首页 > 最新文献

Experimental psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Loud Auditory Distractors Are More Difficult to Ignore After All. 毕竟,响亮的听觉干扰更难以忽视。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000554
Lejla Alikadic, Jan Philipp Röer

Working memory performance is markedly disrupted when task-irrelevant sound is played during item presentation or retention. In a preregistered replication study, we systematically examined the role of intensity in two types of auditory distraction. The first type of distraction is the changing-state effect (i.e., increased disruption by changing-state relative to steady-state sequences). The second type is the auditory deviant effect (i.e., increased disruption by auditory deviant relative to steady-state sequences). In previous experiments, the changing-state effect was independent of intensity. Whether a deviation in intensity leads to an increase in disruption has not yet been examined. We replicated the classic finding that the increased disruption by changing-state relative to steady-state sequences is independent of intensity. Contrary to previous studies, we found an unexpected main effect of intensity. Steady-state and changing-state sequences presented at 75 dB(A) were more disruptive than presented at 45 dB(A), suggesting that intensity plays a more important role than previously assumed in the disruption of working memory performance. Furthermore, we tested the prediction of the violation of expectancy account, according to which deviant distractors at a lower and higher intensity than the rest of the sequence should be equally disruptive. Our results were consistent with this prediction.

当在项目展示或保留过程中播放与任务无关的声音时,工作记忆表现明显受到干扰。在一项预先登记的重复研究中,我们系统地检查了强度在两种类型的听觉分心中的作用。第一种类型的分心是状态变化效应(即,相对于稳态序列,状态变化会增加干扰)。第二种类型是听觉偏差效应(即,相对于稳态序列,听觉偏差造成的干扰增加)。在以往的实验中,变态效应与强度无关。强度的偏差是否会导致破坏的增加还没有得到检验。我们复制了经典的发现,即相对于稳态序列,变化状态增加的破坏与强度无关。与以往的研究相反,我们发现了一个意想不到的强度的主要影响。与45 dB(A)相比,75 dB(A)下的稳态和变化状态序列对工作记忆的干扰更大,表明强度在工作记忆的干扰中起着比之前假设的更重要的作用。此外,我们还测试了违反期望帐户的预测,根据该帐户,低于和高于其他序列的偏离干扰因素的强度应该是相同的破坏性。我们的结果与这一预测一致。
{"title":"Loud Auditory Distractors Are More Difficult to Ignore After All.","authors":"Lejla Alikadic,&nbsp;Jan Philipp Röer","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000554","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b></b> Working memory performance is markedly disrupted when task-irrelevant sound is played during item presentation or retention. In a preregistered replication study, we systematically examined the role of intensity in two types of auditory distraction. The first type of distraction is the changing-state effect (i.e., increased disruption by changing-state relative to steady-state sequences). The second type is the auditory deviant effect (i.e., increased disruption by auditory deviant relative to steady-state sequences). In previous experiments, the changing-state effect was independent of intensity. Whether a deviation in intensity leads to an increase in disruption has not yet been examined. We replicated the classic finding that the increased disruption by changing-state relative to steady-state sequences is independent of intensity. Contrary to previous studies, we found an unexpected main effect of intensity. Steady-state and changing-state sequences presented at 75 dB(A) were more disruptive than presented at 45 dB(A), suggesting that intensity plays a more important role than previously assumed in the disruption of working memory performance. Furthermore, we tested the prediction of the violation of expectancy account, according to which deviant distractors at a lower and higher intensity than the rest of the sequence should be equally disruptive. Our results were consistent with this prediction.</p>","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 3","pages":"163-171"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9609681/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40337558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Anchoring and Sleep Inertia. 锚定和睡眠惯性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000552
Marie-Lena Frech, Jan Alexander Häusser, Marie-Carolin Siems, David D Loschelder

Many occupational settings require individuals to make important decisions immediately after awakening. Although a plethora of psychological research has separately examined both sleep and anchoring effects on decision-making, little is known about their interaction. In the present study, we seek to shed light on the link between sleep inertia, the performance impairment immediately after awakening, and individuals' susceptibility to the anchoring bias. We proposed that sleep inertia would moderate participants' adjustment from anchors because sleep inertia leads to less cognitive effort invested, resulting in a stronger anchoring effect. One hundred four subjects were randomly assigned to an experimental group that answered anchoring tasks immediately after being awakened at nighttime or a control group that answered anchoring tasks at daytime. Our findings replicated the well-established anchoring effect in that higher anchors led participants to higher estimates than lower anchors. We did not find significant effects of sleep inertia. While the sleep inertia group reported greater sleepiness and having invested less cognitive effort compared to the control group, no systematic anchoring differences emerged, and cognitive effort did not qualify as a mediator of the anchoring effect. Bayesian analyses provide empirical evidence for these null findings. Implications for the anchoring literature and future research are discussed.

