首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
An endless cycle of ignorance is the consequence of not offering classes on IQ and human intelligence 不开设智商和人类智力课程的后果是无休止的无知循环
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101827
Louis D. Matzel
{"title":"An endless cycle of ignorance is the consequence of not offering classes on IQ and human intelligence","authors":"Louis D. Matzel","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101827","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101827","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101827"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140320770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Meta-analytic validity of cognitive ability for hands-on military job proficiency 认知能力对实践性军事工作熟练程度的元分析有效性
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101818
Jeffrey M. Cucina , Scott K. Burtnick , Maria E. De la Flor Musso , Philip T. Walmsley , Kimberly J. Wilson

A meta-analysis of the criterion-related validity of U.S. military enlistment cognitive ability tests was conducted using hands-on performance tests (HOPTs) as the criterion. In a HOPT, incumbents perform a representative sample of the tasks for a position and are evaluated by trained raters. We found operational validities in the .40s to .50s. Our meta-analytic database allowed us to investigate concerns regarding overcorrections (via use of multivariate corrections with credible applicant covariances) and provides a direct measure of job proficiency.

以实际操作能力测试(HOPT)为标准,对美国军队征兵认知能力测试的标准相关效度进行了荟萃分析。在 HOPT 中,应征者要完成一个职位的代表性任务样本,并由训练有素的评分者进行评估。我们发现操作有效性在 0.40 到 0.50 之间。我们的元分析数据库使我们能够调查有关过度校正的问题(通过使用可信的申请人协方差进行多元校正),并提供了工作熟练程度的直接测量方法。
{"title":"Meta-analytic validity of cognitive ability for hands-on military job proficiency","authors":"Jeffrey M. Cucina ,&nbsp;Scott K. Burtnick ,&nbsp;Maria E. De la Flor Musso ,&nbsp;Philip T. Walmsley ,&nbsp;Kimberly J. Wilson","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101818","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101818","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A meta-analysis of the criterion-related validity of U.S. military enlistment cognitive ability tests was conducted using hands-on performance tests (HOPTs) as the criterion. In a HOPT, incumbents perform a representative sample of the tasks for a position and are evaluated by trained raters. We found operational validities in the .40s to .50s. Our meta-analytic database allowed us to investigate concerns regarding overcorrections (via use of multivariate corrections with credible applicant covariances) and provides a direct measure of job proficiency.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101818"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140296398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The structure of individual differences in procedural working memory: Comparing task switching and stimulus response rule information load 程序性工作记忆的个体差异结构:比较任务转换和刺激反应规则信息负荷
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-03-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101817
Dror Garbi , Nachshon Meiran

Working memory (WM) serves not just for remembering facts (“declarative WM”) but also for controlling action and thought (“procedural WM”, pWM) by holding and manipulating task rules and task control parameters such as goals. Yet, the structure of individual differences in pWM is underexplored. The present work compared between two highly similar (in the number of stimulus response rules, stimuli, responses, and scoring method) pWM challenges: Rule WM (rWM) - loading WM with novel arbitrary stimulus response rules, and Task switching between familiar rules. A series of confirmatory bi-factor models fitted to results from two separate highly variable samples (N = 544, 520) support the existence of a common (to Switching and rWM) ability as well as the distinction between rWM and Switching. Latent regression models in which correlated latent variables of Switching, Speed and rWM predicted Reasoning and Anti-saccade performance indicate a different pattern of variance sharing for switching and rWM: Specific rWM and the rWM-Speed variance overlap predicted Reasoning, whereas specific Switch did not. Switch was predictive of Anti-saccade and Reasoning only through its overlap with the rWM (for Reasoning) and with both Speed and rWM (for both outcomes). Together, these results support the conclusion that the ability to meet a switching challenge and the ability to meet a challenge of having many rules to remember (rWM) constitute partly separable sources of individual differences.

