首页 > 最新文献

Experimental psychology最新文献

英文 中文
In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone. 寻找偏好反转区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000542
Steven Glautier,Hedwig Eisenbarth,Anne Macaskill
A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.
当两个奖励在时间上更接近时,对大晚(LL)奖励的偏好比小早(SS)奖励的偏好逆转时,可以观察到偏好逆转。偏好反转在日常生活和实验室中很常见,并且经常被声称支持双曲延迟贴现模型,在其最简单的形式中,可以用一个自由参数来模拟反转。然而,目前尚不清楚偏好逆转的时间位置是否可以先验地预测。测试模型预测的研究并没有找到支持它们的证据,但它们忽视了强化物大小对贴现率的充分证明的影响。因此,我们直接测试了双曲线和指数模型预测在预登记的研究中,通过评估个人贴现率的两个强化幅度。然后,我们对可能发生偏好逆转的成对选择进行了个性化预测。在107名参与者中,我们发现(1)很少有证据表明双曲模型和指数模型可以预测偏好逆转的时间位置,(2)一些证据表明双曲模型比指数模型具有更好的预测性能,(3)与许多先前的研究相比,指数模型通常比双曲模型更适合观察数据。
{"title":"In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone.","authors":"Steven Glautier,Hedwig Eisenbarth,Anne Macaskill","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000542","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000542","url":null,"abstract":"A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138532231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Production Effect Interacts With Serial Positions 生产效应与序列位置相互作用
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000540
Sébastien Gionet, D. Guitard, J. Saint-Aubin
Abstract: Reading some words aloud during presentation, that is, producing them, and reading other words silently generate a large memory advantage for words that are produced. This robust within-list production effect is in contrast with the between-lists condition in which all words are read aloud or silently. In a between-lists condition, produced items are better recognized, but not better recalled. The lack of a between-lists production effect with recall tasks has often been presented as one of its defining characteristics and as a benchmark for evaluating models. Recently, Cyr et al. (2021) showed that this occurs because item production interacts with serial positions: Produced items are less well recalled on the first serial positions than silently read items, while the reverse pattern is observed for the recency portion of the curve. However, this pattern was observed with a repeated-measures design, and it may be a by-product of compensatory processes under the control of participants. Here, using a between-participants design, we observed the predicted interaction between production and serial positions. The results further support the Revised Feature Model (RFM) suggesting that produced items are encoded with more modality-dependent distinctive features, therefore benefiting recall. However, the production of the additional distinctive features would disrupt rehearsal.
摘要:在演讲过程中,大声朗读一些单词,即朗读这些单词,而默读其他单词,对所朗读的单词有很大的记忆优势。这种健壮的列表内生成效果与列表间条件形成对比,在列表间条件下,所有单词都被大声或静默地读出。在中间列表的条件下,生产的产品被更好地识别,但不是更好地召回。缺乏列表间产生效应的召回任务经常被提出作为其定义特征之一,并作为评估模型的基准。最近,Cyr等人(2021)表明,这种情况的发生是因为物品生产与序列位置相互作用:在第一个序列位置上,生产的物品比静默读取的物品回忆得更少,而在曲线的最近部分,观察到相反的模式。然而,这种模式是通过重复测量设计观察到的,它可能是参与者控制下代偿过程的副产品。在这里,使用参与者之间的设计,我们观察到生产和系列位置之间的预测交互作用。结果进一步支持了修正特征模型(RFM),表明生产出来的物品被编码了更多与模态相关的显著特征,因此有利于回忆。然而,制作额外的特色会干扰排练。
{"title":"The Production Effect Interacts With Serial Positions","authors":"Sébastien Gionet, D. Guitard, J. Saint-Aubin","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000540","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000540","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract: Reading some words aloud during presentation, that is, producing them, and reading other words silently generate a large memory advantage for words that are produced. This robust within-list production effect is in contrast with the between-lists condition in which all words are read aloud or silently. In a between-lists condition, produced items are better recognized, but not better recalled. The lack of a between-lists production effect with recall tasks has often been presented as one of its defining characteristics and as a benchmark for evaluating models. Recently, Cyr et al. (2021) showed that this occurs because item production interacts with serial positions: Produced items are less well recalled on the first serial positions than silently read items, while the reverse pattern is observed for the recency portion of the curve. However, this pattern was observed with a repeated-measures design, and it may be a by-product of compensatory processes under the control of participants. Here, using a between-participants design, we observed the predicted interaction between production and serial positions. The results further support the Revised Feature Model (RFM) suggesting that produced items are encoded with more modality-dependent distinctive features, therefore benefiting recall. However, the production of the additional distinctive features would disrupt rehearsal.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41651118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone. 寻找偏好反转区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk
S. Glautier, H. Eisenbarth, Anne C. Macaskill
A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.
