首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
The genetics of specific cognitive abilities 特定认知能力的遗传学
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101689
Francesca Procopio , Quan Zhou , Ziye Wang , Agnieska Gidziela , Kaili Rimfeld , Margherita Malanchini , Robert Plomin

Most research on individual differences in performance on tests of cognitive ability focuses on general cognitive ability (g), the highest level in the three-level Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) hierarchical model of intelligence. About 50% of the variance of g is due to inherited DNA differences (heritability) which increases across development. Much less is known about the genetics of the middle level of the CHC model, which includes 16 broad factors such as fluid reasoning, processing speed, and quantitative knowledge. We provide a meta-analytic review of 747,567 monozygotic-dizygotic twin comparisons from 77 publications for these middle-level factors, which we refer to as specific cognitive abilities (SCA), even though these factors are not independent of g. Twin comparisons were available for 11 of the 16 CHC domains. The average heritability across all SCA is 56%, similar to that of g. However, there is substantial differential heritability across SCA and SCA do not show the developmental increase in heritability seen for g. We also investigated SCA independent of g (SCA.g). A surprising finding is that SCA.g remain substantially heritable (53% on average), even though 25% of the variance of SCA that covaries with g has been removed. Our review highlights the need for more research on SCA and especially on SCA.g. Despite limitations of SCA research, our review frames expectations for genomic research that will use polygenic scores to predict SCA and SCA.g. Genome-wide association studies of SCA.g are needed to create polygenic scores that can predict SCA profiles of cognitive abilities and disabilities independent of g.

大多数关于认知能力测试中个体差异的研究都集中在一般认知能力(g)上,这是Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC)智力层次模型的最高水平。大约50%的g变异是由于遗传的DNA差异(遗传性),这种差异随着发育而增加。关于CHC模型中层的遗传学知之甚少,其中包括16个广泛的因素,如流体推理、处理速度和定量知识。我们对来自77篇出版物的747,567个单卵-双卵双胞胎的比较进行了荟萃分析,研究了这些中间水平因素,我们称之为特定认知能力(SCA),尽管这些因素并非独立于g。双胞胎比较可用于16个CHC域中的11个。所有SCA的平均遗传率为56%,与g相似。然而,SCA之间的遗传率存在显著差异,SCA没有显示出g所见的遗传率的发育增加。我们还研究了独立于g的SCA (SCA.g)。一个令人惊讶的发现是SCA。尽管与g共变的SCA方差的25%已被去除,但g仍然具有很大的可遗传性(平均为53%)。我们的综述强调需要对SCA,特别是SCA.g进行更多的研究。尽管SCA研究存在局限性,但我们的综述框架了基因组研究将使用多基因评分来预测SCA和SCA.g的期望。SCA的全基因组关联研究。g需要创建多基因评分,可以预测独立于g的认知能力和残疾的SCA概况。
{"title":"The genetics of specific cognitive abilities","authors":"Francesca Procopio ,&nbsp;Quan Zhou ,&nbsp;Ziye Wang ,&nbsp;Agnieska Gidziela ,&nbsp;Kaili Rimfeld ,&nbsp;Margherita Malanchini ,&nbsp;Robert Plomin","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101689","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101689","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most research on individual differences in performance on tests of cognitive ability focuses on general cognitive ability (g), the highest level in the three-level Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) hierarchical model of intelligence. About 50% of the variance of g is due to inherited DNA differences (heritability) which increases across development. Much less is known about the genetics of the middle level of the CHC model, which includes 16 broad factors such as fluid reasoning, processing speed, and quantitative knowledge. We provide a meta-analytic review of 747,567 monozygotic-dizygotic twin comparisons from 77 publications for these middle-level factors, which we refer to as specific cognitive abilities (SCA), even though these factors are not independent of g. Twin comparisons were available for 11 of the 16 CHC domains. The average heritability across all SCA is 56%, similar to that of g. However, there is substantial differential heritability across SCA and SCA do not show the developmental increase in heritability seen for g. We also investigated SCA independent of g (SCA.g). A surprising finding is that SCA.g remain substantially heritable (53% on average), even though 25% of the variance of SCA that covaries with g has been removed. Our review highlights the need for more research on SCA and especially on SCA.g. Despite limitations of SCA research, our review frames expectations for genomic research that will use polygenic scores to predict SCA and SCA.g. Genome-wide association studies of SCA.g are needed to create polygenic scores that can predict SCA profiles of cognitive abilities and disabilities independent of g.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10184120/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9841626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sophisticated deviants: Intelligence and radical economic attitudes 老练的离经叛道者:智慧和激进的经济态度
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101699
Chien-An Lin, Timothy C. Bates

Conservative economic attitudes have been theorized as symptoms of low cognitive ability. Studies suggest the opposite, linking more conservative views weakly to higher, not lower, cognitive ability, but with very large between-study variability. Here, we propose and replicate a new model linking cognitive ability not to liberal or conservative economics, but to economic extremism: How far individuals deviate from prevailing centrist views. Two large pre-registered studies in the UK (N = 700 & 700) and the British Cohort Study dataset (N = 11,563) replicated the predicted association of intelligence with economic deviance (β = 0.4 to 0.12). These findings were robust and expand the role of cognitive ability from tracking the economic consensus to influencing support for (relatively) extremist views. They suggest opportunities to understand the generation and mainstreaming of radical fringe social attitudes.