工作记忆(WM)不仅用于记忆事实("陈述性工作记忆"),还通过保持和操纵任务规则和任务控制参数(如目标)来控制行动和思维("程序性工作记忆",pWM)。然而,人们对 pWM 的个体差异结构还缺乏研究。本研究比较了两种高度相似(在刺激反应规则、刺激物、反应和评分方法的数量上)的 pWM 挑战:规则 WM(rWM)--用新的任意刺激反应规则加载 WM,以及在熟悉的规则之间进行任务切换。根据两个高度可变样本(N = 544、520)的结果建立的一系列确认性双因素模型证明,存在一种共同的(切换和规则记忆)能力,以及规则记忆和切换之间的区别。通过切换、速度和 rWM 的相关潜变量预测推理和反斜视表现的潜回归模型表明,切换和 rWM 的方差共享模式不同:特定的 rWM 和 rWM-速度方差重叠预测推理,而特定的切换则不预测推理。只有通过与 rWM 的重叠(对于推理)以及与速度和 rWM 的重叠(对于这两种结果),Switch 才能预测反犹豫和推理。总之,这些结果支持这样的结论,即应对转换挑战的能力和应对有许多规则需要记忆的挑战的能力(rWM)构成了部分可分离的个体差异来源。
{"title":"The structure of individual differences in procedural working memory: Comparing task switching and stimulus response rule information load","authors":"Dror Garbi ,&nbsp;Nachshon Meiran","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101817","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101817","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Working memory (WM) serves not just for remembering facts (“declarative WM”) but also for controlling action and thought (“procedural WM”, pWM) by holding and manipulating task rules and task control parameters such as goals. Yet, the structure of individual differences in pWM is underexplored. The present work compared between two highly similar (in the number of stimulus response rules, stimuli, responses, and scoring method) pWM challenges: Rule WM (rWM) - loading WM with novel arbitrary stimulus response rules, and Task switching between familiar rules. A series of confirmatory bi-factor models fitted to results from two separate highly variable samples (<em>N</em> = 544, 520) support the existence of a common (to Switching and rWM) ability as well as the distinction between rWM and Switching. Latent regression models in which correlated latent variables of Switching, Speed and rWM predicted Reasoning and Anti-saccade performance indicate a different pattern of variance sharing for switching and rWM: Specific rWM and the rWM-Speed variance overlap predicted Reasoning, whereas specific Switch did not. Switch was predictive of Anti-saccade and Reasoning only through its overlap with the rWM (for Reasoning) and with both Speed and rWM (for both outcomes). Together, these results support the conclusion that the ability to meet a switching challenge and the ability to meet a challenge of having many rules to remember (rWM) constitute partly separable sources of individual differences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101817"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140122563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How useful are specific cognitive ability scores? An investigation of their stability and incremental validity beyond general intelligence 特定认知能力分数有多大用处?对其稳定性和超越一般智力的增量有效性的研究
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101816
Moritz Breit, Vsevolod Scherrer, Franzis Preckel

Many intelligence tests measure multiple specific cognitive abilities. Practitioners use these specific ability scores, which encompass both specific ability and general intelligence variance, and the resulting intelligence profiles to make counseling and intervention decisions. In the present study, we investigated the temporal stability of eight specific abilities and their profiles over one school year, as well as their incremental validity in the prediction of school grades with German grade 7 to 9 students (N = 326 at T1; N = 311 at T2; N = 257 with IQ data at both times of measurement). The mean rank-order stability was 0.80 and ranged from 0.71 to 0.85. Intelligence profiles replicated significantly above chance levels (Mdnκ = 0.31). The incremental validity coefficients were mostly small, but the Reasoning score substantially contributed to the prediction of math grades (ΔR2 = 0.07–0.09), the Verbal Ability score to the prediction of German grades (ΔR2 = 0.05–0.09), and the Crystallized Intelligence score to the prediction of geography grades (ΔR2 = 0.03–0.08) beyond the general intelligence score. Our study of specific ability scores indicated moderate to high rank-order stability, fair to moderate profile stability, and substantial incremental validity for some specific ability scores.