许多职业环境要求个人在醒来后立即做出重要决定。尽管大量的心理学研究分别研究了睡眠和锚定对决策的影响,但人们对它们之间的相互作用知之甚少。在本研究中,我们试图揭示睡眠惯性、醒来后立即的表现障碍和个体对锚定偏见的易感性之间的联系。我们提出睡眠惯性会调节参与者对锚点的调整,因为睡眠惯性会导致投入的认知努力减少,从而产生更强的锚点效应。104名受试者被随机分配到实验组,实验组在夜间醒来后立即回答锚定任务,对照组则在白天回答锚定任务。我们的发现重复了公认的锚定效应,即较高的锚点比较低的锚点导致参与者的估计更高。我们没有发现睡眠惯性的显著影响。虽然与对照组相比,睡眠惯性组报告的困倦程度更高,投入的认知努力也更少,但没有出现系统的锚定差异,认知努力也不能作为锚定效应的中介。贝叶斯分析为这些无效发现提供了经验证据。对锚定文献和未来研究的启示进行了讨论。
{"title":"Anchoring and Sleep Inertia.","authors":"Marie-Lena Frech,&nbsp;Jan Alexander Häusser,&nbsp;Marie-Carolin Siems,&nbsp;David D Loschelder","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000552","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000552","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b></b> Many occupational settings require individuals to make important decisions immediately after awakening. Although a plethora of psychological research has separately examined both sleep and anchoring effects on decision-making, little is known about their interaction. In the present study, we seek to shed light on the link between sleep inertia, the performance impairment immediately after awakening, and individuals' susceptibility to the anchoring bias. We proposed that sleep inertia would moderate participants' adjustment from anchors because sleep inertia leads to less cognitive effort invested, resulting in a stronger anchoring effect. One hundred four subjects were randomly assigned to an experimental group that answered anchoring tasks immediately after being awakened at nighttime or a control group that answered anchoring tasks at daytime. Our findings replicated the well-established anchoring effect in that higher anchors led participants to higher estimates than lower anchors. We did not find significant effects of sleep inertia. While the sleep inertia group reported greater sleepiness and having invested <i>less</i> cognitive effort compared to the control group, no systematic anchoring differences emerged, and cognitive effort did not qualify as a mediator of the anchoring effect. Bayesian analyses provide empirical evidence for these null findings. Implications for the anchoring literature and future research are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 3","pages":"146-154"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9645437/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40337556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do Beliefs About Font Size Affect Retrospective Metamemory Judgments in Addition to Prospective Judgments? 除了前瞻判断外,关于字体大小的信念是否影响回顾性元记忆判断?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-17 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000549
Karlos Luna, Pedro B Albuquerque

Beliefs about how memory works explain several effects on prospective metamemory judgments (e.g., the effect of font size on judgments of learning; JOLs). Less is known about the effect of beliefs on retrospective judgments (i.e., confidence). Here, we tested whether font size also affects confidence ratings and whether beliefs play a similar role in confidence than in JOLs. In two experiments, participants studied words in small and large size, rated JOLs, and completed a font-size test in which they indicated the font size at study and a standard old/new recognition test. The results confirmed that font size affected both JOLs and confidence ratings. The presentation of the counter-belief that memory is better for words in small font size in Experiment 2 and the analyses of confidence for participants who did not believe that large fonts improved memory suggested that the effect of font size on confidence was based on beliefs. This research shows that the debate on theory-based and experience-based factors should not be limited to prospective metamemory judgments but also encompass retrospective judgments.

