首页 > 最新文献

Contemporary Educational Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Feelings on feedback: Children’s emotional responses during mathematics problem solving 反馈感受:儿童在数学问题解决过程中的情绪反应
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102209
Megan Merrick , Emily R. Fyfe

Theories of learning emphasize the importance of both the cognitive and affective state of the learner. The current study focused on children’s affective reactions to corrective feedback during mathematics problem solving. Eighty-seven elementary school children (M age = 7.6 years, 41% female, 68% White) solved mathematical equivalence problems during an online video call and received trial-by-trial feedback on their answers. Trained researchers used children’s facial expressions, tone of voice, and verbal statements to quantify their positive and negative affect on each trial. Overall, children tended to express more positive affect than negative affect. However, negative affect was more prominent when the child was incorrect and received negative feedback, and higher negative affect was associated with lower accuracy and lower persistence on the task. These results provide novel empirical evidence for the role of emotions during children’s STEM learning in a non-evaluative context.

学习理论强调学习者的认知和情感状态的重要性。目前的研究集中在儿童在数学问题解决过程中对纠正反馈的情感反应上。87名小学生(M年龄=7.6岁,41%为女性,68%为白人)在一次在线视频通话中解决了数学等价问题,并接受了对其答案的逐个试验的反馈。经过培训的研究人员使用儿童的面部表情、语调和言语陈述来量化他们对每次试验的积极和消极影响。总的来说,孩子们倾向于表达更多的积极情绪,而不是消极情绪。然而,当孩子不正确并收到负面反馈时,负面影响更为突出,负面影响越大,任务的准确性越低,持续性越低。这些结果为情绪在儿童非评价性STEM学习中的作用提供了新的经验证据。
{"title":"Feelings on feedback: Children’s emotional responses during mathematics problem solving","authors":"Megan Merrick ,&nbsp;Emily R. Fyfe","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102209","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102209","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Theories of learning emphasize the importance of both the cognitive and affective state of the learner. The current study focused on children’s affective reactions to corrective feedback during mathematics problem solving. Eighty-seven elementary school children (<em>M</em> age = 7.6 years, 41% female, 68% White) solved mathematical equivalence problems during an online video call and received trial-by-trial feedback on their answers. Trained researchers used children’s facial expressions, tone of voice, and verbal statements to quantify their positive and negative affect on each trial. Overall, children tended to express more positive affect than negative affect. However, negative affect was more prominent when the child was incorrect and received negative feedback, and higher negative affect was associated with lower accuracy and lower persistence on the task. These results provide novel empirical evidence for the role of emotions during children’s STEM learning in a non-evaluative context.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10420002/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10005472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Profiles of afterschool motivations: A situated expectancy-value approach 放学后动机的概况:一种情境预期价值方法
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102197
Ashlee L. Sjogren , Kristy A. Robinson , Alison C. Koenka

Despite the benefits of afterschool programs, we know very little about what motivates adolescents to attend and what costs they might associate with doing so. Situated expectancy-value theory (SEVT) proposes that expectancy for success, value, and cost perceptions are motivational beliefs that are important precursors to students’ engagement in such programs, and thus may shape the extent to which students can benefit from them. Accordingly, we examined profiles of expectancy, value, and cost beliefs associated with afterschool program participation in a sample of middle school students (N = 197) in an urban context. We then examined profiles for their relations to student demographics (gender, grade, race/ethnicity), achievement (English and Math grades), and program attendance. Latent profile analyses yielded three profiles: a moderate-low mixed motivation profile, a high cost and mixed motivation profile, and a positively motivated with moderate effort cost profile, thus uniquely contributing to the literature by describing the nature and incidence of how multiple motivational beliefs co-occur among groups of students in afterschool spaces. Subsequent analyses revealed that the positively motivated with moderate effort cost profile was associated with higher program attendance rates than the other two profiles. These results extend the theoretical knowledge base by exploring students’ expectancies, values, and costs in an informal educational context and have important implications for afterschool educators and policymakers.