许多智力测验都测量多种特定的认知能力。从业人员利用这些包含特定能力和一般智力差异的特定能力分数,以及由此得出的智力概况来做出咨询和干预决策。在本研究中,我们以德国七至九年级的学生(T1时人数=326;T2时人数=311;两次测量时均有智商数据的人数=257)为对象,调查了八种特定能力及其特征在一学年中的时间稳定性,以及它们在预测学校成绩时的递增有效性。平均秩稳定度为 0.80,范围在 0.71 至 0.85 之间。智力曲线的重复性明显高于偶然水平(Mdnκ = 0.31)。增量效度系数大多较小,但推理能力得分对数学成绩的预测作用很大(ΔR2 = 0.07-0.09),言语能力得分对德语成绩的预测作用很大(ΔR2 = 0.05-0.09),结晶智力得分对地理成绩的预测作用很大(ΔR2 = 0.03-0.08),超过了一般智力得分。我们对特殊能力得分的研究表明,某些特殊能力得分具有中高的等级阶次稳定性、一般到中等的轮廓稳定性和显著的递增效度。
{"title":"How useful are specific cognitive ability scores? An investigation of their stability and incremental validity beyond general intelligence","authors":"Moritz Breit,&nbsp;Vsevolod Scherrer,&nbsp;Franzis Preckel","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101816","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101816","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many intelligence tests measure multiple specific cognitive abilities. Practitioners use these specific ability scores, which encompass both specific ability and general intelligence variance, and the resulting intelligence profiles to make counseling and intervention decisions. In the present study, we investigated the temporal stability of eight specific abilities and their profiles over one school year, as well as their incremental validity in the prediction of school grades with German grade 7 to 9 students (<em>N</em> = 326 at T1; <em>N</em> = 311 at T2; <em>N</em> = 257 with IQ data at both times of measurement). The mean rank-order stability was 0.80 and ranged from 0.71 to 0.85. Intelligence profiles replicated significantly above chance levels (<em>Mdn</em><sub><em>κ</em></sub> = 0.31). The incremental validity coefficients were mostly small, but the Reasoning score substantially contributed to the prediction of math grades (<em>ΔR</em><sup><em>2</em></sup> = 0.07–0.09), the Verbal Ability score to the prediction of German grades (<em>ΔR</em><sup><em>2</em></sup> = 0.05–0.09), and the Crystallized Intelligence score to the prediction of geography grades (<em>ΔR</em><sup><em>2</em></sup> = 0.03–0.08) beyond the general intelligence score. Our study of specific ability scores indicated moderate to high rank-order stability, fair to moderate profile stability, and substantial incremental validity for some specific ability scores.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"103 ","pages":"Article 101816"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000102/pdfft?md5=656736e6fe2b4fd1c6f179e0ab473742&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000102-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139915362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attitude toward numbers: A better predictor of financial literacy and intelligence than need for cognition 对数字的态度:比认知需求更好地预测金融知识和智力
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101808
Gilles E. Gignac, Elizabeth M. Stevens

In a recent meta-analysis, financial literacy – understanding basic financial terms and concepts – was significantly correlated with general intelligence (r ≈ 0.62). However, this correlation may be underestimated, as few studies employed robust measures of both intelligence (more than one subtest) and financial literacy (>12 items). Additionally, cognitive reflection has not been investigated for its unique capacity at predicting financial literacy, an important consideration, as cognitive reflection has been suggested to be a measure of financial cognition. Beyond cognitive factors, non-cognitive predictors like need for cognition and attitude toward numbers (comfort with numerical concepts) may also influence financial literacy. In our study of 688 young adults (17–40 years), we administered four intelligence subtests (including a measure of cognitive reflection), a financial literacy test, and questionnaires assessing need for cognition and attitude toward numbers - the first to investigate all of these dimensions simultaneously. Results revealed a stronger latent correlation (0.76) between general intelligence and financial literacy than previously reported. Cognitive reflection was found to yield some incremental predictive variance in predicting financial literacy beyond general intelligence. Finally, attitude toward numbers and need for cognition both correlated positively with both general intelligence and financial literacy, however, only attitude toward numbers uniquely predicted financial literacy in a structural equation model. These findings suggest that financial literacy shares closer to 50–60% of its variance with general intelligence. They also highlight the potential value of fostering a positive numerical attitude alongside financial knowledge in educational interventions.