关于记忆如何工作的信念解释了前瞻元记忆判断的几种影响(例如,字体大小对学习判断的影响;约尔)。人们对信念对回溯性判断(即信心)的影响知之甚少。在这里,我们测试了字体大小是否也会影响信心评级,以及信念在信心中的作用是否与joll相似。在两个实验中,参与者学习了大小字体的单词,对JOLs进行了评分,并完成了一个字体大小测试,在测试中他们指出了研究中的字体大小和一个标准的新旧识别测试。结果证实,字体大小对joll和信心评级都有影响。实验2中出现的“小字体记忆效果更好”的反信念,以及对不相信大字体记忆效果的参与者的信心分析表明,字体大小对信心的影响是基于信念的。本研究表明,关于理论因素和经验因素的争论不应局限于前瞻性元记忆判断,而应包括回顾性判断。
{"title":"Do Beliefs About Font Size Affect Retrospective Metamemory Judgments in Addition to Prospective Judgments?","authors":"Karlos Luna,&nbsp;Pedro B Albuquerque","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000549","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000549","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b></b> Beliefs about how memory works explain several effects on prospective metamemory judgments (e.g., the effect of font size on judgments of learning; JOLs). Less is known about the effect of beliefs on retrospective judgments (i.e., confidence). Here, we tested whether font size also affects confidence ratings and whether beliefs play a similar role in confidence than in JOLs. In two experiments, participants studied words in small and large size, rated JOLs, and completed a font-size test in which they indicated the font size at study and a standard old/new recognition test. The results confirmed that font size affected both JOLs and confidence ratings. The presentation of the counter-belief that memory is better for words in small font size in Experiment 2 and the analyses of confidence for participants who did not believe that large fonts improved memory suggested that the effect of font size on confidence was based on beliefs. This research shows that the debate on theory-based and experience-based factors should not be limited to prospective metamemory judgments but also encompass retrospective judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 3","pages":"172-184"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40420471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Collective Punishment and Cheating in the Die-Under-the-Cup Task. 集体惩罚与杯赛任务中的作弊。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-20 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000543
Erez Siniver, Yossef Tobol, Gideon Yaniv
A popular tool in the experimental research on dishonest behavior is the die-under-the-cup (DUTC) task in which subjects roll a die in private and report the outcome to the experimenter after being promised a payoff which increases with the die's outcome. The present paper reports the results of incorporating collective punishment into the DUTC task. We ran two experiments, each involving two rounds of the task performed in a computer lab. Despite being asked not to cheat, the average reported outcome in the first round exceeded the statistical expectancy of 3.5. The second round of the first experiment involved the threat that if this happened again, each subject would be fined by the difference between the average reported outcome and 3.5. Nevertheless, the average reported outcome in the second round significantly exceeded that of the first round. Running a second experiment, this time without the punishment threat, we ruled out the possibility that the increased cheating in the second round of the first experiment was due to a feedback effect, concluding that the threat of collective punishment acted to encourage cheating rather than helped deterring it.
在对不诚实行为的实验研究中,一个流行的工具是杯子下的骰子(DUTC)任务。在这个任务中,受试者私下掷骰子,并在得到奖励后向实验者报告结果,奖励随着骰子的结果而增加。本文报告了将集体惩罚纳入DUTC任务的结果。我们进行了两个实验,每个实验都包括在计算机实验室进行的两轮任务。尽管被要求不要作弊,第一轮报告的平均结果还是超过了统计预期的3.5分。第一个实验的第二轮涉及到这样的威胁:如果这种情况再次发生,每个受试者将被平均报告结果与3.5之间的差异所罚款。然而,第二轮报告的平均结果明显超过了第一轮。在没有惩罚威胁的情况下,我们进行了第二个实验,排除了第一个实验第二轮作弊增加是由于反馈效应的可能性,得出的结论是,集体惩罚的威胁起到了鼓励作弊的作用,而不是起到了阻止作弊的作用。
{"title":"Collective Punishment and Cheating in the Die-Under-the-Cup Task.","authors":"Erez Siniver, Yossef Tobol, Gideon Yaniv","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000543","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000543","url":null,"abstract":"A popular tool in the experimental research on dishonest behavior is the die-under-the-cup (DUTC) task in which subjects roll a die in private and report the outcome to the experimenter after being promised a payoff which increases with the die's outcome. The present paper reports the results of incorporating collective punishment into the DUTC task. We ran two experiments, each involving two rounds of the task performed in a computer lab. Despite being asked not to cheat, the average reported outcome in the first round exceeded the statistical expectancy of 3.5. The second round of the first experiment involved the threat that if this happened again, each subject would be fined by the difference between the average reported outcome and 3.5. Nevertheless, the average reported outcome in the second round significantly exceeded that of the first round. Running a second experiment, this time without the punishment threat, we ruled out the possibility that the increased cheating in the second round of the first experiment was due to a feedback effect, concluding that the threat of collective punishment acted to encourage cheating rather than helped deterring it.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42268727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Pervasive Problem of Post Hoc Data Selection in Studies on Unconscious Processing. 无意识处理研究中普遍存在的后置数据选择问题。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-11 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000541
