首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
Occupation-related differences in cognitive aging: Comparative effects of job type, skill level, and education 认知老化中与职业相关的差异:工作类型、技能水平和教育程度的比较效应
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101877
Stephen Aichele , Antony Payton , Andrew C. Robinson , Patrick Rabbitt
A century of psychometric research has shown that intelligence is robustly associated with occupational status. Despite a rapidly aging global workforce, occupational differences in cognitive decline remain under-investigated. In a large sample of middle-aged and older adults (N = 5542; age 41–97 years; 70.6 % female), we compared age-based trajectories of general fluid cognition across occupational groups (categorized both by specialization area and skill level). Occupational grouping accounted for 18.6 % of variability in baseline cognitive performance and 3.9 % of variability in rates of decline. Cognitive differences across occupational groups generally followed a skill gradient. These differences were largely retained with increasing age—although between-group variability in rates of decline were also present. Moreover, occupation-cognition associations remained significant after adjustment for education (occupation contributed an additional 5.9 % and 1.8 % to explained variation in baseline cognitive performance and decline in performance, respectively). Having more hobbies in later life accounted for an additional 2.7 % and 1.2 % in explained variation for baseline differences and declines in cognition, respectively. These associations were minimally affected by further adjustment for sociodemographic and lifestyle covariates, including retirement status. The marked contrast between the cognitive trajectories of academic professionals vs. those of other occupational groups suggests that long-term immersive intellectual engagement may provide tangible benefits for cognitive aging.
一个世纪的心理测量研究表明,智力与职业状况密切相关。尽管全球劳动力迅速老龄化,但认知能力下降的职业差异仍未得到充分研究。我们对大量中老年人样本(样本数:5542;年龄:41-97 岁;70.6% 为女性)进行了研究,比较了不同职业群体(按专业领域和技能水平分类)基于年龄的一般流体认知轨迹。职业分组占基线认知能力差异的 18.6%,占下降率差异的 3.9%。不同职业类别之间的认知差异一般呈技能梯度分布。随着年龄的增长,这些差异在很大程度上得以保持--尽管在下降率方面也存在组间差异。此外,在对教育程度进行调整后,职业与认知之间的关联仍然显著(职业分别为基线认知能力和能力下降的变异提供了 5.9% 和 1.8% 的额外解释)。晚年拥有更多爱好分别对基线差异和认知能力下降的解释变量增加了 2.7% 和 1.2%。进一步调整社会人口学和生活方式协变量(包括退休状况)后,这些关联的影响微乎其微。学术界专业人士与其他职业群体的认知轨迹之间的明显对比表明,长期沉浸式的智力活动可能会为认知老化带来切实的益处。
{"title":"Occupation-related differences in cognitive aging: Comparative effects of job type, skill level, and education","authors":"Stephen Aichele ,&nbsp;Antony Payton ,&nbsp;Andrew C. Robinson ,&nbsp;Patrick Rabbitt","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101877","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101877","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>A century of psychometric research has shown that intelligence is robustly associated with occupational status. Despite a rapidly aging global workforce, occupational differences in cognitive decline remain under-investigated. In a large sample of middle-aged and older adults (<em>N</em> = 5542; age 41–97 years; 70.6 % female), we compared age-based trajectories of general fluid cognition across occupational groups (categorized both by specialization area and skill level). Occupational grouping accounted for 18.6 % of variability in baseline cognitive performance and 3.9 % of variability in rates of decline. Cognitive differences across occupational groups generally followed a skill gradient. These differences were largely retained with increasing age—although between-group variability in rates of decline were also present. Moreover, occupation-cognition associations remained significant after adjustment for education (occupation contributed an additional 5.9 % and 1.8 % to explained variation in baseline cognitive performance and decline in performance, respectively). Having more hobbies in later life accounted for an additional 2.7 % and 1.2 % in explained variation for baseline differences and declines in cognition, respectively. These associations were minimally affected by further adjustment for sociodemographic and lifestyle covariates, including retirement status. The marked contrast between the cognitive trajectories of academic professionals vs. those of other occupational groups suggests that long-term immersive intellectual engagement may provide tangible benefits for cognitive aging.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101877"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142658770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A special contribution from spatial ability to math word problem solving: Evidence from structural equation modelling and network analysis 空间能力对数学单词解题的特殊贡献:来自结构方程模型和网络分析的证据
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101875
Maxim Likhanov , Fang Wang , Jianing Lyu , Li Wang , Xinlin Zhou
There is a growing body of research into the factors contributing to math word problem solving. However, these studies usually use limited number of potential predictors (precluding assessing of their contribution in comparison with other factors or “g” general intelligence) and some predictors (such as analogical and hypothetical reasoning) are largely omitted. Thus, the aim of the current study was to explore contributions of different types of reasoning to math word problem solving and whether these contributions have added value compared with each other and general cognitive ability. Chinese schoolchildren in Grades 3 (N = 199; Mage = 102.4 months), 4 (N = 162; Mage = 114.6), 5 (N = 174; Mage = 126.1) and 6 (N = 180; Mage = 138.6) completed 8 tasks tapping into spatial, mechanical, verbal, mathematic, hypothetical and analogical reasoning. Our data showed that when 6 general cognitive factors load onto General cognitive ability factor in a Structural Equation Model (SEM), only spatial visualization has additional contribution to Word problem solving factor. Gaussian Graphical models (GGMs) showed that 2 verbal tasks and spatial visualization showed stable (present in at least 3 out of 4 grades) contributions to both word problem solving tasks. Analogical reasoning showed contribution to process of word problem solving only. To sum up, both SEM and GGMs converged on the importance of spatial ability for math word problems solving. Our results call for verbal and spatial ability to be routinely assessed and targeted by educational interventions within math curriculum.