在最近的一项荟萃分析中,金融素养--理解基本的金融术语和概念--与一般智力显著相关(r ≈ 0.62)。然而,这种相关性可能被低估了,因为很少有研究同时对智力(一个以上的子测试)和金融素养(12 个项目)进行了可靠的测量。此外,认知反思在预测金融素养方面的独特能力尚未得到研究,这是一个重要的考虑因素,因为认知反思被认为是金融认知的一种测量方法。除认知因素外,非认知预测因素如认知需求和对数字的态度(对数字概念的舒适度)也可能影响金融素养。在对 688 名年轻成年人(17-40 岁)的研究中,我们进行了四项智力测验(包括一项认知反思测量)、一项金融素养测试以及评估认知需求和对数字的态度的问卷,这是首次同时对所有这些维度进行调查。研究结果表明,一般智力与金融素养之间的潜在相关性(0.76)比以往报告的更强。研究发现,认知反思在预测金融素养方面产生了一些超出一般智力的增量预测变量。最后,对数字的态度和对认知的需求都与一般智力和金融素养呈正相关,然而,在结构方程模型中,只有对数字的态度能独特地预测金融素养。这些研究结果表明,金融素养与一般智力的差异接近 50%-60%。这些发现还强调了在教育干预中培养积极的数字态度和金融知识的潜在价值。
{"title":"Attitude toward numbers: A better predictor of financial literacy and intelligence than need for cognition","authors":"Gilles E. Gignac,&nbsp;Elizabeth M. Stevens","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101808","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101808","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In a recent meta-analysis, financial literacy – understanding basic financial terms and concepts – was significantly correlated with general intelligence (<em>r</em> ≈ 0.62). However, this correlation may be underestimated, as few studies employed robust measures of both intelligence (more than one subtest) and financial literacy (&gt;12 items). Additionally, cognitive reflection has not been investigated for its unique capacity at predicting financial literacy, an important consideration, as cognitive reflection has been suggested to be a measure of financial cognition. Beyond cognitive factors, non-cognitive predictors like need for cognition and attitude toward numbers (comfort with numerical concepts) may also influence financial literacy. In our study of 688 young adults (17–40 years), we administered four intelligence subtests (including a measure of cognitive reflection), a financial literacy test, and questionnaires assessing need for cognition and attitude toward numbers - the first to investigate all of these dimensions simultaneously. Results revealed a stronger latent correlation (0.76) between general intelligence and financial literacy than previously reported. Cognitive reflection was found to yield some incremental predictive variance in predicting financial literacy beyond general intelligence. Finally, attitude toward numbers and need for cognition both correlated positively with both general intelligence and financial literacy, however, only attitude toward numbers uniquely predicted financial literacy in a structural equation model. These findings suggest that financial literacy shares closer to 50–60% of its variance with general intelligence. They also highlight the potential value of fostering a positive numerical attitude alongside financial knowledge in educational interventions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"103 ","pages":"Article 101808"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000023/pdfft?md5=5b1fe7e1aae827a7037f0efcbe14ed33&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000023-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139494202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Functional brain networks involved in the Raven's standard progressive matrices task and their relation to theories of fluid intelligence 参与瑞文标准渐进矩阵任务的大脑功能网络及其与流体智力理论的关系
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101807
Riley Zurrin , Samantha Tze Sum Wong , Meighen M. Roes , Chantal M. Percival , Abhijit Chinchani , Leo Arreaza , Mavis Kusi , Ava Momeni , Maiya Rasheed , Zhaoyi Mo , Vina M. Goghari , Todd S. Woodward

A dimensionality reduction method was used to determine the task-timing-related functional brain networks underlying the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), a non-verbal estimate of fluid intelligence (Gf). We identified five macro-scale task-based blood‑oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD)-signal brain networks and interpreted their network-level task-induced BOLD changes to provide functional interpretations separately for each network. This led to new observations about the brain networks underlying the RSPM: (1) the multiple demand network (MDN) for solution searching peaked early in the trial (∼9 s peak), followed by response (RESP) for response selection (∼12 s), and re-evaluation (RE-EV) for solution checking (∼18 s peak), (2) high activity in the MDN was correlated with high activity in the later-peaking RE-EV network, proposed to underpin cooperative solution searching (MDN) and checking (RE-EV) processes, and (3) high activity in the MDN in all conditions was associated with low accuracy in the hard RSPM condition, suggesting that those with lower performance on hard problems allocate more resources into solution-searching across all conditions. These findings corroborate the MDN's significance in Gf solution searching, and add the RE-EV network as playing an important checking role, providing overlap with the proposed abstraction/elaboration and hypothesis testing phases of the Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT). Therefore, this set of results not only supports past theoretical work on the brain networks underlying Gf and the RSPM task, but extends it by providing more complete anatomical, temporal, and functional information based on a set of brain task-based networks which replicate over many tasks.