Marcus Rothkirch, D. Shanks, G. Hesselmann
Studies on unconscious mental processes typically require that participants are unaware of some information (e.g., a visual stimulus). An important methodological question in this field of research is how to deal with data from participants who become aware of the critical stimulus according to some measure of awareness. While it has previously been argued that the post hoc selection of participants dependent on an awareness measure may often result in regression-to-the-mean artifacts (Shanks, 2017), a recent article (Sklar et al., 2021) challenged this conclusion claiming that the consideration of this statistical artifact might lead to unjustified rejections of true unconscious influences. In this reply, we explain this pervasive statistical problem with a basic and concrete example, show that Sklar et al. fundamentally mischaracterize it, and then refute the argument that the influence of the artifact has previously been overestimated. We conclude that, without safeguards, the method of post hoc data selection should never be employed in studies on unconscious processing.
对无意识心理过程的研究通常要求参与者不知道一些信息(例如视觉刺激)。这一研究领域的一个重要方法论问题是,如何处理参与者根据某种意识程度意识到关键刺激的数据。虽然之前有人认为,依赖于意识测量的参与者的事后选择往往会导致回归到平均假象(Shanks,2017),但最近的一篇文章(Sklar et al.,2021)对这一结论提出了质疑,声称对这种统计假象的考虑可能会导致对真正的无意识影响的不合理拒绝。在这个回复中,我们用一个基本而具体的例子解释了这个普遍存在的统计问题,表明Sklar等人从根本上错误地描述了它,然后反驳了人工制品的影响以前被高估的说法。我们的结论是,如果没有保障措施,事后数据选择的方法永远不应该用于无意识处理的研究。
{"title":"The Pervasive Problem of Post Hoc Data Selection in Studies on Unconscious Processing.","authors":"Marcus Rothkirch, D. Shanks, G. Hesselmann","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000541","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000541","url":null,"abstract":"Studies on unconscious mental processes typically require that participants are unaware of some information (e.g., a visual stimulus). An important methodological question in this field of research is how to deal with data from participants who become aware of the critical stimulus according to some measure of awareness. While it has previously been argued that the post hoc selection of participants dependent on an awareness measure may often result in regression-to-the-mean artifacts (Shanks, 2017), a recent article (Sklar et al., 2021) challenged this conclusion claiming that the consideration of this statistical artifact might lead to unjustified rejections of true unconscious influences. In this reply, we explain this pervasive statistical problem with a basic and concrete example, show that Sklar et al. fundamentally mischaracterize it, and then refute the argument that the influence of the artifact has previously been overestimated. We conclude that, without safeguards, the method of post hoc data selection should never be employed in studies on unconscious processing.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48151513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The Nature of Word Associations in Sentence Contexts. 句子语境中单词联想的本质
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-13 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000547
Clara Planchuelo, Francisco Buades-Sitjar, José Antonio Hinojosa, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia

How words are interrelated in the human mind is a scientific topic on which there is still no consensus, with different views on how word co-occurrence and semantic relatedness mediate word association. Recent research has shown that lexical associations are strongly predicted by the similarity of those words in terms of valence, arousal, and concreteness ratings. In the current study, we aimed at extending these results to more complex and realistic linguistic scenarios, since human communication is not done with word pairs, but rather through sentences. Hence, the aim of the current study was to verify whether valence, arousal, and concreteness also articulate sentence-level lexical representations. To this end, 32 native Spanish speakers were given cue words and asked to use them in sentences that would provide a meaningful context. The content words of the written sentences were then analyzed. Our results showed that the emotional dimensions (valence and arousal) and concreteness values of the cue words effectively predicted the same values of said dimensions of their sentences' words. In sum, the similarity in the emotional dimensions and concreteness are crucial mechanisms behind word association in the human mind.