尽管课后项目有好处,但我们对是什么激励青少年参加以及他们可能为此付出的代价知之甚少。情境预期价值理论(SEVT)提出,对成功的预期、价值和成本感知是动机信念,是学生参与此类项目的重要前兆,从而可以影响学生从中受益的程度。因此,我们在城市背景下对中学生(N=197)的样本进行了预期、价值和成本信念的调查。然后,我们检查了他们与学生人口统计(性别、年级、种族/民族)、成绩(英语和数学成绩)和项目出勤率的关系。潜在档案分析产生了三个档案:中等-低混合动机档案、高成本-混合动机档案和积极动机-中等努力成本档案,因此通过描述多种动机信念如何在放学后空间中在学生群体中共同发生的性质和发生率,对文献做出了独特的贡献。随后的分析表明,与其他两种情况相比,积极主动、努力成本适中的情况与更高的项目出勤率有关。这些结果通过探索学生在非正规教育背景下的期望、价值观和成本,扩展了理论知识库,对课后教育工作者和政策制定者具有重要意义。
{"title":"Profiles of afterschool motivations: A situated expectancy-value approach","authors":"Ashlee L. Sjogren ,&nbsp;Kristy A. Robinson ,&nbsp;Alison C. Koenka","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102197","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102197","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite the benefits of afterschool programs, we know very little about what motivates adolescents to attend and what costs they might associate with doing so. Situated expectancy-value theory (SEVT) proposes that expectancy for success, value, and cost perceptions are motivational beliefs that are important precursors to students’ engagement in such programs, and thus may shape the extent to which students can benefit from them. Accordingly, we examined profiles of expectancy, value, and cost beliefs associated with afterschool program participation in a sample of middle school students (<em>N</em> = 197) in an urban context. We then examined profiles for their relations to student demographics (gender, grade, race/ethnicity), achievement (English and Math grades), and program attendance. Latent profile analyses yielded three profiles: a <em>moderate-low mixed motivation</em> profile, a <em>high cost and mixed motivation profile</em>, and a <em>positively motivated with moderate effort cost</em> profile, thus uniquely contributing to the literature by describing the nature and incidence of how multiple motivational beliefs co-occur among groups of students in afterschool spaces. Subsequent analyses revealed that the <em>positively motivated with moderate effort cost</em> profile was associated with higher program attendance rates than the other two profiles. These results extend the theoretical knowledge base by exploring students’ expectancies, values, and costs in an informal educational context and have important implications for afterschool educators and policymakers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49813113","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What is the effect of talking heads in educational videos with different types of narrated slides? 在带有不同类型解说幻灯片的教育视频中,会说话的人有什么效果?
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102207
Christina Sondermann, Martin Merkt

Video-based learning plays an increasingly important role and thus the optimal design of video-based learning materials attracts the attention of scientists and practitioners alike. In this context, producers of educational videos often include a talking head in their videos, although theory (e.g., cognitive theory of multimedia learning) also suggests potential disadvantages for this format. Since talking heads attract a lot of visual attention, further empirical research is necessary to investigate whether a talking head can hinder learning, especially presented next to graphic-based learning content. To address this research gap, we conducted two online experiments to investigate the effects of a talking head in educational videos with narrated slides (short slideshow lectures) on learning outcomes (i.e., factual knowledge acquisition) and participants’ subjective ratings of the learning material (e.g., perceived learning). In Experiment 1 (N = 96), we varied whether the instructor’s talking head was present or absent in the videos as a between-factor, and whether the visual content on the slides was graphic-based (pictures, diagrams, maps) or text-based (bullet points) as a within-factor (slide type). In Experiment 2 (N = 184), we additionally varied as a between-factor whether the contents appeared sequentially or statically all at once (presentation type). Our results showed that the talking head did not affect learning outcomes, regardless of slide type and presentation type of the videos suggesting that the inclusion of a talking head offers neither clear advantages nor disadvantages. Potential explanations for the findings and directions for future research are discussed.