我们采用了一种降维方法来确定瑞文标准渐进矩阵(RSPM)(一种流体智力(Gf)的非语言估计值)中与任务相关的脑功能网络。我们确定了五个基于任务的宏观血氧水平依赖(BOLD)信号脑网络,并解释了其网络级任务诱导的BOLD变化,为每个网络分别提供了功能解释。这导致了对 RSPM 基础脑网络的新观察:(1)用于搜索解决方案的多需求网络(MDN)在试验早期达到峰值(∼9 秒),随后是用于选择响应的响应网络(RESP)(∼12 秒)和用于检查解决方案的重新评估网络(RE-EV)(∼18 秒),(2)MDN 的高活动与后期 RE-EV 网络的高活动相关、(3)在所有条件下,MDN 的高活动与 RSPM 难题条件下的低准确率相关,这表明在所有条件下,那些在难题上表现较差的人分配了更多的资源用于寻找解决方案。这些发现证实了 MDN 在 Gf 解题搜索中的重要性,并增加了 RE-EV 网络扮演的重要检查角色,与旁额整合理论(Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory,P-FIT)中提出的抽象/演算和假设检验阶段相重叠。因此,这组研究结果不仅支持了过去有关 Gf 和 RSPM 任务基础大脑网络的理论研究,而且还基于一组可在多项任务中复制的基于任务的大脑网络,提供了更完整的解剖学、时间和功能信息,从而对其进行了扩展。
{"title":"Functional brain networks involved in the Raven's standard progressive matrices task and their relation to theories of fluid intelligence","authors":"Riley Zurrin ,&nbsp;Samantha Tze Sum Wong ,&nbsp;Meighen M. Roes ,&nbsp;Chantal M. Percival ,&nbsp;Abhijit Chinchani ,&nbsp;Leo Arreaza ,&nbsp;Mavis Kusi ,&nbsp;Ava Momeni ,&nbsp;Maiya Rasheed ,&nbsp;Zhaoyi Mo ,&nbsp;Vina M. Goghari ,&nbsp;Todd S. Woodward","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101807","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101807","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A dimensionality reduction method was used to determine the task-timing-related functional brain networks underlying the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), a non-verbal estimate of fluid intelligence (Gf). We identified five macro-scale task-based blood‑oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD)-signal brain networks and interpreted their network-level task-induced BOLD changes to provide functional interpretations separately for each network. This led to new observations about the brain networks underlying the RSPM: (1) the multiple demand network (MDN) for solution searching peaked early in the trial (∼9 s peak), followed by response (RESP) for response selection (∼12 s), and re-evaluation (RE-EV) for solution checking (∼18 s peak), (2) high activity in the MDN was correlated with high activity in the later-peaking RE-EV network, proposed to underpin cooperative solution searching (MDN) and checking (RE-EV) processes, and (3) high activity in the MDN in all conditions was associated with low accuracy in the hard RSPM condition, suggesting that those with lower performance on hard problems allocate more resources into solution-searching across all conditions. These findings corroborate the MDN's significance in Gf solution searching, and add the RE-EV network as playing an important checking role, providing overlap with the proposed abstraction/elaboration and hypothesis testing phases of the Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT). Therefore, this set of results not only supports past theoretical work on the brain networks underlying Gf and the RSPM task, but extends it by providing more complete anatomical, temporal, and functional information based on a set of brain task-based networks which replicate over many tasks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"103 ","pages":"Article 101807"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000011/pdfft?md5=a8fbc296343facb0eebf1caee31dc972&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000011-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139436218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No spatial advantage in adolescent hockey players? Exploring measure specificity and masked effects 青少年冰球运动员没有空间优势?探索测量特异性和掩蔽效应
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101805
Ksenia Bartseva , Maxim Likhanov , Elina Tsigeman , Evgenia Alenina , Ivan Reznichenko , Elena Soldatova , Yulia Kovas

The study examines how intensive hockey training is linked with spatial ability and academic performance. Participants were hockey players from top junior teams (N = 225, mean age = 14.25, all boys) and their unselected peers (N = 278, mean age = 15.47, all boys). Compared to the unselected group, hockey players showed lower results in 10 small-scale spatial tests (Cohen's d ranging from 0.42 to 1.04), Raven's Progressive Matrices (d = 0.41), and 12 school subjects (d for the sum of grades = 1.17). The differences in spatial ability remained significant after controlling for Raven's (d varying from 0.26 to 1.03). The absence of spatial advantage in athletes suggests that effects of sports on cognition are complex: spatial ability facet-specific, sport-specific, professional and intensity level-specific. Moreover, these effects might be confounded by differences in academic engagement, investment of effort and psychological and physiological effects of intensive sports engagement.

