lipstick is effective in reinforcing femininity and increasing beauty. This study conducted a psychophysical experiment to investigate the association between lipstick colors and the impressions they create. Thirty-five observers were invited to participate in the experiment. They were asked to assess 20 lipstick colors on 23 impression scales. The results showed that the impressions created by the lipstick colors comprised three factors: charm, cuteness, and contemporary. Charm was correlated with a* and lightness value of lipstick color and the color difference between lipstick-skin colors. Cuteness was negatively correlated with the hue angle of the lipstick colors and lightness differences between lipstick-skin colors. Contemporary was correlated with chroma differences and negatively correlated with b* of lipstick color. These lipstick colors can be classified into three groups based on impression factors. The results showed that a* value of lipstick color and the color difference (E) between lipstick and skin color are identified to determine the grouping of lipstick colors.

由于现代社会对美的需求,化妆品对于提升女性魅力和吸引力至关重要。口红能有效强化女性气质,增加美感。本研究进行了一项心理物理实验,以调查口红颜色与其给人留下的印象之间的关联。35 名观察者应邀参加了实验。他们被要求在 23 个印象量表上对 20 种口红颜色进行评估。结果显示,口红颜色给人的印象包括三个因素:魅力、可爱和现代感。魅力与口红颜色的a*和亮度值以及口红与皮肤颜色之间的色差相关。可爱度与口红颜色的色相角度和口红-皮肤颜色之间的明度差异呈负相关。当代性与色度差异相关,与口红颜色的 b* 负相关。根据印象因素,这些口红颜色可分为三组。结果表明,口红颜色的 a* 值和口红与皮肤颜色之间的色差()可以确定口红颜色的分组。
{"title":"A study on the impressions induced by lipstick colors","authors":"Yu-An Wu,&nbsp;Shi-Min Gong,&nbsp;Wen-Yuan Lee","doi":"10.1002/col.22922","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22922","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Owing to the need for beauty in modern society, cosmetics are essential to enhance femininity and attractiveness. Lipstick is effective in reinforcing femininity and increasing beauty. This study conducted a psychophysical experiment to investigate the association between lipstick colors and the impressions they create. Thirty-five observers were invited to participate in the experiment. They were asked to assess 20 lipstick colors on 23 impression scales. The results showed that the impressions created by the lipstick colors comprised three factors: charm, cuteness, and contemporary. Charm was correlated with <i>a</i>* and lightness value of lipstick color and the color difference between lipstick-skin colors. Cuteness was negatively correlated with the hue angle of the lipstick colors and lightness differences between lipstick-skin colors. Contemporary was correlated with chroma differences and negatively correlated with <i>b</i>* of lipstick color. These lipstick colors can be classified into three groups based on impression factors. The results showed that <i>a</i>* value of lipstick color and the color difference (<span></span><math>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <mo>△</mo>\u0000 <mi>E</mi>\u0000 </mrow></math>) between lipstick and skin color are identified to determine the grouping of lipstick colors.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 4","pages":"374-383"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139945434","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring color concepts in physics education: Addressing common preconceptions among teachers-in-training 探索物理教育中的色彩概念:解决受训教师中常见的成见问题
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-01-22 DOI: 10.1002/col.22919
Guadalupe Martínez-Borreguero, Francisco L. Naranjo-Correa, Milagros Mateos Nuñez

In the field of physics education, the identification of students' preconceptions—especially regarding light and color—has been pivotal, as these foundational ideas often persist through higher education. Within teacher education, this is particularly crucial, as these early conceptions can deeply influence how future educators understand and teach these concepts, highlighting the need for an approach that weaves these initial understandings into a coherent scientific framework. Instructing students on color concepts reveals that many grapple with the fundamental optical principles. These initial understandings are integral to designing educational sequences that facilitate conceptual evolution, allowing for a deeper grasp of scientific ideas. This study aimed to examine and interpret the initial conceptions that aspiring primary and secondary educators hold about color and its optical fundamentals, while also developing and applying virtual teaching resources to promote a conceptual change about these foundational concepts. A quasi-experimental approach was employed, comprising control and experimental groups, and utilizing both qualitative and quantitative methods for a comprehensive analysis. Engaging 409 trainee teachers through non-probabilistic sampling, the study stratified participants by degree program. These groupings facilitated a comparative evaluation of the pedagogical efficacy of the educational resources developed. The study utilized an online assessment comprising single-answer multiple-choice questions, grounded in prior research, which was psychometrically validated. Educational tools were then developed and implemented to foster a deeper understanding of color concepts among prospective teachers. Results analysis highlighted the existence of preconceptions about color among participants from diverse educational backgrounds. The interventions employed were effective in catalyzing a shift in conceptual understanding and in elevating the knowledge levels of the trainee teachers.