视频学习在教学中扮演着越来越重要的角色,因此视频学习材料的优化设计受到了科学家和实践者的广泛关注。在这种情况下,教育视频的制作者通常在他们的视频中包括一个说话的头,尽管理论(例如,多媒体学习的认知理论)也表明这种格式的潜在缺点。由于说话头吸引了大量的视觉注意力,因此需要进一步的实证研究来调查说话头是否会阻碍学习,特别是在基于图形的学习内容旁边。为了解决这一研究缺口,我们进行了两个在线实验,以调查在带有叙述幻灯片(短幻灯片讲座)的教育视频中说话的头对学习成果(即事实知识获取)和参与者对学习材料的主观评分(例如感知学习)的影响。在实验1 (N = 96)中,我们改变了讲师说话的头是否出现在视频中作为中间因素,以及幻灯片上的视觉内容是基于图形的(图片,图表,地图)还是基于文本的(项目符号)作为内因素(幻灯片类型)。在实验2 (N = 184)中,我们还作为一个中间因素改变了内容是顺序出现还是静态出现(呈现类型)。我们的研究结果表明,无论幻灯片类型和视频演示类型如何,说话头都不会影响学习结果,这表明加入说话头既没有明显的优势,也没有明显的劣势。对研究结果的可能解释和未来的研究方向进行了讨论。
{"title":"What is the effect of talking heads in educational videos with different types of narrated slides?","authors":"Christina Sondermann,&nbsp;Martin Merkt","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102207","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102207","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Video-based learning plays an increasingly important role and thus the optimal design of video-based learning materials attracts the attention of scientists and practitioners alike. In this context, producers of educational videos often include a talking head in their videos, although theory (e.g., cognitive theory of multimedia learning) also suggests potential disadvantages for this format. Since talking heads attract a lot of visual attention, further empirical research is necessary to investigate whether a talking head can hinder learning, especially presented next to graphic-based learning content. To address this research gap, we conducted two online experiments to investigate the effects of a talking head in educational videos with narrated slides (short slideshow lectures) on learning outcomes (i.e., factual knowledge acquisition) and participants’ subjective ratings of the learning material (e.g., perceived learning). In Experiment<!--> <!-->1 (<em>N</em> = 96), we varied whether the instructor’s talking head was present or absent in the videos as a between-factor, and whether the visual content on the slides was graphic-based (pictures, diagrams, maps) or text-based (bullet points) as a within-factor (slide type). In Experiment<!--> <!-->2 (<em>N</em> = 184), we additionally varied as a between-factor whether the contents appeared sequentially or statically all at once (presentation type). Our results showed that the talking head did not affect learning outcomes, regardless of slide type and presentation type of the videos suggesting that the inclusion of a talking head offers neither clear advantages nor disadvantages. Potential explanations for the findings and directions for future research are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43493791","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The predictive role of early childhood dysregulation profile on the parallel growth trajectories of reading and math performance across elementary and middle school 儿童早期调节障碍对中小学阅读和数学成绩平行成长轨迹的预测作用
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102200
Qinxin Shi , Florina Erbeli , Marianne Rice , Jonathan E. Butner

This study examined the heterogeneity in the co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance (i.e., parallel changes in the initial scores and growth patterns) and identified the number, size and shape of the co-developmental trajectory across elementary and middle schools. In addition, this investigation focused on how an early childhood dysregulation profile (DP; indexed by a high co-occurrence of emotional, behavior, and attention problems) was associated with distinct co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance. Specifically, we examined whether early childhood DP level can predict (a) membership assignment into each distinct co-development group and (b) variability in initial scores and changes in growth with each distinct co-development group. Participants were 784 academically at-risk students (47 % girls) predominantly from low socioeconomic status families who were recruited in first grade (Mean age = 6.57 years) and followed annually through the final year of middle school (ninth grade). Results revealed two distinct co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance, including (a) a lower initial reading (higher increasing) and higher initial math (lower increasing) class (85.3 %) and (b) a lower initial math (higher increasing) and higher initial reading (lower increasing) class (14.7 %). Our results provided evidence for the compensatory pattern of co-developmental trajectories, indicating initial lower skills grow at a faster rate than the initial high. Further, early childhood DP was not associated with the membership assignment for these two distinct classes, which means that regardless of children’s early DP level, they have equal chances to be assigned to each of the classes. However, children with higher parent and teacher-reported DP in first grade demonstrated lower initial scores and a slower improvement rate in both classes after controlling for kindergarten literacy skills, gender, ethnicity, intelligence, socioeconomic status, and grade retention. Our study findings demonstrated (a) substantial heterogeneity in the co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance across elementary and middle school ages; and (b) the importance of promoting self-regulation beginning in early childhood, especially for academically at-risk children in families facing economic challenges.