这项研究探讨了强化曲棍球训练与空间能力和学习成绩之间的关系。参与者是来自顶级青少年冰球队的冰球运动员(人数 = 225,平均年龄 = 14.25,均为男生)和未入选的同龄人(人数 = 278,平均年龄 = 15.47,均为男生)。与未入选组相比,曲棍球运动员在 10 项小规模空间测试(科恩氏 d 为 0.42 至 1.04)、瑞文渐进矩阵(d = 0.41)和 12 项学校科目(成绩总和的 d = 1.17)中的成绩较低。在控制了瑞文数模(d 从 0.26 到 1.03 不等)之后,空间能力的差异仍然显著。运动员缺乏空间优势表明,体育运动对认知的影响是复杂的:空间能力方面的特异性、体育运动的特异性、职业和强度水平的特异性。此外,这些影响还可能受到学业参与、努力投入以及密集运动的心理和生理影响等方面差异的影响。
{"title":"No spatial advantage in adolescent hockey players? Exploring measure specificity and masked effects","authors":"Ksenia Bartseva ,&nbsp;Maxim Likhanov ,&nbsp;Elina Tsigeman ,&nbsp;Evgenia Alenina ,&nbsp;Ivan Reznichenko ,&nbsp;Elena Soldatova ,&nbsp;Yulia Kovas","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101805","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101805","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The study examines how intensive hockey training is linked with spatial ability and academic performance. Participants were hockey players from top junior teams (<em>N</em> = 225, mean age = 14.25, all boys) and their unselected peers (<em>N</em> = 278, mean age = 15.47, all boys). Compared to the unselected group, hockey players showed lower results in 10 small-scale spatial tests (Cohen's d ranging from 0.42 to 1.04), Raven's Progressive Matrices (d = 0.41), and 12 school subjects (d for the sum of grades = 1.17). The differences in spatial ability remained significant after controlling for Raven's (d varying from 0.26 to 1.03). The absence of spatial advantage in athletes suggests that effects of sports on cognition are complex: spatial ability facet-specific, sport-specific, professional and intensity level-specific. Moreover, these effects might be confounded by differences in academic engagement, investment of effort and psychological and physiological effects of intensive sports engagement.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"102 ","pages":"Article 101805"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289623000867/pdfft?md5=f09603cbe20f2ad4088d8ef7a46dc517&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289623000867-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139070868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The taboo remains: Responding to a critical commentary 禁忌依然存在回应批评性评论
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101806
Noah Carl, Michael A. Woodley of Menie

Jackson Jr. and Winston (2021) claim there is no taboo against research on race and intelligence. We argue, to the contrary, that there is a taboo against such research. We begin by addressing Jackson Jr. and Winston's arguments concerning expert surveys, as well as their criticisms of our published database of controversies. We then provide additional evidence of the taboo, and explain why it would be expected given the features of academic social science in Western countries. We also point out that Jackson Jr. and Winston's paper exemplifies the very taboo they claim does not exist.

小杰克逊和温斯顿(2021 年)声称,对种族和智力的研究不存在禁忌。而我们的观点恰恰相反,我们认为对此类研究存在禁忌。我们首先讨论了小杰克逊和温斯顿关于专家调查的论点,以及他们对我们公布的争议数据库的批评。然后,我们提供了有关禁忌的更多证据,并解释了为什么从西方国家社会科学学术的特点来看,这种禁忌是意料之中的。我们还指出,小杰克逊和温斯顿的论文恰恰体现了他们声称不存在的禁忌。
{"title":"The taboo remains: Responding to a critical commentary","authors":"Noah Carl,&nbsp;Michael A. Woodley of Menie","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101806","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101806","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Jackson Jr. and Winston (2021) claim there is no taboo against research on race and intelligence. We argue, to the contrary, that there is a taboo against such research. We begin by addressing Jackson Jr. and Winston's arguments concerning expert surveys, as well as their criticisms of our published database of controversies. We then provide additional evidence of the taboo, and explain why it would be expected given the features of academic social science in Western countries. We also point out that Jackson Jr. and Winston's paper exemplifies the very taboo they claim does not exist.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"102 ","pages":"Article 101806"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139099536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Don't waste your time measuring intelligence: Further evidence for the validity of a three-minute speeded reasoning test 不要浪费时间测量智力三分钟快速推理测试有效性的进一步证据
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-12-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101804
Anna-Lena Schubert , Christoph Löffler , Clara Wiebel , Florian Kaulhausen , Tanja Gabriele Baudson

The rise of large-scale collaborative panel studies has generated a need for fast, reliable, and valid assessments of cognitive abilities. In these studies, a detailed characterization of participants' cognitive abilities is often unnecessary, leading to the selection of tests based on convenience, duration, and feasibility. This often results in the use of abbreviated measures or proxies, potentially compromising their reliability and validity. Here we evaluate the mini-q (Baudson & Preckel, 2016), a three-minute speeded reasoning test, as a brief assessment of general cognitive abilities. The mini-q exhibited excellent reliability (0.96–0.99) and a substantial correlation with general cognitive abilities measured with a comprehensive test battery (r = 0.57; age-corrected r = 0.50), supporting its potential as a brief screening of cognitive abilities. Working memory capacity accounted for the majority (54%) of the association between test performance and general cognitive abilities, whereas individual differences in processing speed did not contribute to this relationship. Our results support the notion that the mini-q can be used as a brief, reliable, and valid assessment of general cognitive abilities. We therefore developed a computer-based version, ensuring its adaptability for large-scale panel studies. The paper- and computer-based versions demonstrated scalar measurement invariance and can therefore be used interchangeably. We provide norm data for young (18 to 30 years) and middle-aged (31 to 60 years) adults and provide recommendations for incorporating the mini-q in panel studies. Additionally, we address potential challenges stemming from language diversity, wide age ranges, and online testing in such studies.