在物理教育领域,识别学生的先入之见--尤其是关于光和颜色的先入之见--至关重要,因为这些基础观念往往会在高等教育中持续存在。在师范教育中,这一点尤为重要,因为这些早期概念会深刻影响未来教育者如何理解和教授这些概念,这就凸显了将这些初步理解编织成一个连贯的科学框架的方法的必要性。在指导学生学习色彩概念时,会发现许多学生都在努力学习基本的光学原理。这些初步理解对于设计促进概念演化的教育序列是不可或缺的,可以让学生更深入地掌握科学思想。本研究旨在研究和解释有抱负的中小学教育工作者对颜色及其光学基本原理所持有的初步概念,同时开发和应用虚拟教学资源,以促进这些基础概念的观念转变。研究采用了准实验方法,包括对照组和实验组,并利用定性和定量方法进行综合分析。该研究通过非概率抽样的方式吸引了 409 名受训教师参与,并按学位课程对参与者进行了分层。这些分组有利于对所开发教育资源的教学效果进行比较评估。研究采用了一种在线评估方法,包括单选题和多选题,该方法以先前的研究为基础,并经过心理测试验证。随后开发并实施了教育工具,以促进未来教师加深对色彩概念的理解。结果分析显示,来自不同教育背景的参与者对肤色存在成见。所采用的干预措施有效地促进了概念理解的转变,并提高了受训教师的知识水平。
{"title":"Exploring color concepts in physics education: Addressing common preconceptions among teachers-in-training","authors":"Guadalupe Martínez-Borreguero,&nbsp;Francisco L. Naranjo-Correa,&nbsp;Milagros Mateos Nuñez","doi":"10.1002/col.22919","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22919","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the field of physics education, the identification of students' preconceptions—especially regarding light and color—has been pivotal, as these foundational ideas often persist through higher education. Within teacher education, this is particularly crucial, as these early conceptions can deeply influence how future educators understand and teach these concepts, highlighting the need for an approach that weaves these initial understandings into a coherent scientific framework. Instructing students on color concepts reveals that many grapple with the fundamental optical principles. These initial understandings are integral to designing educational sequences that facilitate conceptual evolution, allowing for a deeper grasp of scientific ideas. This study aimed to examine and interpret the initial conceptions that aspiring primary and secondary educators hold about color and its optical fundamentals, while also developing and applying virtual teaching resources to promote a conceptual change about these foundational concepts. A quasi-experimental approach was employed, comprising control and experimental groups, and utilizing both qualitative and quantitative methods for a comprehensive analysis. Engaging 409 trainee teachers through non-probabilistic sampling, the study stratified participants by degree program. These groupings facilitated a comparative evaluation of the pedagogical efficacy of the educational resources developed. The study utilized an online assessment comprising single-answer multiple-choice questions, grounded in prior research, which was psychometrically validated. Educational tools were then developed and implemented to foster a deeper understanding of color concepts among prospective teachers. Results analysis highlighted the existence of preconceptions about color among participants from diverse educational backgrounds. The interventions employed were effective in catalyzing a shift in conceptual understanding and in elevating the knowledge levels of the trainee teachers.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 3","pages":"339-355"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139518675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Color sorting and color term evolution 颜色分类和颜色术语演变
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.1002/col.22918
Delwin T. Lindsey, Angela M. Brown, Aimee N. Violette, Ryan Lange, Prutha S. Deshpande

When participants sort color samples into piles, Boster showed that their color groupings can resemble the “stages” of Kay and McDaniel's model of color term evolution. Boster concluded that both the unfolding of color piles in a sequential color sorting task and the unfolding of color terms according to Kay and McDaniel's model reveal how human beings understand color. If this is correct, then: (1) pile sorts should be reasonably robust across variations in the palette of colors to be sorted, as long as the palette contains good examples of Berlin and Kay's universal color categories, and (2) pile-sorting should be more related to lexical effects and less related to perceptual processes governed by similarity judgments alone. We report three studies on English speakers and Somali speakers (Study 1 only), where participants sorted colors into 2…6 piles. The three studies used varying numbers of palette colors (25, 30, or 145 colors) and varying chromaticity schemes (mainly hue, widely-separated in hue and lightness, or densely distributed at high chroma). We compared human sorting behavior to Kay and McDaniel's model and to the “optimal” patterns of color sorting predicted by Regier's well-formedness statistic, which quantifies the perceived similarity between colors. Neither hypothesis is confirmed by the results of our studies. We propose that color sorts are determined by pragmatic influences based on heuristics that are inspired by the palette of colors that are available and the task that the viewer is asked to perform.