当两个奖励在时间上更接近时,对大晚(LL)奖励的偏好比小早(SS)奖励的偏好逆转时,可以观察到偏好逆转。偏好反转在日常生活和实验室中很常见,并且经常被声称支持双曲延迟贴现模型,在其最简单的形式中,可以用一个自由参数来模拟反转。然而,目前尚不清楚偏好逆转的时间位置是否可以先验地预测。测试模型预测的研究并没有找到支持它们的证据,但它们忽视了强化物大小对贴现率的充分证明的影响。因此,我们直接测试了双曲线和指数模型预测在预登记的研究中,通过评估个人贴现率的两个强化幅度。然后,我们对可能发生偏好逆转的成对选择进行了个性化预测。在107名参与者中,我们发现(1)很少有证据表明双曲模型和指数模型可以预测偏好逆转的时间位置,(2)一些证据表明双曲模型比指数模型具有更好的预测性能,(3)与许多先前的研究相比,指数模型通常比双曲模型更适合观察数据。
{"title":"In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone.","authors":"S. Glautier, H. Eisenbarth, Anne C. Macaskill","doi":"10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk","url":null,"abstract":"A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41612516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Functional Specificity of the Affordance of Reaching. 可达性的功能特异性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000544
Tyler Surber, Tyler Overstreet, Hannah L Masoner, Catherine Dowell, A. Hajnal
The information that specifies whether an object is within reach is a complex pattern that depends on body-scaled parameters measured from an egocentric reference point. The pattern is a function of relevant body proportions (eye height, shoulder height [SH], arm length) with respect to the spatial location of the target object. In addition to not knowing how these factors map onto perception, it is also not known whether the egocentric viewpoint is centered at the eye or the shoulder. In three experiments, we systematically tested whether observers can perceive eye height and SH (Experiment 1), whether they can point accurately in the direction of a target object (Experiment 2), and whether they can point accurately to judge if the target object is within reach (Experiment 3). Experiment 1 demonstrated that participants are more accurate at judging their own eye height than SH. Experiment 2 revealed that participants can more accurately point to a target object's location when measured from the shoulder as a reference point than when measured from the eye. In Experiment 3, we showed that a higher-order variable that includes arm length, body height, and angle of declination to the target successfully predicted affordance judgments, regardless of a reference point. We consider this as evidence that the invariant is functionally specific, not tied to any one particular anatomical body part.