随着大规模合作小组研究的兴起,人们需要对认知能力进行快速、可靠和有效的评估。在这些研究中,往往不需要对参与者的认知能力进行详细描述,而是根据方便程度、持续时间和可行性来选择测试。这往往会导致使用简略的测量方法或替代方法,从而可能影响其可靠性和有效性。在此,我们对迷你 Q(Baudson & Preckel, 2016)进行了评估,这是一个三分钟的快速推理测试,可作为一般认知能力的简短评估。迷你 Q 表现出极佳的可靠性(0.96-0.99),并且与综合测试组测量的一般认知能力有很大的相关性(r = 0.57;年龄校正 r = 0.50),支持其作为认知能力简短筛查的潜力。工作记忆能力占测试成绩与一般认知能力之间关系的大部分(54%),而处理速度方面的个体差异并不影响这种关系。我们的研究结果支持这样一种观点,即迷你 Q 可以作为一种简短、可靠且有效的一般认知能力评估工具。因此,我们开发了基于计算机的版本,以确保其适用于大规模的小组研究。纸质版和电脑版均显示了标度测量的不变性,因此可以互换使用。我们提供了年轻人(18 至 30 岁)和中年人(31 至 60 岁)的常模数据,并为将迷你 Q 纳入小组研究提供了建议。此外,我们还探讨了此类研究中因语言多样性、年龄跨度大以及在线测试而可能面临的挑战。
{"title":"Don't waste your time measuring intelligence: Further evidence for the validity of a three-minute speeded reasoning test","authors":"Anna-Lena Schubert ,&nbsp;Christoph Löffler ,&nbsp;Clara Wiebel ,&nbsp;Florian Kaulhausen ,&nbsp;Tanja Gabriele Baudson","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101804","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101804","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The rise of large-scale collaborative panel studies has generated a need for fast, reliable, and valid assessments of cognitive abilities. In these studies, a detailed characterization of participants' cognitive abilities is often unnecessary, leading to the selection of tests based on convenience, duration, and feasibility. This often results in the use of abbreviated measures or proxies, potentially compromising their reliability and validity. Here we evaluate the mini-q (Baudson &amp; Preckel, 2016), a three-minute speeded reasoning test, as a brief assessment of general cognitive abilities. The mini-q exhibited excellent reliability (0.96–0.99) and a substantial correlation with general cognitive abilities measured with a comprehensive test battery (<em>r</em> = 0.57; age-corrected <em>r</em> = 0.50), supporting its potential as a brief screening of cognitive abilities. Working memory capacity accounted for the majority (54%) of the association between test performance and general cognitive abilities, whereas individual differences in processing speed did not contribute to this relationship. Our results support the notion that the mini-q can be used as a brief, reliable, and valid assessment of general cognitive abilities. We therefore developed a computer-based version, ensuring its adaptability for large-scale panel studies. The paper- and computer-based versions demonstrated scalar measurement invariance and can therefore be used interchangeably. We provide norm data for young (18 to 30 years) and middle-aged (31 to 60 years) adults and provide recommendations for incorporating the mini-q in panel studies. Additionally, we address potential challenges stemming from language diversity, wide age ranges, and online testing in such studies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"102 ","pages":"Article 101804"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289623000855/pdfft?md5=bd0207bc1befe85ebaa4b91bf6f31d69&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289623000855-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138490454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unsuccessful and successful complex problem solvers – A log file analysis of complex problem solving strategies across multiple tasks 不成功和成功的复杂问题解决者——跨多个任务的复杂问题解决策略的日志文件分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101793
Björn Nicolay , Florian Krieger , Jörg-Tobias Kuhn , Arthur C. Graesser , Dirk Ifenthaler , Ryan Baker , Samuel Greiff