本研究考察了阅读和数学成绩共同发展轨迹的异质性(即初始分数和成长模式的平行变化),并确定了小学和中学共同发展轨迹的数量、大小和形状。此外,本研究的重点是如何早期儿童失调档案(DP;(情绪、行为和注意力问题的高度共现)与阅读和数学表现的明显共同发展轨迹相关。具体来说,我们研究了幼儿DP水平是否可以预测(a)每个不同的共同发展组的成员分配和(b)初始分数的可变性和每个不同的共同发展组的生长变化。参与者是784名学业上有风险的学生(47%是女孩),主要来自低社会经济地位的家庭,他们在一年级时被招募(平均年龄= 6.57岁),每年随访一次,直到中学的最后一年(九年级)。结果显示,阅读和数学成绩有两种明显的共同发展轨迹,包括(a)较低的初始阅读(较高的增长)和较高的初始数学(较低的增长)类别(85.3%)和(b)较低的初始数学(较高的增长)和较高的初始阅读(较低的增长)类别(14.7%)。我们的研究结果为共同发展轨迹的补偿模式提供了证据,表明最初的低技能比最初的高技能增长得更快。此外,幼儿DP与这两个不同班级的成员分配无关,这意味着无论儿童的早期DP水平如何,他们都有平等的机会被分配到每个班级。然而,在控制幼儿园识字技能、性别、种族、智力、社会经济地位和成绩保留后,父母和老师报告DP较高的一年级儿童的初始分数较低,两个班级的进步速度较慢。我们的研究结果表明:(a)阅读和数学成绩的共同发展轨迹在小学和中学年龄段存在显著的异质性;(b)从儿童早期开始促进自我调节的重要性,特别是对于面临经济挑战的家庭中学业上有风险的儿童。
{"title":"The predictive role of early childhood dysregulation profile on the parallel growth trajectories of reading and math performance across elementary and middle school","authors":"Qinxin Shi ,&nbsp;Florina Erbeli ,&nbsp;Marianne Rice ,&nbsp;Jonathan E. Butner","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102200","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102200","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examined the heterogeneity in the co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance (i.e., parallel changes in the initial scores and growth patterns) and identified the number, size and shape of the co-developmental trajectory across elementary and middle schools. In addition, this investigation focused on how an early childhood dysregulation profile (DP; indexed by a high co-occurrence of emotional, behavior, and attention problems) was associated with distinct co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance. Specifically, we examined whether early childhood DP level can predict (a) membership assignment into each distinct co-development group and (b) variability in initial scores and changes in growth with each distinct co-development group. Participants were 784 academically at-risk students (47 % girls) predominantly from low socioeconomic status families who were recruited in first grade (Mean age = 6.57 years) and followed annually through the final year of middle school (ninth grade). Results revealed two distinct co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance, including (a) a lower initial reading (higher increasing) and higher initial math (lower increasing) class (85.3 %) and (b) a lower initial math (higher increasing) and higher initial reading (lower increasing) class (14.7 %). Our results provided evidence for the compensatory pattern of co-developmental trajectories, indicating initial <em>lower</em> skills grow at a faster rate than the initial high. Further, early childhood DP was not associated with the membership assignment for these two distinct classes, which means that regardless of children’s early DP level, they have equal chances to be assigned to each of the classes. However, children with higher parent and teacher-reported DP in first grade demonstrated lower initial scores and a slower improvement rate in both classes after controlling for kindergarten literacy skills, gender, ethnicity, intelligence, socioeconomic status, and grade retention. Our study findings demonstrated (a) substantial heterogeneity in the co-developmental trajectories of reading and math performance across elementary and middle school ages; and (b) the importance of promoting self-regulation beginning in early childhood, especially for academically at-risk children in families facing economic challenges.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45109440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Measuring teachers’ momentary affect: An exploratory experience sampling study 教师瞬时情感的测量:一项探索性经验抽样研究
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102205
Kristabel Stark , Nathan Jones , Eric Camburn , Lindsey Kaler

As researchers and school leaders increasingly recognize the importance of teachers’ emotions for both teachers and students, it is imperative that researchers document teachers’ affective experiences using ecologically valid methodologies. In this study, we use the experience sampling method to explore the momentary emotions of 238 teachers in two suburban school districts in the Northeast region of the United States. Using 1443 momentary affective reports, we report patterns in affect that emerged across the full sample of teachers, including the types of emotions teachers experienced most frequently and most intensely, as well as variance in momentary affective experiences, both within and across teachers. We consider ways in which professional role, professional activity, and affective appraisals relate to teachers’ momentary affective experiences, and conclude with a discussion of implications for future research and practice.

随着研究人员和学校领导越来越认识到教师情感对教师和学生的重要性,研究人员必须使用生态有效的方法来记录教师的情感体验。本研究采用经验抽样的方法,对美国东北地区两个郊区学区238名教师的瞬间情绪进行了调查。使用1443份瞬间情感报告,我们报告了整个教师样本中出现的情感模式,包括教师最频繁和最强烈的情感类型,以及教师内部和教师之间瞬间情感体验的差异。我们考虑了专业角色、专业活动和情感评价与教师瞬间情感体验的关系,并讨论了对未来研究和实践的影响。
{"title":"Measuring teachers’ momentary affect: An exploratory experience sampling study","authors":"Kristabel Stark ,&nbsp;Nathan Jones ,&nbsp;Eric Camburn ,&nbsp;Lindsey Kaler","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102205","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102205","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As researchers and school leaders increasingly recognize the importance of teachers’ emotions for both teachers and students, it is imperative that researchers document teachers’ affective experiences using ecologically valid methodologies. In this study, we use the experience sampling method to explore the momentary emotions of 238 teachers in two suburban school districts in the Northeast region of the United States. Using 1443 momentary affective reports, we report patterns in affect that emerged across the full sample of teachers, including the types of emotions teachers experienced most frequently and most intensely, as well as variance in momentary affective experiences, both within and across teachers. We consider ways in which professional role, professional activity, and affective appraisals relate to teachers’ momentary affective experiences, and conclude with a discussion of implications for future research and practice.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45715532","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A bioecological perspective on mindset 心态的生物生态学视角
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102173
Matthew H. Kim