指定物体是否在可及范围内的信息是一个复杂的模式,它取决于从以自我为中心的参考点测量的身体尺度参数。该模式是相对于目标物体的空间位置的相关身体比例(眼高、肩高[SH]、臂长)的函数。除了不知道这些因素如何映射到感知之外,也不知道以自我为中心的观点是集中在眼睛还是肩膀上。在三个实验中,我们系统地测试了观察者是否能够感知眼睛高度和高度(实验1),他们是否能够准确地指向目标物体的方向(实验2),实验1表明,被试判断自己的眼睛高度比SH更准确。实验2表明,被试以肩膀为参考点比以眼睛为参考点能更准确地指出目标物体的位置。在实验3中,我们展示了一个高阶变量,包括臂长、身体高度和俯角到目标的角度,成功地预测了功能判断,而与参考点无关。我们认为这是不变量是功能特异性的证据,而不是与任何一个特定的身体解剖部位相关联。
{"title":"Functional Specificity of the Affordance of Reaching.","authors":"Tyler Surber, Tyler Overstreet, Hannah L Masoner, Catherine Dowell, A. Hajnal","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000544","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000544","url":null,"abstract":"The information that specifies whether an object is within reach is a complex pattern that depends on body-scaled parameters measured from an egocentric reference point. The pattern is a function of relevant body proportions (eye height, shoulder height [SH], arm length) with respect to the spatial location of the target object. In addition to not knowing how these factors map onto perception, it is also not known whether the egocentric viewpoint is centered at the eye or the shoulder. In three experiments, we systematically tested whether observers can perceive eye height and SH (Experiment 1), whether they can point accurately in the direction of a target object (Experiment 2), and whether they can point accurately to judge if the target object is within reach (Experiment 3). Experiment 1 demonstrated that participants are more accurate at judging their own eye height than SH. Experiment 2 revealed that participants can more accurately point to a target object's location when measured from the shoulder as a reference point than when measured from the eye. In Experiment 3, we showed that a higher-order variable that includes arm length, body height, and angle of declination to the target successfully predicted affordance judgments, regardless of a reference point. We consider this as evidence that the invariant is functionally specific, not tied to any one particular anatomical body part.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48854641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Blink and the Body 眨眼与身体
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000539
Erik M. Benau, R. Atchley
Abstract. We evaluated the interaction of emotion, interoceptive awareness (IA), and attention using an attentional blink (AB) task. Healthy undergraduates completed a cardiac awareness task and, based on previously validated cut scores, were classified as high or average perceivers (n = 19 in each group; matched on age and gender). Participants completed an AB task with counterbalanced emotional and/or neutral lexical stimuli as the first target (T1) and/or the second target (T2). Both high and average perceivers exhibited retroactive interference in conditions where T2 immediately followed T1. However, only the average perceivers exhibited a significant blink effect: They reported T2 inaccurately in trials in which one intervening stimulus occurred between T1 and T2. High perceivers exhibited their best performance in trials where both targets were emotional; average perceivers exhibited their worst performance in these trials. These results contribute to a small but growing literature that suggests IA and exteroceptive attention are related systems.
摘要我们使用注意眨眼(AB)任务评估了情绪、内感受意识(IA)和注意力的相互作用。健康的本科生完成了心脏意识任务,并根据之前验证的分数,被归类为高感知者或中等感知者(每组n=19;根据年龄和性别匹配)。参与者以平衡的情绪和/或中性词汇刺激作为第一目标(T1)和/或第二目标(T2)完成AB任务。在T2紧随T1之后的条件下,高感知者和平均感知者都表现出追溯干扰。然而,只有普通感知者表现出显著的眨眼效应:他们在T1和T2之间发生一次干预刺激的试验中不准确地报告了T2。高感知者在两个目标都情绪化的试验中表现出最佳表现;普通感知者在这些试验中表现出最差的表现。这些结果有助于一个小规模但不断增长的文献表明IA和外显注意是相关的系统。
{"title":"The Blink and the Body","authors":"Erik M. Benau, R. Atchley","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000539","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000539","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. We evaluated the interaction of emotion, interoceptive awareness (IA), and attention using an attentional blink (AB) task. Healthy undergraduates completed a cardiac awareness task and, based on previously validated cut scores, were classified as high or average perceivers (n = 19 in each group; matched on age and gender). Participants completed an AB task with counterbalanced emotional and/or neutral lexical stimuli as the first target (T1) and/or the second target (T2). Both high and average perceivers exhibited retroactive interference in conditions where T2 immediately followed T1. However, only the average perceivers exhibited a significant blink effect: They reported T2 inaccurately in trials in which one intervening stimulus occurred between T1 and T2. High perceivers exhibited their best performance in trials where both targets were emotional; average perceivers exhibited their worst performance in these trials. These results contribute to a small but growing literature that suggests IA and exteroceptive attention are related systems.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46031340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infusing Context Into Emotion Perception Impacts Emotion Decoding Accuracy. 情境对情绪感知的影响
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000531
Ursula Hess, Konstantinos Kafetsios

The accurate decoding of facial emotion expressions lies at the center of many research traditions in psychology. Much of this research, while paying lip service to the importance of context in emotion perception, has used stimuli that were carefully created to be deprived of contextual information. The participants' task is to associate the expression shown in the face with a correct label, essentially changing a social perception task into a cognitive task. In fact, in many cases, the task can be carried out correctly without engaging emotion recognition at all. The present article argues that infusing context in emotion perception does not only add an additional source of information but changes the way that participants approach the task by rendering it a social perception task rather than a cognitive task. Importantly, distinguishing between accuracy (perceiving the intended emotions) and bias (perceiving additional emotions to those intended) leads to a more nuanced understanding of social emotion perception. Results from several studies that use the Assessment of Contextual Emotions demonstrate the significance and social functionality of simultaneously considering emotion decoding accuracy and bias for social interaction in different cultures, their key personality and societal correlates, and their function for close relationships processes.