当参与者将颜色样本分类堆放时,博斯特发现他们的颜色分组与凯和麦克丹尼尔的颜色术语演变模型中的 "阶段 "相似。博斯特的结论是,在有序的颜色分类任务中,颜色堆的展开以及根据凯和麦克丹尼尔的模型,颜色术语的展开都揭示了人类是如何理解颜色的。如果这一点是正确的,那么:(1) 只要调色板中包含柏林和凯的通用颜色类别的良好示例,那么堆叠排序在待排序颜色调色板的变化中应具有合理的稳健性;(2) 堆叠排序应更多地与词汇效应相关,而较少地与仅受相似性判断支配的知觉过程相关。我们报告了对讲英语和索马里语的人进行的三项研究(仅研究 1),参与者将颜色分成 2...6 堆。这三项研究使用了不同数量的调色板颜色(25、30 或 145 种颜色)和不同的色度方案(以色相为主、色相和明度相差较大或高色度密集分布)。我们将人类的排序行为与 Kay 和 McDaniel 的模型进行了比较,并与 Regier 的 "良好形成性 "统计所预测的 "最佳 "颜色排序模式进行了比较。我们的研究结果证实了这两种假设。我们认为,颜色排序是由实用性影响因素决定的,而实用性影响因素是受可用颜色调色板和要求观众完成的任务启发的启发式方法。
{"title":"Color sorting and color term evolution","authors":"Delwin T. Lindsey,&nbsp;Angela M. Brown,&nbsp;Aimee N. Violette,&nbsp;Ryan Lange,&nbsp;Prutha S. Deshpande","doi":"10.1002/col.22918","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22918","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When participants sort color samples into piles, Boster showed that their color groupings can resemble the “stages” of Kay and McDaniel's model of color term evolution. Boster concluded that both the unfolding of color piles in a sequential color sorting task and the unfolding of color terms according to Kay and McDaniel's model reveal how human beings understand color. If this is correct, then: (1) pile sorts should be reasonably robust across variations in the palette of colors to be sorted, as long as the palette contains good examples of Berlin and Kay's universal color categories, and (2) pile-sorting should be more related to lexical effects and less related to perceptual processes governed by similarity judgments alone. We report three studies on English speakers and Somali speakers (Study 1 only), where participants sorted colors into 2…6 piles. The three studies used varying numbers of palette colors (25, 30, or 145 colors) and varying chromaticity schemes (mainly hue, widely-separated in hue and lightness, or densely distributed at high chroma). We compared human sorting behavior to Kay and McDaniel's model and to the “optimal” patterns of color sorting predicted by Regier's well-formedness statistic, which quantifies the perceived similarity between colors. Neither hypothesis is confirmed by the results of our studies. We propose that color sorts are determined by pragmatic influences based on heuristics that are inspired by the palette of colors that are available and the task that the viewer is asked to perform.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 3","pages":"318-338"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/col.22918","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139094566","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of team affiliation on color-valence associations 团队隶属关系对颜色-价值联想的影响
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-19 DOI: 10.1002/col.22915
Declan Forrester, Heather Winskel, Mitchell Longstaff

This study aims to empirically test whether identifying as a supporter of either New South Wales (NSW) or Queensland (QLD) rugby league teams influences the extent that their respective team colors blue and maroon are associated with positively and negatively valenced words. We used a valence categorization experiment and affective rating task (valence and preference) to investigate if team affiliation and shared ingroup experience influenced affective associations with team colors. NSW supporters were faster and more accurate when categorizing positive words presented in blue than maroon font and negative words in maroon than blue font. While QLD supporters did not significantly differ when categorizing words in either blue or maroon, they rated blue and maroon equally positively in contrast to the NSW supporters. Results from this study give us greater insights into how color-valence associations can be formed through subcultural ingroup affiliations.