Mindset—beliefs about the malleability of intelligence—has been traditionally conceptualized as an individual-level construct. The present study adapts, proposes, and applies a bioecological model to examine how learners perceive the intelligence mindset beliefs possessed by important socializing agents—parents, teachers, and peers—and whether and how these perceived mindset beliefs shape individuals’ own understanding of intelligence and intelligence mindset. Inductive coding of focus group data from 42 undergraduate students in the United States revealed six themes reflecting the dynamic interplay between person and context: (1) Definitions of intelligence reflect resource, force, and demand characteristics, (2) Students largely endorse growth mindset beliefs, but not unconditionally, (3) Supports and supportive environments shape intelligence mindset, (4) Individual differences in perspective taking shape perceptions of intelligence mindset, (5) The impact of age and generational status on intelligence mindset depends on socializing agent, and (6) Identity markers shape student perceptions of intelligence and intelligence mindset. Findings demonstrate the utility in applying a bioecological model for the study of mindset and point to new directions for examining the synergistic impact of person-context interactions on student motivation, learning, and achievement.

心态——关于智力可塑性的信念——传统上被概念化为个人层面的结构。本研究采用、提出并应用了一个生物生态学模型来研究学习者如何感知重要的社交主体——父母、老师和同伴——所拥有的智力心态信念,以及这些感知到的心态信念是否以及如何塑造个人对智力和智力心态的理解。对42名美国本科生的焦点小组数据进行归纳编码,揭示了六个反映人与环境动态相互作用的主题:(1)智力的定义反映了资源、力量和需求特征;(2)学生对成长心态信念的认同程度较高,但并非无条件;(3)支持和支持环境塑造了智力心态;(4)视角的个体差异塑造了智力心态的感知;(5)年龄和代际地位对智力心态的影响取决于社会化主体。(6)身份标记塑造学生的智力认知和智力心态。研究结果证明了将生物生态学模型应用于思维模式研究的实用性,并为研究人-环境互动对学生动机、学习和成就的协同影响指明了新的方向。
{"title":"A bioecological perspective on mindset","authors":"Matthew H. Kim","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102173","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102173","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mindset—beliefs about the malleability of intelligence—has been traditionally conceptualized as an individual-level construct. The present study adapts, proposes, and applies a bioecological model to examine how learners perceive the intelligence mindset beliefs possessed by important socializing agents—parents, teachers, and peers—and whether and how these perceived mindset beliefs shape individuals’ own understanding of intelligence and intelligence mindset. Inductive coding of focus group data from 42 undergraduate students in the United States revealed six themes reflecting the dynamic interplay between person and context: (1) Definitions of intelligence reflect resource, force, and demand characteristics, (2) Students largely endorse growth mindset beliefs, but not unconditionally, (3) Supports and supportive environments shape intelligence mindset, (4) Individual differences in perspective taking shape perceptions of intelligence mindset, (5) The impact of age and generational status on intelligence mindset depends on socializing agent, and (6) Identity markers shape student perceptions of intelligence and intelligence mindset. Findings demonstrate the utility in applying a bioecological model for the study of mindset and point to new directions for examining the synergistic impact of person-context interactions on student motivation, learning, and achievement.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45363679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A school-based intervention on elementary students’ school engagement 基于学校的小学生学校参与干预
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102148
R. Azevedo , P. Rosário , J.C. Núñez , G. Vallejo , S. Fuentes , P. Magalhães

Prior research has reported signs of low engagement in the early stages of schooling. The present study assessed the effectiveness of a school-based intervention that promotes cognitive, emotional, and behavioral engagement in elementary school children through a story tool. The study followed a cluster-randomized design with 259 fourth graders nested in 12 classes; the classes, not the individuals, were randomly assigned to an intervention or control group. Both groups were assessed in four waves in two measures for each engagement dimension. Data were analyzed with a multilevel approach. Findings show that the intervention enhanced students’ cognitive, emotional, and behavioral engagement. Still, there is a delay before the intervention program exhibits a beneficial effect.

Moreover, gender discrepancies were found. Before the intervention, girls showed higher cognitive and emotional engagement, but boys exhibited higher emotional engagement after the intervention. In addition, current results indicate that the program benefited the boys more than the girls. Finally, there was no evidence that the engagement outcomes differed depending on the parent’s educational level. Findings provide valuable information for future research and educational practice.