摘要单词在人类思维中是如何相互关联的,这是一个尚未达成共识的科学话题,对于单词共现和语义关联如何介导单词联想,人们有不同的看法。最近的研究表明,词汇联想可以通过这些单词在配价、唤起和具体性评级方面的相似性来强烈预测。在目前的研究中,我们的目的是将这些结果扩展到更复杂和现实的语言场景中,因为人类的交流不是通过单词对进行的,而是通过句子进行的。因此,本研究的目的是验证配价、唤醒和具体性是否也能表达句子层面的词汇表征。为此,向32名以西班牙语为母语的人提供了提示词,并要求他们在提供有意义的上下文的句子中使用这些提示词。然后对书面句子的内容词进行分析。我们的研究结果表明,提示词的情感维度(效价和唤醒)和具体性值有效地预测了句子单词的所述维度的相同值。总之,情感维度的相似性和具体性是人类思维中单词联想的关键机制。
{"title":"The Nature of Word Associations in Sentence Contexts.","authors":"Clara Planchuelo, Francisco Buades-Sitjar, José Antonio Hinojosa, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000547","DOIUrl":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000547","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b></b> How words are interrelated in the human mind is a scientific topic on which there is still no consensus, with different views on how word co-occurrence and semantic relatedness mediate word association. Recent research has shown that lexical associations are strongly predicted by the similarity of those words in terms of valence, arousal, and concreteness ratings. In the current study, we aimed at extending these results to more complex and realistic linguistic scenarios, since human communication is not done with word pairs, but rather through sentences. Hence, the aim of the current study was to verify whether valence, arousal, and concreteness also articulate sentence-level lexical representations. To this end, 32 native Spanish speakers were given cue words and asked to use them in sentences that would provide a meaningful context. The content words of the written sentences were then analyzed. Our results showed that the emotional dimensions (valence and arousal) and concreteness values of the cue words effectively predicted the same values of said dimensions of their sentences' words. In sum, the similarity in the emotional dimensions and concreteness are crucial mechanisms behind word association in the human mind.</p>","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 1","pages":"104-110"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9386508/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41540607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone. 寻找偏好反转区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000542
Steven Glautier,Hedwig Eisenbarth,Anne Macaskill
A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.
当两个奖励在时间上更接近时,对大晚(LL)奖励的偏好比小早(SS)奖励的偏好逆转时,可以观察到偏好逆转。偏好反转在日常生活和实验室中很常见,并且经常被声称支持双曲延迟贴现模型,在其最简单的形式中,可以用一个自由参数来模拟反转。然而,目前尚不清楚偏好逆转的时间位置是否可以先验地预测。测试模型预测的研究并没有找到支持它们的证据,但它们忽视了强化物大小对贴现率的充分证明的影响。因此,我们直接测试了双曲线和指数模型预测在预登记的研究中,通过评估个人贴现率的两个强化幅度。然后,我们对可能发生偏好逆转的成对选择进行了个性化预测。在107名参与者中,我们发现(1)很少有证据表明双曲模型和指数模型可以预测偏好逆转的时间位置,(2)一些证据表明双曲模型比指数模型具有更好的预测性能,(3)与许多先前的研究相比,指数模型通常比双曲模型更适合观察数据。
{"title":"In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone.","authors":"Steven Glautier,Hedwig Eisenbarth,Anne Macaskill","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000542","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000542","url":null,"abstract":"A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"5 1","pages":"46-59"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138532231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Production Effect Interacts With Serial Positions 生产效应与序列位置相互作用
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000540
Sébastien Gionet, D. Guitard, J. Saint-Aubin
Abstract: Reading some words aloud during presentation, that is, producing them, and reading other words silently generate a large memory advantage for words that are produced. This robust within-list production effect is in contrast with the between-lists condition in which all words are read aloud or silently. In a between-lists condition, produced items are better recognized, but not better recalled. The lack of a between-lists production effect with recall tasks has often been presented as one of its defining characteristics and as a benchmark for evaluating models. Recently, Cyr et al. (2021) showed that this occurs because item production interacts with serial positions: Produced items are less well recalled on the first serial positions than silently read items, while the reverse pattern is observed for the recency portion of the curve. However, this pattern was observed with a repeated-measures design, and it may be a by-product of compensatory processes under the control of participants. Here, using a between-participants design, we observed the predicted interaction between production and serial positions. The results further support the Revised Feature Model (RFM) suggesting that produced items are encoded with more modality-dependent distinctive features, therefore benefiting recall. However, the production of the additional distinctive features would disrupt rehearsal.