关于数学单词解题的因素的研究越来越多。然而,这些研究通常使用的潜在预测因素数量有限(无法评估这些因素与其他因素或 "g "一般智力相比的贡献),而且有些预测因素(如类比推理和假设推理)在很大程度上被忽略。因此,本研究旨在探讨不同类型的推理对数学单词解题的贡献,以及这些贡献与其他推理和一般认知能力相比是否具有附加价值。中国三年级(N = 199;Mage = 102.4 个月)、四年级(N = 162;Mage = 114.6)、五年级(N = 174;Mage = 126.1)和六年级(N = 180;Mage = 138.6)的学生分别完成了空间推理、机械推理、语言推理、数学推理、假设推理和类比推理等 8 项任务。我们的数据显示,在结构方程模型(SEM)中,当 6 个一般认知因子加载到一般认知能力因子上时,只有空间可视化对文字问题解决因子有额外的贡献。高斯图形模型(GGMs)显示,两项言语任务和空间可视化对两项文字问题解决任务都有稳定的贡献(在四个年级中至少有三个年级存在)。类比推理只对文字解题过程有贡献。总之,SEM 和 GGM 都一致认为空间能力对于数学文字问题的解决非常重要。我们的研究结果要求在数学课程中对言语和空间能力进行常规评估,并采取有针对性的教育干预措施。
{"title":"A special contribution from spatial ability to math word problem solving: Evidence from structural equation modelling and network analysis","authors":"Maxim Likhanov ,&nbsp;Fang Wang ,&nbsp;Jianing Lyu ,&nbsp;Li Wang ,&nbsp;Xinlin Zhou","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101875","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101875","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>There is a growing body of research into the factors contributing to math word problem solving. However, these studies usually use limited number of potential predictors (precluding assessing of their contribution in comparison with other factors or “g” general intelligence) and some predictors (such as analogical and hypothetical reasoning) are largely omitted. Thus, the aim of the current study was to explore contributions of different types of reasoning to math word problem solving and whether these contributions have added value compared with each other and general cognitive ability. Chinese schoolchildren in Grades 3 (<em>N</em> = 199; Mage = 102.4 months), 4 (<em>N</em> = 162; Mage = 114.6), 5 (<em>N</em> = 174; Mage = 126.1) and 6 (<em>N</em> = 180; Mage = 138.6) completed 8 tasks tapping into spatial, mechanical, verbal, mathematic, hypothetical and analogical reasoning. Our data showed that when 6 general cognitive factors load onto General cognitive ability factor in a Structural Equation Model (SEM), only spatial visualization has additional contribution to Word problem solving factor. Gaussian Graphical models (GGMs) showed that 2 verbal tasks and spatial visualization showed stable (present in at least 3 out of 4 grades) contributions to both word problem solving tasks. Analogical reasoning showed contribution to process of word problem solving only. To sum up, both SEM and GGMs converged on the importance of spatial ability for math word problems solving. Our results call for verbal and spatial ability to be routinely assessed and targeted by educational interventions within math curriculum.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101875"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142594146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating measurement invariance of the IDS-2 intelligence scales between migrant and non-migrant groups 研究 IDS-2 智力测验量表在移民群体和非移民群体之间的测量不变性
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101873
Lily Gantscheva, Martin Steppan, Alexander Grob
Intelligence plays a crucial role in various aspects of human life, impacting health, academic achievement, and socio-economic success. However, cultural and linguistic disparities in intelligence testing pose challenges, particularly for individuals from migrant backgrounds. This study replicates and extends the landmark study by Wicherts and Dolan (2010) exploring measurement invariance of the German language intelligence test, the Intelligence and Development Scales – 2 (IDS-2), between children and adolescents from migrant (N = 132) and non-migrant (N = 1898) groups. The results revealed partial strict measurement invariance in the IDS-2 intelligence scale subtests across the examined groups. The breach of full strict measurement invariance is primarily due to intercept differences on the verbally loaded subtests—Naming Categories, Naming Opposites, and Story Recall—highlighting the confounding impact of language complexity on test outcomes. These discrepancies resulted in a cumulative intercept difference disadvantaging migrant participants of approximately 4 IQ points on the Full-Scale IQ Score. The findings indicate that while the IDS-2 scales generally assess intelligence consistently across diverse groups, the influence of language complexity on the verbal subtests may result in a disadvantage for children and adolescents with migration backgrounds. To address these biases, we propose the development of non-verbal and culturally fair intelligence tests.