摘要面部情绪表情的准确解码是许多心理学研究传统的核心。这类研究的大部分,虽然口头上强调情境在情绪感知中的重要性,但使用的刺激都是精心制造的,被剥夺了情境信息。参与者的任务是将面部表情与正确的标签联系起来,从本质上将社会感知任务转变为认知任务。事实上,在许多情况下,这项任务完全可以在不参与情绪识别的情况下正确执行。本文认为,在情绪感知中注入情境不仅增加了额外的信息来源,而且通过将其呈现为社会感知任务而不是认知任务,改变了参与者处理任务的方式。重要的是,区分准确性(感知预期情绪)和偏见(感知预期情绪之外的额外情绪)可以更细致地理解社会情绪感知。使用情境情绪评估的几项研究结果表明,在不同文化的社会交往中,同时考虑情绪解码的准确性和偏见,他们的关键个性和社会相关因素,以及他们在亲密关系过程中的功能,具有重要意义和社会功能。
{"title":"Infusing Context Into Emotion Perception Impacts Emotion Decoding Accuracy.","authors":"Ursula Hess, Konstantinos Kafetsios","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000531","DOIUrl":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000531","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b></b> The accurate decoding of facial emotion expressions lies at the center of many research traditions in psychology. Much of this research, while paying lip service to the importance of context in emotion perception, has used stimuli that were carefully created to be deprived of contextual information. The participants' task is to associate the expression shown in the face with a <i>correct</i> label, essentially changing a social perception task into a cognitive task. In fact, in many cases, the task can be carried out correctly without engaging emotion recognition at all. The present article argues that infusing context in emotion perception does not only add an additional source of information but changes the way that participants approach the task by rendering it a social perception task rather than a cognitive task. Importantly, distinguishing between accuracy (perceiving the intended emotions) and bias (perceiving additional emotions to those intended) leads to a more nuanced understanding of social emotion perception. Results from several studies that use the Assessment of Contextual Emotions demonstrate the significance and social functionality of simultaneously considering emotion decoding accuracy and bias for social interaction in different cultures, their key personality and societal correlates, and their function for close relationships processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9446470/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44276184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Interval Anchoring Effect. 区间锚定效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000534
Manru Liu, Jianmin Zeng, Ziyun Gao
The anchoring effect refers to a decision bias that initial irrelevant information can influence late judgment. So far, most (if not all) studies on the anchoring effect adopted only point anchors (e.g., "Do you want to buy a computer with a price higher or lower than $1,000?"). In reality, people also use interval anchors (e.g., "Do you want to buy a computer with a price within $800-1,200?"). Can interval anchors also produce anchoring effect? Which kind of anchors have stronger anchoring effect? To answer these questions, we conducted four experiments involving quite different content. In each experiment, we found extremely significant anchoring effects for point anchors and interval anchors, respectively, but no significant difference between them. The results suggest that rarely researched interval anchors can be as powerful as intensively investigated point anchors and thus deserve more research and applications henceforth.