摘要:在演讲过程中,大声朗读一些单词,即朗读这些单词,而默读其他单词,对所朗读的单词有很大的记忆优势。这种健壮的列表内生成效果与列表间条件形成对比,在列表间条件下,所有单词都被大声或静默地读出。在中间列表的条件下,生产的产品被更好地识别,但不是更好地召回。缺乏列表间产生效应的召回任务经常被提出作为其定义特征之一,并作为评估模型的基准。最近,Cyr等人(2021)表明,这种情况的发生是因为物品生产与序列位置相互作用:在第一个序列位置上,生产的物品比静默读取的物品回忆得更少,而在曲线的最近部分,观察到相反的模式。然而,这种模式是通过重复测量设计观察到的,它可能是参与者控制下代偿过程的副产品。在这里,使用参与者之间的设计,我们观察到生产和系列位置之间的预测交互作用。结果进一步支持了修正特征模型(RFM),表明生产出来的物品被编码了更多与模态相关的显著特征,因此有利于回忆。然而,制作额外的特色会干扰排练。
{"title":"The Production Effect Interacts With Serial Positions","authors":"Sébastien Gionet, D. Guitard, J. Saint-Aubin","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000540","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000540","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract: Reading some words aloud during presentation, that is, producing them, and reading other words silently generate a large memory advantage for words that are produced. This robust within-list production effect is in contrast with the between-lists condition in which all words are read aloud or silently. In a between-lists condition, produced items are better recognized, but not better recalled. The lack of a between-lists production effect with recall tasks has often been presented as one of its defining characteristics and as a benchmark for evaluating models. Recently, Cyr et al. (2021) showed that this occurs because item production interacts with serial positions: Produced items are less well recalled on the first serial positions than silently read items, while the reverse pattern is observed for the recency portion of the curve. However, this pattern was observed with a repeated-measures design, and it may be a by-product of compensatory processes under the control of participants. Here, using a between-participants design, we observed the predicted interaction between production and serial positions. The results further support the Revised Feature Model (RFM) suggesting that produced items are encoded with more modality-dependent distinctive features, therefore benefiting recall. However, the production of the additional distinctive features would disrupt rehearsal.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 1","pages":"12 - 22"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41651118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone. 寻找偏好反转区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk
S. Glautier, H. Eisenbarth, Anne C. Macaskill
A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.
当两个奖励在时间上更接近时,对大晚(LL)奖励的偏好比小早(SS)奖励的偏好逆转时,可以观察到偏好逆转。偏好反转在日常生活和实验室中很常见,并且经常被声称支持双曲延迟贴现模型,在其最简单的形式中,可以用一个自由参数来模拟反转。然而,目前尚不清楚偏好逆转的时间位置是否可以先验地预测。测试模型预测的研究并没有找到支持它们的证据,但它们忽视了强化物大小对贴现率的充分证明的影响。因此,我们直接测试了双曲线和指数模型预测在预登记的研究中,通过评估个人贴现率的两个强化幅度。然后,我们对可能发生偏好逆转的成对选择进行了个性化预测。在107名参与者中,我们发现(1)很少有证据表明双曲模型和指数模型可以预测偏好逆转的时间位置,(2)一些证据表明双曲模型比指数模型具有更好的预测性能,(3)与许多先前的研究相比,指数模型通常比双曲模型更适合观察数据。
{"title":"In Search of the Preference Reversal Zone.","authors":"S. Glautier, H. Eisenbarth, Anne C. Macaskill","doi":"10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/zg6fk","url":null,"abstract":"A preference reversal is observed when a preference for a larger-later (LL) reward over a smaller-sooner (SS) reward reverses as both rewards come closer in time. Preference reversals are common in everyday life and in the laboratory and are often claimed to support hyperbolic delay-discounting models which, in their simplest form, can model reversals with only one free parameter. However, it is not clear if the temporal location of preference reversals can be predicted a priori. Studies testing model predictions have not found support for them, but they overlooked the well-documented effect of reinforcer magnitude on discounting rate. Therefore, we directly tested hyperbolic and exponential model predictions in a pre-registered study by assessing individual discount rates for two reinforcer magnitudes. We then made individualized predictions about pairs of choices between which preference reversals should occur. With 107 participants, we found (1) little evidence that hyperbolic and exponential models could predict the temporal location of preference reversals, (2) some evidence that hyperbolic models had better predictive performance than exponential models, and (3) in contrast to many previous studies, that exponential models generally produced superior fits to the observed data than hyperbolic models.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 1 1","pages":"46-59"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41612516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Functional Specificity of the Affordance of Reaching. 可达性的功能特异性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000544