先前的研究报告了在学校教育的早期阶段低参与度的迹象。本研究评估了通过故事工具促进小学生认知、情感和行为参与的学校干预的有效性。该研究采用集群随机设计,259名四年级学生被安排在12个班级;这些班级,而不是个人,被随机分配到干预组或对照组。这两组人在每个参与维度的两个测量中分为四波进行评估。数据分析采用多层次方法。研究结果表明,干预提高了学生的认知、情感和行为投入。然而,在干预计划显示出有益效果之前,还有一段时间。此外,还发现了性别差异。干预前女生表现出较高的认知投入和情感投入,干预后男生表现出较高的情感投入。此外,目前的结果表明,该计划使男孩比女孩受益更多。最后,没有证据表明参与的结果会因父母的教育水平而有所不同。研究结果为今后的研究和教育实践提供了有价值的信息。
{"title":"A school-based intervention on elementary students’ school engagement","authors":"R. Azevedo ,&nbsp;P. Rosário ,&nbsp;J.C. Núñez ,&nbsp;G. Vallejo ,&nbsp;S. Fuentes ,&nbsp;P. Magalhães","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102148","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102148","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prior research has reported signs of low engagement in the early stages of schooling. The present study assessed the effectiveness of a school-based intervention that promotes cognitive, emotional, and behavioral engagement in elementary school children through a story tool. The study followed a cluster-randomized design with 259 fourth graders nested in 12 classes; the classes, not the individuals, were randomly assigned to an intervention or control group. Both groups were assessed in four waves in two measures for each engagement dimension. Data were analyzed with a multilevel approach. Findings show that the intervention enhanced students’ cognitive, emotional, and behavioral engagement. Still, there is a delay before the intervention program exhibits a beneficial effect.</p><p>Moreover, gender discrepancies were found. Before the intervention, girls showed higher cognitive and emotional engagement, but boys exhibited higher emotional engagement after the intervention. In addition, current results indicate that the program benefited the boys more than the girls. Finally, there was no evidence that the engagement outcomes differed depending on the parent’s educational level. Findings provide valuable information for future research and educational practice.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45677216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The associations between child and item characteristics, use of vocabulary scaffolds, and reading comprehension in a digital environment: Insights from a big data approach 数字环境中儿童与项目特征、词汇支架的使用和阅读理解之间的联系:来自大数据方法的见解
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102165
Laura Diprossimo , Anastasia Ushakova , Jennifer Zoski , Harrison Gamble , Robin Irey , Kate Cain

Scaffolding features that provide multimodal support for the pronunciation and meaning of words are increasingly common in digital reading environments. These vocabulary scaffolds are intended to aid the accurate pronunciation and understanding of individual words in context, thus supporting both vocabulary development and comprehension of text. However, the evidence on their efficacy remains inconclusive. The present study adds to the evidence base by examining: 1) whether child characteristics predict the use of vocabulary scaffolds; 2) whether the use of vocabulary scaffolds is associated with reading comprehension performance; and 3) whether the association between the use of scaffolds and reading comprehension is modulated by child and/or item characteristics. A large cohort (N ∼ 120,000) of 5- to 8-year-old children in the United States interacted with a gamified digital reading environment with embedded vocabulary scaffolds, thereby generating a large observational dataset of user log files. Confirmatory analyses with Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs) indicated that children with lower literacy skills, beginning readers, girls, and bilingual students were more likely to use the scaffold. Overall, the use of scaffolds was associated with better reading comprehension performance. The association between the use of scaffolds and reading comprehension was modulated by both child and item characteristics. We conclude that vocabulary scaffolds may be promising tools to facilitate reading comprehension and reduce performance differences amongst diverse learners in digital reading environments. Educational implications and recommendations for future research are discussed.