智力在人类生活的各个方面都起着至关重要的作用,影响着健康、学业成绩和社会经济成就。然而,智力测试中的文化和语言差异带来了挑战,尤其是对来自移民背景的个人而言。本研究复制并扩展了 Wicherts 和 Dolan(2010 年)的一项里程碑式的研究,该研究探讨了德语智力测验--《智力与发展量表-2》(IDS-2)--在移民群体(132 人)和非移民群体(1898 人)的儿童和青少年之间的测量不变性。研究结果表明,IDS-2 智力量表各分测验在受试群体间存在部分严格测量不变性。违反完全严格测量不变量的主要原因是语言负荷分测验--命名类别、命名对立面和故事回忆--的截距差异,凸显了语言复杂性对测验结果的混杂影响。这些差异导致移民参与者在智商满分上的累计截距差异约为 4 个智商点。研究结果表明,虽然IDS-2量表对不同群体的智力评估总体上是一致的,但语言复杂性对言语分测验的影响可能会导致有移民背景的儿童和青少年处于不利地位。为了解决这些偏差,我们建议开发非语言和文化公平的智力测验。
{"title":"Investigating measurement invariance of the IDS-2 intelligence scales between migrant and non-migrant groups","authors":"Lily Gantscheva,&nbsp;Martin Steppan,&nbsp;Alexander Grob","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101873","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101873","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Intelligence plays a crucial role in various aspects of human life, impacting health, academic achievement, and socio-economic success. However, cultural and linguistic disparities in intelligence testing pose challenges, particularly for individuals from migrant backgrounds. This study replicates and extends the landmark study by Wicherts and Dolan (2010) exploring measurement invariance of the German language intelligence test, the Intelligence and Development Scales – 2 (IDS-2), between children and adolescents from migrant (<em>N</em> = 132) and non-migrant (<em>N</em> = 1898) groups. The results revealed partial strict measurement invariance in the IDS-2 intelligence scale subtests across the examined groups. The breach of full strict measurement invariance is primarily due to intercept differences on the verbally loaded subtests—Naming Categories, Naming Opposites, and Story Recall—highlighting the confounding impact of language complexity on test outcomes. These discrepancies resulted in a cumulative intercept difference disadvantaging migrant participants of approximately 4 IQ points on the Full-Scale IQ Score. The findings indicate that while the IDS-2 scales generally assess intelligence consistently across diverse groups, the influence of language complexity on the verbal subtests may result in a disadvantage for children and adolescents with migration backgrounds. To address these biases, we propose the development of non-verbal and culturally fair intelligence tests.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101873"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142528448","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investment theory and tilt: Evidence from jobs and job families 投资理论与倾斜:来自工作和工作家庭的证据
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-10-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101872
Thomas R. Carretta , Malcolm James Ree
Cattell's (1987) investment theory states that individuals make choices to “invest” their cognitive ability in some areas but not in others. The theory suggests that individuals should gravitate to training and occupations that align with their investments. To test this theory, scores reflecting academic ability (ACAD) and technical knowledge (TECH) were derived from the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery for a large sample of United States Air Force enlistees. The ACAD and TECH scores were used to create an index of ACAD-TECH tilt. Analyses were performed separately for 19 training courses with a technical knowledge requirement (mechanical or electronic) and 34 courses with no technical knowledge requirement. Consistent with investment theory, most of the technical training courses (63 %) had a technical tilt (TECH > ACAD), whereas most of the non-technical training courses (88 %) had an academic tilt (ACAD > TECH). ACAD had the strongest correlation with training grades for both the technical (r = 0.386) and non-technical (r = 0.318) courses. Tilt demonstrated weaker correlations with training grades than either ACAD or TECH for both the technical and non-technical courses. Final School Grade (FSG) was regressed on ACAD, TECH, and Sex. Similar results were observed for all courses, technical courses only, and non-technical courses only. ACAD was significantly correlated with FSG, with little incremental validity for either TECH (Δ r from 0.000 to 0.010) or the contribution of TECH and Sex (Δ r from 0.001 to 0.012).