本研究旨在通过实证方法检验,作为新南威尔士(NSW)或昆士兰(QLD)橄榄球联盟球队的支持者,是否会影响蓝色和栗色这两种球队颜色与积极和消极情感词的关联程度。我们使用了情绪分类实验和情绪评级任务(情绪和偏好)来研究球队归属和共同的群体经验是否会影响对球队颜色的情绪联想。新南威尔士州的支持者在对蓝色字体比栗色字体的正面词语和栗色字体比蓝色字体的负面词语进行分类时,速度更快、更准确。而昆士兰州的支持者在对蓝色或栗色字体的词语进行分类时没有明显差异,但他们对蓝色和栗色字体的评价同样积极,与新南威尔士州的支持者形成鲜明对比。这项研究的结果让我们更深入地了解了色彩与价值的关联是如何通过亚文化中的群体归属而形成的。
{"title":"Effects of team affiliation on color-valence associations","authors":"Declan Forrester,&nbsp;Heather Winskel,&nbsp;Mitchell Longstaff","doi":"10.1002/col.22915","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22915","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study aims to empirically test whether identifying as a supporter of either New South Wales (NSW) or Queensland (QLD) rugby league teams influences the extent that their respective team colors blue and maroon are associated with positively and negatively valenced words. We used a valence categorization experiment and affective rating task (valence and preference) to investigate if team affiliation and shared ingroup experience influenced affective associations with team colors. NSW supporters were faster and more accurate when categorizing positive words presented in blue than maroon font and negative words in maroon than blue font. While QLD supporters did not significantly differ when categorizing words in either blue or maroon, they rated blue and maroon equally positively in contrast to the NSW supporters. Results from this study give us greater insights into how color-valence associations can be formed through subcultural ingroup affiliations.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 3","pages":"306-317"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/col.22915","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138816445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of an automatic conversion method for the conversion of a CIE-XYZ color system to a DIN color system 开发将 CIE-XYZ 色彩系统转换为 DIN 色彩系统的自动转换方法
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1002/col.22916
Hongying Yang, Nuoya Feng, Jingjing Zhang, Ge Zhang, Yubin Yang

The DIN color system is a visually uniform system adopted as the standard by several countries in central Europe. Currently, the conversion method from CIE-XYZ color system to DIN color system consists of the DIN 6164 method combined with the manual reading method. However, the DIN 6164 method cannot be used for the edges of a chromaticity diagram. The manual reading method can read all areas in a chromaticity diagram; however, it is too inefficient to convert large quantities of data. This study builds on the manual reading method to create a new method entitled the program reading method. Upon comparing the relative error, absolute error and conversion times of all methods, it was determined that the program reading method achieved higher accuracy than that of the manual reading method. The maximum relative error of the program reading method was 3.24%; however, the conversion time for each sample was 0.07 s, which was 3.5 times slower than that of the DIN 6164 method. Based on these findings, this study proposes an automatic conversion method that combines the strengths of both approaches: the DIN 6164 method to be used for interpolable areas and the program reading method to be used for areas that cannot be interpolated. The automatic conversion method outlined in this study provides an efficient, precise and fast way to convert a CIE-XYZ color system to a DIN color system.