为单词的发音和含义提供多模态支持的脚手架特性在数字阅读环境中越来越普遍。这些词汇脚手架旨在帮助准确发音和理解上下文中的单个单词,从而支持词汇的发展和对文本的理解。然而,关于它们的功效的证据仍然没有定论。本研究通过考察儿童特征是否能预测词汇支架的使用来补充证据基础;2)词汇脚手架的使用是否与阅读理解表现相关;3)脚手架的使用与阅读理解之间的关联是否受到儿童和/或项目特征的调节。在美国,一个由5至8岁儿童组成的大型队列(N ~ 120,000)与嵌入词汇支架的游戏化数字阅读环境进行了互动,从而生成了一个大型用户日志文件观察数据集。用广义线性混合模型(glmm)验证性分析表明,读写能力较低的儿童、初级读者、女孩和双语学生更有可能使用脚手架。总体而言,使用支架与更好的阅读理解表现有关。脚手架的使用与阅读理解之间的关系受到儿童和项目特征的调节。我们得出结论,词汇支架可能是促进阅读理解和减少数字阅读环境中不同学习者之间表现差异的有前途的工具。讨论了教育意义和对未来研究的建议。
{"title":"The associations between child and item characteristics, use of vocabulary scaffolds, and reading comprehension in a digital environment: Insights from a big data approach","authors":"Laura Diprossimo ,&nbsp;Anastasia Ushakova ,&nbsp;Jennifer Zoski ,&nbsp;Harrison Gamble ,&nbsp;Robin Irey ,&nbsp;Kate Cain","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102165","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102165","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Scaffolding features that provide multimodal support for the pronunciation and meaning of words are increasingly common in digital reading environments. These vocabulary scaffolds are intended to aid the accurate pronunciation and understanding of individual words in context, thus supporting both vocabulary development and comprehension of text. However, the evidence on their efficacy remains inconclusive. The present study adds to the evidence base by examining: 1) whether child characteristics predict the use of vocabulary scaffolds; 2) whether the use of vocabulary scaffolds is associated with reading comprehension performance; and 3) whether the association between the use of scaffolds and reading comprehension is modulated by child and/or item characteristics. A large cohort (N ∼ 120,000) of 5- to 8-year-old children in the United States interacted with a gamified digital reading environment with embedded vocabulary scaffolds, thereby generating a large observational dataset of user log files. Confirmatory analyses with Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs) indicated that children with lower literacy skills, beginning readers, girls, and bilingual students were more likely to use the scaffold. Overall, the use of scaffolds was associated with better reading comprehension performance. The association between the use of scaffolds and reading comprehension was modulated by both child and item characteristics. We conclude that vocabulary scaffolds may be promising tools to facilitate reading comprehension and reduce performance differences amongst diverse learners in digital reading environments. Educational implications and recommendations for future research are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48178503","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance during presentations: A question of challenge and threat responses? 演讲中的表现:挑战与威胁反应的问题?
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102168
Marco Schickel , Nina Minkley , Tobias Ringeisen

The current study investigated how changes in psychological (cognitions and emotions) and physiological (cortisol concentrations) threat/challenge responses develop over the course of a presentation and whether initial intensity levels and their changes are related to self-efficacy and presentation performance. Based on the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), 123 students held video-recorded presentations about their dream job, which were evaluated by three raters. Selfefficacy was measured before the TSST, saliva cortisol concentrations and psychological threat and challenge responses three times during the TSST. Data were analyzed with latent growth modeling. Threat and cortisol increased during the presentation, while challenge decreased. The growth curve coefficients of challenge correlated negatively with the respective coefficients of threat. Also, initial intensity of challenge responses correlated positively with corresponding cortisol concentrations. Higher self-efficacy was associated with higher initial intensity of challenge responses, lower corresponding concentrations of cortisol, and a smaller cortisol increase, but neither with the growth curve coefficients of threat responses nor with presentation performance. Better performance was associated with lower initial intensity of threat and a smaller increase in cortisol. Threat responses and increasing cortisol concentration appear to inhibit presentation performance, while self-efficacy may strengthen challenge responses and reduce corresponding levels of cortisol concentration as well as its increase.

目前的研究调查了心理(认知和情绪)和生理(皮质醇浓度)威胁/挑战反应的变化如何在演讲过程中发展,以及初始强度水平及其变化是否与自我效能和演讲表现有关。在特里尔社会压力测试(TSST)的基础上,123名学生录制了关于他们梦想工作的视频,并由三位评分者对其进行评估。在TSST测试前分别测量自我效能、唾液皮质醇浓度和心理威胁和挑战反应。数据分析采用潜在生长模型。在演讲过程中,威胁和皮质醇增加,而挑战减少。攻毒系数与威胁系数呈负相关。此外,挑战反应的初始强度与相应的皮质醇浓度呈正相关。较高的自我效能感与较高的挑战反应初始强度、较低的皮质醇浓度和较小的皮质醇增幅相关,但与威胁反应的生长曲线系数和呈现表现无关。较好的表现与较低的初始威胁强度和较小的皮质醇增加有关。威胁反应和皮质醇浓度的增加似乎会抑制陈述表现,而自我效能感可能会增强挑战反应,降低相应的皮质醇浓度水平及其升高。
{"title":"Performance during presentations: A question of challenge and threat responses?","authors":"Marco Schickel ,&nbsp;Nina Minkley ,&nbsp;Tobias Ringeisen","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102168","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102168","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The current study investigated how changes in psychological (cognitions and emotions) and physiological (cortisol concentrations) threat/challenge responses develop over the course of a presentation and whether initial intensity levels and their changes are related to self-efficacy and presentation performance. Based on the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), 123 students held video-recorded presentations about their dream job, which were evaluated by three raters. Selfefficacy was measured before the TSST, saliva cortisol concentrations and psychological threat and challenge responses three times during the TSST. Data were analyzed with latent growth modeling. Threat and cortisol increased during the presentation, while challenge decreased. The growth curve coefficients of challenge correlated negatively with the respective coefficients of threat. Also, initial intensity of challenge responses correlated positively with corresponding cortisol concentrations. Higher self-efficacy was associated with higher initial intensity of challenge responses, lower corresponding concentrations of cortisol, and a smaller cortisol increase, but neither with the growth curve coefficients of threat responses nor with presentation performance. Better performance was associated with lower initial intensity of threat and a smaller increase in cortisol. Threat responses and increasing cortisol concentration appear to inhibit presentation performance, while self-efficacy may strengthen challenge responses and reduce corresponding levels of cortisol concentration as well as its increase.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44269317","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Why do students strive to outperform classmates? Unpacking their reasons for pursuing performance goals 为什么学生努力超越同学?解开他们追求绩效目标的原因
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102178
Corwin Senko , Gregory Arief D. Liem , Thanita Lerdpornkulrat , Chanut Poondej