卡特尔(1987 年)的投资理论认为,个人会选择将其认知能力 "投资 "于某些领域,而不是其他领域。该理论认为,个人应倾向于与其投资相一致的培训和职业。为了验证这一理论,我们从美国空军应征入伍者的大量样本中提取了反映学术能力(ACAD)和技术知识(TECH)的分数。ACAD 和 TECH 分数被用来创建 ACAD-TECH 倾斜指数。对 19 门有技术知识要求(机械或电子)的培训课程和 34 门无技术知识要求的课程分别进行了分析。与投资理论一致,大多数技术培训课程(63%)具有技术倾斜(TECH > ACAD),而大多数非技术培训课程(88%)具有学术倾斜(ACAD > TECH)。在技术课程(r = 0.386)和非技术课程(r = 0.318)中,ACAD 与培训成绩的相关性最强。在技术课程和非技术课程中,倾斜度与培训成绩的相关性均弱于 ACAD 或 TECH。最终学业成绩(FSG)对 ACAD、TECH 和性别进行了回归。所有课程、仅技术课程和仅非技术课程的结果相似。ACAD 与 FSG 有明显的相关性,而 TECH(Δ r 从 0.000 到 0.010)或 TECH 和性别(Δ r 从 0.001 到 0.012)的贡献几乎没有增效作用。
{"title":"Investment theory and tilt: Evidence from jobs and job families","authors":"Thomas R. Carretta ,&nbsp;Malcolm James Ree","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101872","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101872","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Cattell's (1987) investment theory states that individuals make choices to “invest” their cognitive ability in some areas but not in others. The theory suggests that individuals should gravitate to training and occupations that align with their investments. To test this theory, scores reflecting academic ability (ACAD) and technical knowledge (TECH) were derived from the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery for a large sample of United States Air Force enlistees. The ACAD and TECH scores were used to create an index of ACAD-TECH tilt. Analyses were performed separately for 19 training courses with a technical knowledge requirement (mechanical or electronic) and 34 courses with no technical knowledge requirement. Consistent with investment theory, most of the technical training courses (63 %) had a technical tilt (TECH &gt; ACAD), whereas most of the non-technical training courses (88 %) had an academic tilt (ACAD &gt; TECH). ACAD had the strongest correlation with training grades for both the technical (<em>r</em> = 0.386) and non-technical (<em>r</em> = 0.318) courses. Tilt demonstrated weaker correlations with training grades than either ACAD or TECH for both the technical and non-technical courses. Final School Grade (FSG) was regressed on ACAD, TECH, and Sex. Similar results were observed for all courses, technical courses only, and non-technical courses only. ACAD was significantly correlated with FSG, with little incremental validity for either TECH (Δ <em>r</em> from 0.000 to 0.010) or the contribution of TECH and Sex (Δ <em>r</em> from 0.001 to 0.012).</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101872"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142528446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
DNA and IQ: Big deal or much ado about nothing? – A meta-analysis DNA 和智商:大事还是小事?- 荟萃分析
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-10-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101871
Florence A.R. Oxley , Kirsty Wilding , Sophie von Stumm
Intelligence is polygenic, highly heritable, and predicts wide-ranging life outcomes. Here, we meta-analysed the predictive validity of polygenic scores for intelligence based on the largest available genome-wide association study (or GWAS; Savage et al., 2018) for tested, phenotypic intelligence to date. Across 32 estimates from 9 independent samples, which all came from WEIRD countries and were of European ancestry (Ntotal = 452,864), our meta-analytic estimate for the association between polygenic and phenotypic intelligence was ρ = 0.245 (p < .001, 95 % CI = 0.184–0.307), an effect of medium size. The meta-analytic estimate varied across samples, studies, and phenotypic measures of intelligence, and even after accounting for these moderators, polygenic score predictions remained significantly heterogenous. Our findings support claims that polygenic predictions of intelligence benefit and advance research but their utility in other contexts is yet to be demonstrated.
智力是多基因的,具有高度遗传性,并能预测各种生活结果。在此,我们基于迄今为止最大的全基因组关联研究(或称 GWAS;Savage 等人,2018 年),对智力多基因评分的预测有效性进行了荟萃分析。来自9个独立样本的32个估计值均来自WEIRD国家,且均为欧洲血统(Ntotal = 452 864),我们对多基因与表型智力之间关联的元分析估计值为ρ = 0.245(p < .001,95 % CI = 0.184-0.307),属于中等规模效应。荟萃分析的估计值因样本、研究和智力表型测量而异,即使考虑了这些调节因素,多基因得分预测仍存在显著差异。我们的研究结果支持了多基因智力预测有利于和推动研究的说法,但它们在其他情况下的效用还有待证明。
{"title":"DNA and IQ: Big deal or much ado about nothing? – A meta-analysis","authors":"Florence A.R. Oxley ,&nbsp;Kirsty Wilding ,&nbsp;Sophie von Stumm","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101871","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101871","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Intelligence is polygenic, highly heritable, and predicts wide-ranging life outcomes. Here, we meta-analysed the predictive validity of polygenic scores for intelligence based on the largest available genome-wide association study (or <em>GWAS</em>; Savage et al., 2018) for tested, phenotypic intelligence to date. Across 32 estimates from 9 independent samples, which all came from WEIRD countries and were of European ancestry (N<sub>total</sub> = 452,864), our meta-analytic estimate for the association between polygenic and phenotypic intelligence was <em>ρ</em> <em>=</em> <em>0</em>.245 (<em>p</em> <em>&lt;</em> .001, 95 % CI = 0.184–0.307), an effect of medium size. The meta-analytic estimate varied across samples, studies, and phenotypic measures of intelligence, and even after accounting for these moderators, polygenic score predictions remained significantly heterogenous. Our findings support claims that polygenic predictions of intelligence benefit and advance research but their utility in other contexts is yet to be demonstrated.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101871"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142528447","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ecological cognitive assessment has incremental validity for predicting academic performance over and above single occasion cognitive assessments 生态认知评估在预测学习成绩方面的有效性高于单一场合认知评估的有效性
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101869
Arabella C. Vaughan, Damian P. Birney