Complex problem solving (CPS) is a key competence in educational contexts with strong conceptual links to students' overall intelligence. However, the mechanisms underlying successful CPS are not fully understood. Therefore, this study investigated several factors presumed to be relevant to CPS success using log file data to code each individual student action during six CPS tasks with different characteristics (N = 1276). We coded individual strategy combinations per student for each item for different strategy combinations of vary-one-thing-at-a-time (VOTAT), hold-one-thing-at-a-time (HOTAT), vary-no-thing-at-a-time (NOTAT), and change-all (CA). Results from generalized linear mixed models showed that CPS success was likely to be achieved by using VOTAT. However, there was an increased chance of solving an item when additional strategies, such as NOTAT or NOTAT plus HOTAT were used. This result was moderated by the presence/absence of eigendynamics as an important determinant of item difficulty. Strategy combinations of VOTAT together with other strategies (all including NOTAT) showed higher chances of CPS success when eigendynamics were present. Additionally, higher chances of solving an item when using VOTAT with additional strategies was demonstrated for items without eigendynamics. Overall, our results suggest that flexibility in strategy application is the driving force for successful CPS performance. Implications are discussed in light of the presumed benefit of pedagogically relevant metacognitive skills, such as planning, monitoring and reflecting, for CPS success. Based on our findings, we provide specific recommendations for the development of computer-based learning simulations to train CPS and related competencies, ultimately enhancing students' skills in educational contexts.

复杂问题解决能力(CPS)是教育环境中的一项关键能力,与学生的整体智力有着很强的概念联系。然而,成功的CPS背后的机制尚不完全清楚。因此,本研究调查了几个被认为与CPS成功相关的因素,使用日志文件数据对六个具有不同特征的CPS任务中的每个学生行为进行编码(N = 1276)。我们为每个学生的每个项目编码了不同的策略组合,包括一次只改变一件事(VOTAT)、一次只保留一件事(HOTAT)、一次不改变一件事(NOTAT)和全部改变(CA)。广义线性混合模型的结果表明,使用VOTAT有可能实现CPS的成功。然而,当使用额外的策略,如NOTAT或NOTAT加HOTAT时,解决一个问题的机会增加了。作为项目难度的重要决定因素,特征动力的存在/缺失缓和了这一结果。当特征动力学存在时,VOTAT与其他策略(包括NOTAT)的策略组合显示出更高的CPS成功机会。此外,当使用附加策略的VOTAT时,对于没有特征动力学的项目,解决问题的机会更高。总体而言,我们的研究结果表明,策略应用的灵活性是CPS成功绩效的驱动力。根据教学相关的元认知技能(如计划、监测和反思)对CPS成功的假定益处,讨论了其影响。根据我们的研究结果,我们为开发基于计算机的学习模拟提供了具体建议,以培训CPS和相关能力,最终提高学生在教育环境中的技能。
{"title":"Unsuccessful and successful complex problem solvers – A log file analysis of complex problem solving strategies across multiple tasks","authors":"Björn Nicolay ,&nbsp;Florian Krieger ,&nbsp;Jörg-Tobias Kuhn ,&nbsp;Arthur C. Graesser ,&nbsp;Dirk Ifenthaler ,&nbsp;Ryan Baker ,&nbsp;Samuel Greiff","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101793","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101793","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Complex problem solving (CPS) is a key competence in educational contexts with strong conceptual links to students' overall intelligence. However, the mechanisms underlying successful CPS are not fully understood. Therefore, this study investigated several factors presumed to be relevant to CPS success using log file data to code each individual student action during six CPS tasks with different characteristics (<em>N</em> = 1276). We coded individual strategy combinations per student for each item for different strategy combinations of vary-one-thing-at-a-time (VOTAT), hold-one-thing-at-a-time (HOTAT), vary-no-thing-at-a-time (NOTAT), and change-all (CA). Results from generalized linear mixed models showed that CPS success was likely to be achieved by using VOTAT. However, there was an increased chance of solving an item when additional strategies, such as NOTAT or NOTAT plus HOTAT were used. This result was moderated by the presence/absence of eigendynamics as an important determinant of item difficulty. Strategy combinations of VOTAT together with other strategies (all including NOTAT) showed higher chances of CPS success when eigendynamics were present. Additionally, higher chances of solving an item when using VOTAT with additional strategies was demonstrated for items without eigendynamics. Overall, our results suggest that flexibility in strategy application is the driving force for successful CPS performance. Implications are discussed in light of the presumed benefit of pedagogically relevant metacognitive skills, such as planning, monitoring and reflecting, for CPS success. Based on our findings, we provide specific recommendations for the development of computer-based learning simulations to train CPS and related competencies, ultimately enhancing students' skills in educational contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"101 ","pages":"Article 101793"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91992119","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1