Tyler Surber, Tyler Overstreet, Hannah L Masoner, Catherine Dowell, A. Hajnal
The information that specifies whether an object is within reach is a complex pattern that depends on body-scaled parameters measured from an egocentric reference point. The pattern is a function of relevant body proportions (eye height, shoulder height [SH], arm length) with respect to the spatial location of the target object. In addition to not knowing how these factors map onto perception, it is also not known whether the egocentric viewpoint is centered at the eye or the shoulder. In three experiments, we systematically tested whether observers can perceive eye height and SH (Experiment 1), whether they can point accurately in the direction of a target object (Experiment 2), and whether they can point accurately to judge if the target object is within reach (Experiment 3). Experiment 1 demonstrated that participants are more accurate at judging their own eye height than SH. Experiment 2 revealed that participants can more accurately point to a target object's location when measured from the shoulder as a reference point than when measured from the eye. In Experiment 3, we showed that a higher-order variable that includes arm length, body height, and angle of declination to the target successfully predicted affordance judgments, regardless of a reference point. We consider this as evidence that the invariant is functionally specific, not tied to any one particular anatomical body part.
指定物体是否在可及范围内的信息是一个复杂的模式,它取决于从以自我为中心的参考点测量的身体尺度参数。该模式是相对于目标物体的空间位置的相关身体比例(眼高、肩高[SH]、臂长)的函数。除了不知道这些因素如何映射到感知之外,也不知道以自我为中心的观点是集中在眼睛还是肩膀上。在三个实验中,我们系统地测试了观察者是否能够感知眼睛高度和高度(实验1),他们是否能够准确地指向目标物体的方向(实验2),实验1表明,被试判断自己的眼睛高度比SH更准确。实验2表明,被试以肩膀为参考点比以眼睛为参考点能更准确地指出目标物体的位置。在实验3中,我们展示了一个高阶变量,包括臂长、身体高度和俯角到目标的角度,成功地预测了功能判断,而与参考点无关。我们认为这是不变量是功能特异性的证据,而不是与任何一个特定的身体解剖部位相关联。
{"title":"Functional Specificity of the Affordance of Reaching.","authors":"Tyler Surber, Tyler Overstreet, Hannah L Masoner, Catherine Dowell, A. Hajnal","doi":"10.1027/1618-3169/a000544","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000544","url":null,"abstract":"The information that specifies whether an object is within reach is a complex pattern that depends on body-scaled parameters measured from an egocentric reference point. The pattern is a function of relevant body proportions (eye height, shoulder height [SH], arm length) with respect to the spatial location of the target object. In addition to not knowing how these factors map onto perception, it is also not known whether the egocentric viewpoint is centered at the eye or the shoulder. In three experiments, we systematically tested whether observers can perceive eye height and SH (Experiment 1), whether they can point accurately in the direction of a target object (Experiment 2), and whether they can point accurately to judge if the target object is within reach (Experiment 3). Experiment 1 demonstrated that participants are more accurate at judging their own eye height than SH. Experiment 2 revealed that participants can more accurately point to a target object's location when measured from the shoulder as a reference point than when measured from the eye. In Experiment 3, we showed that a higher-order variable that includes arm length, body height, and angle of declination to the target successfully predicted affordance judgments, regardless of a reference point. We consider this as evidence that the invariant is functionally specific, not tied to any one particular anatomical body part.","PeriodicalId":12173,"journal":{"name":"Experimental psychology","volume":"69 1 1","pages":"23-39"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48854641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Experimental psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1