Throughout the history of psychometric testing, cognitive ability has predominantly been construed as an ability that differs between individuals but not within individuals. As a result, the influence of time and context on within-person variation in cognitive performance has not been well explored. This is despite the fact that the criterion outcomes that cognitive assessments are used to predict, such as educational and workplace performance, inherently encompass performance variability over time and context. In this paper, we provided novel evidence that ecological cognitive assessment has incremental validity for predicting academic performance over and above single occasion cognitive assessments. We proposed the use of a cognitive Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) approach to generate short-term repeated measures of cognitive performance and recommended a set of parameters that can be used to describe ecological within-person variation in EMA data. We then empirically tested these parameters across two studies in which participants completed a series of cognitive tasks delivered via the EMA as well as a traditional single occasion cognitive assessment that is time and context-invariant. Our findings showed that a range of ecological cognitive performance parameters had incremental utility for predicting first year university performance above and beyond the traditional cognitive assessment. Further, this appeared to occur because ecological cognitive performance parameters describe some aspect of cognitive performance not captured by the traditional time and context-invariant assessment. We suggest that what these parameters capture is information about an individual's typical ecological cognitive performance over the short-term, which is critical information for predicting educational and workplace success.
纵观心理测验的历史,认知能力主要被视为一种在个体之间而非个体内部存在差异的能力。因此,时间和情境对认知能力内部差异的影响还没有得到很好的探讨。尽管认知评估用于预测的标准结果,如教育和工作场所的表现,本身就包含了随时间和环境而变化的表现。在本文中,我们提供了新的证据,证明生态认知评估在预测学业成绩方面比单次认知评估更有效。我们建议使用认知生态瞬间评估(EMA)方法来生成认知表现的短期重复测量结果,并推荐了一组参数,可用于描述 EMA 数据中的人内生态变化。随后,我们在两项研究中对这些参数进行了实证测试,在这两项研究中,参与者通过 EMA 以及时间和情境不变的传统单次认知评估完成了一系列认知任务。我们的研究结果表明,在预测大学一年级的学习成绩方面,一系列生态认知表现参数比传统认知评估具有更高的效用。此外,出现这种情况似乎是因为生态认知成绩参数描述了传统的时间和情境不变评估所没有捕捉到的认知成绩的某些方面。我们认为,这些参数所捕捉到的是有关个人短期内典型生态认知表现的信息,而这正是预测教育和职场成功的关键信息。
{"title":"Ecological cognitive assessment has incremental validity for predicting academic performance over and above single occasion cognitive assessments","authors":"Arabella C. Vaughan,&nbsp;Damian P. Birney","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101869","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101869","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Throughout the history of psychometric testing, cognitive ability has predominantly been construed as an ability that differs between individuals but not within individuals. As a result, the influence of time and context on within-person variation in cognitive performance has not been well explored. This is despite the fact that the criterion outcomes that cognitive assessments are used to predict, such as educational and workplace performance, inherently encompass performance variability over time and context. In this paper, we provided novel evidence that ecological cognitive assessment has incremental validity for predicting academic performance over and above single occasion cognitive assessments. We proposed the use of a cognitive Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) approach to generate short-term repeated measures of cognitive performance and recommended a set of parameters that can be used to describe ecological within-person variation in EMA data. We then empirically tested these parameters across two studies in which participants completed a series of cognitive tasks delivered via the EMA as well as a traditional single occasion cognitive assessment that is time and context-invariant. Our findings showed that a range of ecological cognitive performance parameters had incremental utility for predicting first year university performance above and beyond the traditional cognitive assessment. Further, this appeared to occur because ecological cognitive performance parameters describe some aspect of cognitive performance not captured by the traditional time and context-invariant assessment. We suggest that what these parameters capture is information about an individual's typical ecological cognitive performance over the short-term, which is critical information for predicting educational and workplace success.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101869"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000631/pdfft?md5=c819b7409536b6af3b26837db6f76ad7&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000631-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142314306","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The threshold for teratogenic effects on child intelligence of prenatal exposure to phenylalanine 产前接触苯丙氨酸对儿童智力产生致畸影响的阈值
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101868
Keith F. Widaman , Jonathan Lee Helm
The primary aim of this study was to model the form of the relation between prenatal phenylalanine exposure and offspring cognitive outcomes and thereby identify the threshold for teratogenic effects of prenatal phenylalanine exposure. The participants were the 413 children and their mothers from the International Maternal Phenylketonuria Collaborative (MPKUC) Study. Consistent with prior research, average phenylalanine (PHE) level in the mother's blood throughout the pregnancy was the strongest predictor of later developmental status. Both linear regression and two-piece linear spline models were fit to the offspring outcome data, using average prenatal PHE exposure as independent variable. Results supported a nonlinear relation between prenatal PHE exposure and offspring cognitive outcomes, with damage to the developing fetus if average PHE levels were above approximately 6 to 7 mg/dL (360 to 420 μmol/L). Interestingly, prenatal PHE exposure had a moderately strong effect on offspring outcomes at 1 year of age and then had stronger effects on offspring outcomes at 2 years, even stronger at 4 years, and then stronger still at 7 years of age. The results of this study have major implications for dietary treatment of pregnant women with PKU and for conceptions of the interplay of genetic and environmental factors in affecting children's intellectual development.