锚定效应是指一种决策偏差,即最初不相关的信息会影响后期的判断。到目前为止,大多数(如果不是全部的话)关于锚定效应的研究只采用了点锚点(例如,“你想买一台价格高于或低于1000美元的电脑?”)。实际上,人们也会使用区间锚点(例如,“你想买一台价格在800- 1200美元之间的电脑吗?”)。间歇主播也能产生锚定效应吗?哪种锚的锚定效果更强?为了回答这些问题,我们进行了四个涉及不同内容的实验。在每次实验中,我们都发现点锚和区间锚的锚定效果都非常显著,但两者之间没有显著差异。研究结果表明,研究较少的区间锚可以与研究较多的点锚一样强大,因此值得今后进行更多的研究和应用。
{"title":"The Interval Anchoring Effect.","authors":"Manru Liu, Jianmin Zeng, Ziyun Gao","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000534","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000534","url":null,"abstract":"The anchoring effect refers to a decision bias that initial irrelevant information can influence late judgment. So far, most (if not all) studies on the anchoring effect adopted only point anchors (e.g., \"Do you want to buy a computer with a price higher or lower than $1,000?\"). In reality, people also use interval anchors (e.g., \"Do you want to buy a computer with a price within $800-1,200?\"). Can interval anchors also produce anchoring effect? Which kind of anchors have stronger anchoring effect? To answer these questions, we conducted four experiments involving quite different content. In each experiment, we found extremely significant anchoring effects for point anchors and interval anchors, respectively, but no significant difference between them. The results suggest that rarely researched interval anchors can be as powerful as intensively investigated point anchors and thus deserve more research and applications henceforth.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57294539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fixed Attributes and Discounting Behavior. 固定属性和折扣行为。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000535
Qiongwen Cao, Andre Hofmeyr, Eustace Hsu, Shan Luo, J. Monterosso
Delay discounting tasks present alternatives that differ in two attributes: amount and delay. Typically, choice is modeled by application of a discount function to each option, allowing alternative-wise comparison. However, if participants make decisions by comparing attributes, manipulations that affect the salience of either attribute may affect patience. In Experiment 1, participants completed one block of trials in which amount was a fixed attribute (constant across trials), and another in which delay was fixed. Consistent with the hypothesis that the varying attribute would be more salient, participants exhibited less patience in the amount-fixed condition. Moreover, this effect was larger for participants who responded more quickly when making choices that favored the varying attribute. In Experiment 2, these findings were extended by adding trial blocks with a working memory dual task. We replicated the fixed-attribute effect, along with the aforementioned association with reaction time. Contrary to expectation, the fixed-attribute effect was not larger when participants were under working memory load. Instead, working memory load was associated with more patient responses, which may be related to idiosyncrasies of the task including the absence of immediate rewards. Overall, results suggest a fixed-attribute effect on patience, which is consistent with a multi-attribute decision framework.
延迟贴现任务提供两个不同属性的选择:数量和延迟。通常,选择是通过对每个选项应用折扣函数来建模的,从而允许对不同选项进行比较。然而,如果参与者通过比较属性来做决定,影响任何属性显著性的操作可能会影响耐心。在实验1中,参与者完成了一组以数量为固定属性的试验,另一组以延迟为固定属性的试验。与变化属性更为显著的假设一致,参与者在数量固定的条件下表现出较少的耐心。此外,对于那些在做出有利于不同属性的选择时反应更快的参与者来说,这种影响更大。在实验2中,通过增加工作记忆双重任务的实验块来扩展这些发现。我们复制了固定属性效应,以及前面提到的与反应时间的关联。与预期相反,当被试处于工作记忆负荷下时,固定属性效应并没有更大。相反,工作记忆负荷与更多的病人反应有关,这可能与任务的特质有关,包括缺乏即时奖励。总体而言,结果表明固定属性对耐心的影响,这与多属性决策框架是一致的。
{"title":"Fixed Attributes and Discounting Behavior.","authors":"Qiongwen Cao, Andre Hofmeyr, Eustace Hsu, Shan Luo, J. Monterosso","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000535","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000535","url":null,"abstract":"Delay discounting tasks present alternatives that differ in two attributes: amount and delay. Typically, choice is modeled by application of a discount function to each option, allowing alternative-wise comparison. However, if participants make decisions by comparing attributes, manipulations that affect the salience of either attribute may affect patience. In Experiment 1, participants completed one block of trials in which amount was a fixed attribute (constant across trials), and another in which delay was fixed. Consistent with the hypothesis that the varying attribute would be more salient, participants exhibited less patience in the amount-fixed