DIN 颜色系统是一种视觉上统一的系统,被中欧几个国家作为标准采用。目前,从 CIE-XYZ 颜色系统到 DIN 颜色系统的转换方法包括 DIN 6164 方法和手动读取方法。然而,DIN 6164 方法不能用于色度图的边缘。手动读取法可以读取色度图中的所有区域,但在转换大量数据时效率太低。本研究以手动读图法为基础,创建了一种名为程序读图法的新方法。在比较了所有方法的相对误差、绝对误差和转换时间后,确定程序读图法比手动读图法获得了更高的精确度。程序读数法的最大相对误差为 3.24%,但每个样本的转换时间为 0.07 秒,比 DIN 6164 方法慢 3.5 倍。基于这些发现,本研究提出了一种结合两种方法优点的自动转换方法:DIN 6164 方法用于可内插的区域,而程序读取方法用于无法内插的区域。本研究中概述的自动转换方法为将 CIE-XYZ 颜色系统转换为 DIN 颜色系统提供了一种高效、精确和快速的方法。
{"title":"Development of an automatic conversion method for the conversion of a CIE-XYZ color system to a DIN color system","authors":"Hongying Yang,&nbsp;Nuoya Feng,&nbsp;Jingjing Zhang,&nbsp;Ge Zhang,&nbsp;Yubin Yang","doi":"10.1002/col.22916","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22916","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The DIN color system is a visually uniform system adopted as the standard by several countries in central Europe. Currently, the conversion method from CIE-XYZ color system to DIN color system consists of the DIN 6164 method combined with the manual reading method. However, the DIN 6164 method cannot be used for the edges of a chromaticity diagram. The manual reading method can read all areas in a chromaticity diagram; however, it is too inefficient to convert large quantities of data. This study builds on the manual reading method to create a new method entitled the program reading method. Upon comparing the relative error, absolute error and conversion times of all methods, it was determined that the program reading method achieved higher accuracy than that of the manual reading method. The maximum relative error of the program reading method was 3.24%; however, the conversion time for each sample was 0.07 s, which was 3.5 times slower than that of the DIN 6164 method. Based on these findings, this study proposes an automatic conversion method that combines the strengths of both approaches: the DIN 6164 method to be used for interpolable areas and the program reading method to be used for areas that cannot be interpolated. The automatic conversion method outlined in this study provides an efficient, precise and fast way to convert a CIE-XYZ color system to a DIN color system.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 3","pages":"292-305"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138743537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Building a metric of color reproduction difference by combining multiple observers in a modular online experiment 在模块化在线实验中结合多个观察者建立色彩再现差异度量标准
IF 1.4 3区 工程技术 Q4 CHEMISTRY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-09 DOI: 10.1002/col.22913
Gregory High, Peter Nussbaum, Phil Green

A web-hosted online experiment was previously developed to find the visual difference between four reproduction gamuts using direct magnitude estimation (Proc. IS&T 29th Color and Imaging Conf, 2021:317–322). In order to increase the size of the data set, but without overburdening observers, a modular approach was adopted. The original methodology was therefore extended across 10 linked sub-experiments to make comparisons between some 36 gamuts, which were designed to exhibit a variety of different gamut shapes, contrast ratios, and substrate colors within the constraints of a desktop display. In addition to each set of test images, a common normalization set was included in all sub-experiments in order to adjust each observer's choice of modulus to a global average observer, and thus combine the results into a larger data set. Finally, an interval scale was inferred from the normalized magnitude data using a categorical judgment approach to calculate scale values. The fitted data revealed a power function close to a square-root between the interval and magnitude scales.

以前曾开发过一个网络托管在线实验,利用直接幅度估算来发现四种再现色域之间的视觉差异(Proc. IS&T 29th Color and Imaging Conf, 2021:317-322)。为了增加数据集的规模,但又不给观察者造成过重负担,我们采用了模块化方法。因此,最初的方法被扩展到 10 个相互关联的子实验中,对大约 36 种色域进行比较,这些色域的设计目的是在桌面显示器的限制条件下展示各种不同的色域形状、对比度和基底颜色。除了每组测试图像外,所有子实验还包括一个共同的归一化集,以便将每个观察者选择的模量调整为一个总体平均观察者的模量,从而将结果合并为一个更大的数据集。最后,使用分类判断法计算标度值,从归一化的幅度数据中推断出区间标度。拟合数据显示,区间尺度和幅度尺度之间存在一个接近平方根的幂函数。
{"title":"Building a metric of color reproduction difference by combining multiple observers in a modular online experiment","authors":"Gregory High,&nbsp;Peter Nussbaum,&nbsp;Phil Green","doi":"10.1002/col.22913","DOIUrl":"10.1002/col.22913","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A web-hosted online experiment was previously developed to find the visual difference between four reproduction gamuts using direct magnitude estimation (Proc. IS&amp;T 29th Color and Imaging Conf, 2021:317–322). In order to increase the size of the data set, but without overburdening observers, a modular approach was adopted. The original methodology was therefore extended across 10 linked sub-experiments to make comparisons between some 36 gamuts, which were designed to exhibit a variety of different gamut shapes, contrast ratios, and substrate colors within the constraints of a desktop display. In addition to each set of test images, a common normalization set was included in all sub-experiments in order to adjust each observer's choice of modulus to a global average observer, and thus combine the results into a larger data set. Finally, an interval scale was inferred from the normalized magnitude data using a categorical judgment approach to calculate scale values. The fitted data revealed a power function close to a square-root between the interval and magnitude scales.</p>","PeriodicalId":10459,"journal":{"name":"Color Research and Application","volume":"49 2","pages":"272-287"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/col.22913","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138559547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"工程技术","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Color Research and Application
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1