本研究的主要目的是模拟产前苯丙氨酸暴露与后代认知结果之间的关系形式,从而确定产前苯丙氨酸暴露致畸效应的阈值。这项研究的参与者是国际产妇苯丙酮尿症合作研究(MPKUC)的413名儿童及其母亲。与之前的研究结果一致,母亲在整个孕期血液中的平均苯丙氨酸(PHE)水平是日后发育状况的最强预测因子。以产前 PHE 平均暴露量为自变量,对后代结果数据拟合了线性回归和两段线性样条模型。结果表明,产前 PHE 暴露与后代认知结果之间存在非线性关系,如果平均 PHE 水平超过约 6 至 7 毫克/分升(360 至 420 微摩尔/升),则会对发育中的胎儿造成损害。有趣的是,产前 PHE 暴露对子代 1 岁时的结果有中等程度的影响,对 2 岁时的结果影响更大,对 4 岁时的影响更大,对 7 岁时的影响更大。这项研究的结果对患有 PKU 的孕妇的饮食治疗以及遗传和环境因素在影响儿童智力发育方面的相互作用的概念具有重要意义。
{"title":"The threshold for teratogenic effects on child intelligence of prenatal exposure to phenylalanine","authors":"Keith F. Widaman ,&nbsp;Jonathan Lee Helm","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101868","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101868","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The primary aim of this study was to model the form of the relation between prenatal phenylalanine exposure and offspring cognitive outcomes and thereby identify the threshold for teratogenic effects of prenatal phenylalanine exposure. The participants were the 413 children and their mothers from the International Maternal Phenylketonuria Collaborative (MPKUC) Study. Consistent with prior research, average phenylalanine (PHE) level in the mother's blood throughout the pregnancy was the strongest predictor of later developmental status. Both linear regression and two-piece linear spline models were fit to the offspring outcome data, using average prenatal PHE exposure as independent variable. Results supported a nonlinear relation between prenatal PHE exposure and offspring cognitive outcomes, with damage to the developing fetus if average PHE levels were above approximately 6 to 7 mg/dL (360 to 420 μmol/L). Interestingly, prenatal PHE exposure had a moderately strong effect on offspring outcomes at 1 year of age and then had stronger effects on offspring outcomes at 2 years, even stronger at 4 years, and then stronger still at 7 years of age. The results of this study have major implications for dietary treatment of pregnant women with PKU and for conceptions of the interplay of genetic and environmental factors in affecting children's intellectual development.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101868"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142314307","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
g: Formative, reflective, or both? g:形成性、反思性,还是两者兼有?
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101870
Elena Kazali , George Spanoudis , Andreas Demetriou

This study examined how general cognitive ability, g, is formed from preschool to late childhood and how it interacts with specific mental processes. A large sample (N = 401), about equally drawn from each of the age years 4 through 12 were examined with a large array of attention control, working memory, relational integration, Raven-like matrices, and awareness of perceptual and inferential origins of representations. Confirmatory Factor Analysis examined if g is a reflective construct causally affecting these processes or a formative construct gradually emerging from mastering these processes, and how it varies throughout this age period. We found that g is a reflective construct gearing on a core of relational integration and mental awareness, which changes in cycles: it is primarily based on attention control and perceptual awareness from 4 to 6, inferential awareness and working memory from 7 to 9, and inhibition, inferential awareness, and complex inductive reasoning from 10 to 12 years. The implications of the study for the century-old dispute about the nature and development of human general intelligence and modern theories of intelligence and cognitive development are discussed.