condition. Moreover, this effect was larger for participants who responded more quickly when making choices that favored the varying attribute. In Experiment 2, these findings were extended by adding trial blocks with a working memory dual task. We replicated the fixed-attribute effect, along with the aforementioned association with reaction time. Contrary to expectation, the fixed-attribute effect was not larger when participants were under working memory load. Instead, working memory load was associated with more patient responses, which may be related to idiosyncrasies of the task including the absence of immediate rewards. Overall, results suggest a fixed-attribute effect on patience, which is consistent with a multi-attribute decision framework.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48051864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Postural Effect on the Memory of Manipulable Objects. 姿势对可操纵物体记忆的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000537
Léo Dutriaux, V. Gyselinck
The grounded cognition approach posits the involvement of sensory-motor processes in the representation of knowledge. However, the functional impact of these processes on cognition has been questioned, and some authors have explored the effect of motor interference on memory to test causally this hypothesis. In a seminal study, Dutriaux and Gyselinck (2016) showed that keeping the hands behind the back during learning decreases the memory of manipulable objects, but not the memory of nonmanipulable objects. The aim of this paper is to shed light on the mechanism behind the effect of posture in memory observed by Dutriaux and Gyselinck. The present experiment replicated the posture manipulation during learning but asked participants to keep their hands behind the back during recall. Results showed a similar detrimental effect of the hands behind the back specific to manipulable objects. This shows that the mechanism behind this effect arises from postural interference rather than from a compatibility between the posture during learning and the posture during recall and adds new evidence in favor of the sensory-motor grounding of knowledge.
基础认知方法假定感觉运动过程参与知识的表征。然而,这些过程对认知的功能影响一直受到质疑,一些作者探讨了运动干扰对记忆的影响,以因果地检验这一假设。Dutriaux和Gyselinck(2016)在一项开创性的研究中表明,在学习过程中把手放在背后会降低对可操作物体的记忆,但不会降低对不可操作对象的记忆。本文的目的是阐明Dutriaux和Gyselinck观察到的姿势对记忆影响背后的机制。本实验复制了学习过程中的姿势操作,但要求参与者在回忆过程中把手放在背后。结果显示,可操作物体背后的手也会产生类似的有害影响。这表明,这种影响背后的机制来自姿势干扰,而不是学习过程中的姿势和回忆过程中的姿态之间的兼容性,并为知识的感觉运动基础提供了新的证据。
{"title":"The Postural Effect on the Memory of Manipulable Objects.","authors":"Léo Dutriaux, V. Gyselinck","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000537","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000537","url":null,"abstract":"The grounded cognition approach posits the involvement of sensory-motor processes in the representation of knowledge. However, the functional impact of these processes on cognition has been questioned, and some authors have explored the effect of motor interference on memory to test causally this hypothesis. In a seminal study, Dutriaux and Gyselinck (2016) showed that keeping the hands behind the back during learning decreases the memory of manipulable objects, but not the memory of nonmanipulable objects. The aim of this paper is to shed light on the mechanism behind the effect of posture in memory observed by Dutriaux and Gyselinck. The present experiment replicated the posture manipulation during learning but asked participants to keep their hands behind the back during recall. Results showed a similar detrimental effect of the hands behind the back specific to manipulable objects. This shows that the mechanism behind this effect arises from postural interference rather than from a compatibility between the posture during learning and the posture during recall and adds new evidence in favor of the sensory-motor grounding of knowledge.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44765874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Correction to Wu et al., 2021. 对Wu et al., 2021的修正。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000538
{"title":"Correction to Wu et al., 2021.","authors":"","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000538","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000538","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49265517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Experimental psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1