Achievement goal theory has evolved since introduced about 40 years ago. One of its newer variants is the goal complex model. It assumes that each achievement goal (i.e., performance or mastery) can be pursued for many reasons and, more provocatively, that the goal’s effects depend partly on why it is pursued. Clearly, the first task for this area is to identify likely goal pursuit reasons, develop and validate a measure of reasons, and chart the influence of those reasons. Progress remains limited, however. Nearly all studies have considered only a small set of reasons suggested predominantly by self-determination theory, overlooking several other plausible reasons. Nor is there an established measure of goal pursuit reasons. To overcome those limitations, the current study validated and tested a new goal complex measure that includes several additional goal pursuit reasons, both personal (e.g., pride) and social (e.g., to make close others proud, or to help or serve others) in nature. Two culturally distinct samples of university students – one from the USA (n = 400), the other from Thailand (n = 404) – completed the measure with performance goals in mind and then reported a diverse array of educational outcomes. Their results converge for the most part. In each sample, the new measure proved to have good structural validity and psychometric properties. Several goal complexes, including the new social ones, showed unique and often desirable relationships with outcomes, too. The findings raise several research directions and implications for achievement goal theory.

成就目标理论自40年前提出以来一直在发展。它的一个较新的变体是目标复杂模型。它假设每个成就目标(即表现或精通)可以出于多种原因而追求,更令人不安的是,目标的效果部分取决于追求目标的原因。显然,这个领域的第一项任务是确定可能的目标追求原因,开发和验证原因的度量,并绘制这些原因的影响图表。然而,进展仍然有限。几乎所有的研究都只考虑了主要由自我决定理论提出的一小部分原因,而忽略了其他一些看似合理的原因。也没有既定的衡量目标追求原因的标准。为了克服这些限制,目前的研究验证并测试了一种新的目标复杂测量方法,其中包括几个额外的目标追求原因,包括个人原因(例如,骄傲)和社会原因(例如,让亲近的人感到骄傲,或帮助或服务他人)。两组不同文化背景的大学生——一组来自美国(n = 400),另一组来自泰国(n = 404)——在完成测试时都考虑到了表现目标,然后报告了一系列不同的教育成果。他们的结果在很大程度上是一致的。在每个样本中,新测量都被证明具有良好的结构效度和心理测量特性。一些目标情结,包括新的社会情结,也与结果表现出独特的、通常令人满意的关系。研究结果为成就目标理论提出了若干研究方向和启示。
{"title":"Why do students strive to outperform classmates? Unpacking their reasons for pursuing performance goals","authors":"Corwin Senko ,&nbsp;Gregory Arief D. Liem ,&nbsp;Thanita Lerdpornkulrat ,&nbsp;Chanut Poondej","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102178","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2023.102178","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Achievement goal theory has evolved since introduced about 40 years ago. One of its newer variants is the goal complex model. It assumes that each achievement goal (i.e., performance or mastery) can be pursued for many reasons and, more provocatively, that the goal’s effects depend partly on why it is pursued. Clearly, the first task for this area is to identify likely goal pursuit reasons, develop and validate a measure of reasons, and chart the influence of those reasons. Progress remains limited, however. Nearly all studies have considered only a small set of reasons suggested predominantly by self-determination theory, overlooking several other plausible reasons. Nor is there an established measure of goal pursuit reasons. To overcome those limitations, the current study validated and tested a new goal complex measure that includes several additional goal pursuit reasons, both personal (e.g., pride) and social (e.g., to make close others proud, or to help or serve others) in nature. Two culturally distinct samples of university students – one from the USA (<em>n</em> = 400), the other from Thailand (<em>n</em> = 404) – completed the measure with performance goals in mind and then reported a diverse array of educational outcomes. Their results converge for the most part. In each sample, the new measure proved to have good structural validity and psychometric properties. Several goal complexes, including the new social ones, showed unique and often desirable relationships with outcomes, too. The findings raise several research directions and implications for achievement goal theory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43373596","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Contemporary Educational Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1