这项研究考察了一般认知能力(g)是如何从学龄前到童年晚期形成的,以及它是如何与特定的心理过程相互作用的。大量样本(N = 401)平均来自 4 至 12 岁的每个年龄段,研究对象包括一系列注意力控制、工作记忆、关系整合、类乌鸦矩阵以及对表象的感知和推理起源的认识。确认性因子分析研究了 g 是对这些过程产生因果影响的反思性建构,还是在掌握这些过程中逐渐形成的形成性建构,以及 g 在这一年龄段的变化情况。我们发现,g 是一种反思性建构,以关系整合和心智意识为核心,并呈周期性变化:4 至 6 岁时主要以注意力控制和知觉意识为基础,7 至 9 岁时主要以推理意识和工作记忆为基础,10 至 12 岁时主要以抑制、推理意识和复杂的归纳推理为基础。本研究对有关人类一般智力的性质和发展的百年争议以及现代智力和认知发展理论的影响进行了讨论。
{"title":"g: Formative, reflective, or both?","authors":"Elena Kazali ,&nbsp;George Spanoudis ,&nbsp;Andreas Demetriou","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101870","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101870","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examined how general cognitive ability, g, is formed from preschool to late childhood and how it interacts with specific mental processes. A large sample (<em>N</em> = 401), about equally drawn from each of the age years 4 through 12 were examined with a large array of attention control, working memory, relational integration, Raven-like matrices, and awareness of perceptual and inferential origins of representations. Confirmatory Factor Analysis examined if g is a reflective construct causally affecting these processes or a formative construct gradually emerging from mastering these processes, and how it varies throughout this age period. We found that g is a reflective construct gearing on a core of relational integration and mental awareness, which changes in cycles: it is primarily based on attention control and perceptual awareness from 4 to 6, inferential awareness and working memory from 7 to 9, and inhibition, inferential awareness, and complex inductive reasoning from 10 to 12 years. The implications of the study for the century-old dispute about the nature and development of human general intelligence and modern theories of intelligence and cognitive development are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101870"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142271187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inconsistent Flynn effect patterns may be due to a decreasing positive manifold: Cohort-based measurement-invariant IQ test score changes from 2005 to 2024 不一致的弗林效应模式可能是由于正流形递减造成的:2005 年至 2024 年基于队列的测量变量智商测试分数变化
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101867
Sandra Oberleiter , Jonathan Fries , Florence Dejardin , Johanna Heller , Christian Schaible , Marco Vetter , Martin Voracek , Jakob Pietschnig

Generational IQ test score changes in the general population (i.e., the Flynn effect, typically reported as increases of 2–4 IQ points per decade) have recently been observed to behave inconsistently. It has been speculated that these inconsistencies may be attributable to the well-established negative relation of test score gains with psychometric g. Here, we provide the first direct empirical investigation of cross-temporal changes in the positive manifold of intelligence. In this cohort-comparison study, we examined performance changes in two population-representative Germanophone samples (N = 1267) across six measurement-invariant intelligence subscales from 2005 to 2024. Our analyses revealed substantial declines in single-factor analysis-based g assessments (ΔR2 range: ‐.037 to -.066) from 2005 to 2024. Despite this decrease in the positive manifold strength, we observed meaningful test score increases in all domains (d range: 0.18 to 1.24), with the largest gains in the lower tail of the intelligence distribution (i.e., conforming to Rodgers', 1998, idea of narrowing ability distributions). Our findings provide direct evidence for a decreasing strength of the positive manifold of intelligence as a noticeable driver of the accumulating evidence for negative Flynn effects, which may be a consequence of increasing ability differentiation in the general population.

最近观察到,一般人群中智商测试分数的代际变化(即弗林效应,通常报告为每十年智商增加 2-4 分)表现不一致。据推测,这些不一致可能是由于测验分数的提高与心理测量 g 的负相关关系已经得到证实。在此,我们首次对智力正向流形的跨时空变化进行了直接的实证研究。在这项队列比较研究中,我们考察了两个具有人口代表性的德语样本(N = 1267)从2005年到2024年在六个测量变量智力分量表上的成绩变化。我们的分析表明,从2005年到2024年,基于单因素分析的g评估大幅下降(ΔR2范围:-.037至-.066)。尽管正流形强度有所下降,但我们观察到所有领域的测验分数都有了有意义的增长(d 范围:0.18 至 1.24),其中智力分布的下端增长最大(即符合罗杰斯(Rodgers)1998 年提出的能力分布缩小的观点)。我们的研究结果提供了直接证据,证明智力的正向流形强度在下降,而这正是弗林负效应证据不断积累的一个显著驱动因素,而弗林负效应可能是普通人群能力分化加剧的结果。
{"title":"Inconsistent Flynn effect patterns may be due to a decreasing positive manifold: Cohort-based measurement-invariant IQ test score changes from 2005 to 2024","authors":"Sandra Oberleiter ,&nbsp;Jonathan Fries ,&nbsp;Florence Dejardin ,&nbsp;Johanna Heller ,&nbsp;Christian Schaible ,&nbsp;Marco Vetter ,&nbsp;Martin Voracek ,&nbsp;Jakob Pietschnig","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101867","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101867","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Generational IQ test score changes in the general population (i.e., the Flynn effect, typically reported as increases of 2–4 IQ points per decade) have recently been observed to behave inconsistently. It has been speculated that these inconsistencies may be attributable to the well-established negative relation of test score gains with psychometric <em>g</em>. Here, we provide the first direct empirical investigation of cross-temporal changes in the positive manifold of intelligence. In this cohort-comparison study, we examined performance changes in two population-representative Germanophone samples (<em>N</em> = 1267) across six measurement-invariant intelligence subscales from 2005 to 2024. Our analyses revealed substantial declines in single-factor analysis-based <em>g</em> assessments (Δ<em>R</em><sup>2</sup> range: ‐.037 to -.066) from 2005 to 2024. Despite this decrease in the positive manifold strength, we observed meaningful test score increases in all domains (<em>d</em> range: 0.18 to 1.24), with the largest gains in the lower tail of the intelligence distribution (i.e., conforming to Rodgers', 1998, idea of narrowing ability distributions). Our findings provide direct evidence for a decreasing strength of the positive manifold of intelligence as a noticeable driver of the accumulating evidence for negative Flynn effects, which may be a consequence of increasing ability differentiation in the general population.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101867"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000618/pdfft?md5=a8260e2b3ed8b833cca81e41bd73d121&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000618-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142271186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex differences of school grades in childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal analysis 儿童和青少年时期学校成绩的性别差异:纵向分析
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101857
Claire M. Oakley , Reinhard Pekrun , Gijsbert Stoet

We studied trajectories of school achievement in England to determine sex differences in performance and changes in these differences throughout students' development. Using a sample of 5795 children from England born in 2000–2001, this secondary data analysis examined sex differences across a range of school subjects, including differences at the upper and lower tails of the distribution of performance grades. We expected trajectories to differ by subject and to find support for greater male variability in each subject. We found a small male advantage in mathematics at age 11 but no sex differences at ages 7 and 16. Girls achieved higher language grades at each age, but this advantage was notably wider at age 16. Unlike other educational data, there were no sex differences in science achievement at ages 7 and 11 and a small female advantage in science, biology, and chemistry at age 16. Boys' school grades were more variable than girls' in English, reading, and writing at each age. Boys' STEM grades were not consistently more variable than girls' STEM grades. Sex differences were larger at the lower tail in English and the upper tail in mathematics and more balanced in science after age 7. Trajectories of sex differences are age- and subject-specific. By age 16, fewer boys achieved the upper grades, and more boys achieved the lower grades in mathematics and language than at age 11, and we found a female advantage in most school subjects. Implications for practice and directions for future research are discussed.

我们研究了英格兰学校成绩的发展轨迹,以确定成绩的性别差异以及这些差异在学生成长过程中的变化。这项二级数据分析以英格兰 2000-2001 年出生的 5795 名儿童为样本,研究了一系列学校科目中的性别差异,包括成绩等级分布中上端和下端的差异。我们预期各学科的发展轨迹会有所不同,并发现男性在各学科中的变异性更大。我们发现,男性在 11 岁时的数学成绩略有优势,但在 7 岁和 16 岁时没有性别差异。女生在每个年龄段的语文成绩都较高,但这一优势在 16 岁时明显扩大。与其他教育数据不同的是,7 岁和 11 岁学生的科学成绩没有性别差异,而 16 岁学生的科学、生物和化学成绩则略有女性优势。在每个年龄段,男生的英语、阅读和写作成绩都比女生有更大的差异。男生在科学、技术、工程和数学方面的成绩并不总是比女生的成绩变化更大。性别差异在 7 岁以后的英语成绩下限和数学成绩上限更大,而在科学成绩上则更加均衡。性别差异的轨迹与年龄和学科有关。与 11 岁时相比,到 16 岁时,男生在数学和语文方面取得高分的人数较少,而取得低分的人数较多。我们讨论了对实践的影响和未来研究的方向。
{"title":"Sex differences of school grades in childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal analysis","authors":"Claire M. Oakley ,&nbsp;Reinhard Pekrun ,&nbsp;Gijsbert Stoet","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101857","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101857","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We studied trajectories of school achievement in England to determine sex differences in performance and changes in these differences throughout students' development. Using a sample of 5795 children from England born in 2000–2001, this secondary data analysis examined sex differences across a range of school subjects, including differences at the upper and lower tails of the distribution of performance grades. We expected trajectories to differ by subject and to find support for greater male variability in each subject. We found a small male advantage in mathematics at age 11 but no sex differences at ages 7 and 16. Girls achieved higher language grades at each age, but this advantage was notably wider at age 16. Unlike other educational data, there were no sex differences in science achievement at ages 7 and 11 and a small female advantage in science, biology, and chemistry at age 16. Boys' school grades were more variable than girls' in English, reading, and writing at each age. Boys' STEM grades were not consistently more variable than girls' STEM grades. Sex differences were larger at the lower tail in English and the upper tail in mathematics and more balanced in science after age 7. Trajectories of sex differences are age- and subject-specific. By age 16, fewer boys achieved the upper grades, and more boys achieved the lower grades in mathematics and language than at age 11, and we found a female advantage in most school subjects. Implications for practice and directions for future research are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"107 ","pages":"Article 101857"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000515/pdfft?md5=c7323276a3a3f6f24dec40d59e68ce1f&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000515-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142173649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1