保守的经济态度被理论化为认知能力低下的症状。研究结果恰恰相反,将更保守的观点与更高(而不是更低)的认知能力微弱地联系在一起,但研究之间的差异非常大。在这里,我们提出并复制了一个新的模型,将认知能力与经济极端主义联系起来,而不是自由主义或保守主义经济学:个人偏离主流中间派观点的程度。在英国进行的两项大型预注册研究(N = 700 &700)和英国队列研究数据集(N = 11563)重复了智力与经济偏差的预测关联(β = 0.4至0.12)。这些发现是强有力的,并将认知能力的作用从追踪经济共识扩展到影响对(相对)极端观点的支持。它们提供了理解激进边缘社会态度的产生和主流化的机会。
{"title":"Sophisticated deviants: Intelligence and radical economic attitudes","authors":"Chien-An Lin,&nbsp;Timothy C. Bates","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101699","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101699","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Conservative economic attitudes have been theorized as symptoms of low cognitive ability. Studies suggest the opposite, linking more conservative views weakly to higher, not lower, cognitive ability, but with very large between-study variability. Here, we propose and replicate a new model linking cognitive ability not to liberal or conservative economics, but to economic extremism: How far individuals deviate from prevailing centrist views. Two large pre-registered studies in the UK (<em>N</em> = 700 &amp; 700) and the British Cohort Study dataset (<em>N</em> = 11,563) replicated the predicted association of intelligence with economic deviance (β = 0.4 to 0.12). These findings were robust and expand the role of cognitive ability from tracking the economic consensus to influencing support for (relatively) extremist views. They suggest opportunities to understand the generation and mainstreaming of radical fringe social attitudes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289622000800/pdfft?md5=8d3e5b78845811c00fce177b91d745c9&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289622000800-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45890593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are Piagetian scales just intelligence tests? 皮亚杰音阶只是智力测试吗?
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101702
Jordan Lasker

Abstract

Previous research has noted the relationships between Piagetian and Psychometric tests. The literature on this topic has revealed consistent and moderate-to-high magnitude associations and largely unidimensional tests. However, most of this literature has not explicitly assessed whether Psychometric general intelligence corresponds to a similar factor measurable with Piagetian tests. This study did just that by fitting structural equation models to available data from administrations of Piagetian and Psychometric tests given to the same samples. The tests were modeled separately, and then in models where they were featured together so their general factors could be correlated. The results of those analyses were then subjected to a meta-analysis (K = 8, N = 862) and the meta-analytic relationship between Piagetian and Psychometric general factors was found to be exceptionally high (r = 0.847). Piagetian scales thus test intelligence, and intelligence tests assess Piagetian development.

摘要以往的研究已经注意到皮亚杰测验与心理测量测验之间的关系。关于这一主题的文献揭示了一致的和中等到高量级的关联和很大程度上的单维测试。然而,大多数这类文献并没有明确评估心理测量一般智力是否与皮亚杰测试可测量的类似因素相对应。这项研究正是通过将结构方程模型与提供给相同样本的皮亚杰和心理测量测试的管理数据相匹配来做到这一点的。这些测试分别建模,然后在模型中把它们放在一起,这样它们的一般因素就可以相互关联。然后对这些分析结果进行meta分析(K = 8, N = 862),发现Piagetian和心理测量学一般因素之间的meta分析关系异常高(r = 0.847)。因此,皮亚杰量表测试智力,智力测试评估皮亚杰发展。
{"title":"Are Piagetian scales just intelligence tests?","authors":"Jordan Lasker","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101702","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101702","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Abstract</p><p><span>Previous research has noted the relationships between Piagetian and Psychometric tests. The literature on this topic has revealed consistent and moderate-to-high magnitude associations and largely unidimensional tests. However, most of this literature has not explicitly assessed whether Psychometric general intelligence corresponds to a similar factor measurable with Piagetian tests. This study did just that by fitting structural equation models to available data from administrations of Piagetian and Psychometric tests given to the same samples. The tests were modeled separately, and then in models where they were featured together so their general factors could be correlated. The results of those analyses were then subjected to a meta-analysis (</span><em>K</em> = 8, <em>N</em> = 862) and the meta-analytic relationship between Piagetian and Psychometric general factors was found to be exceptionally high (<em>r</em> = 0.847). Piagetian scales thus test intelligence, and intelligence tests assess Piagetian development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43914484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Form perception speed is critical for the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency 形式感知速度对非语言数感与算术流畅性的关系起着至关重要的作用
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101704
Shijia Fang , Xinlin Zhou

Form perception has been found to be critical for arithmetic fluency. How visual processing of form is related to arithmetic fluency is a question that remains to be answered. The current study hypothesized that form perception speed correlates with arithmetic fluency. Experiment 1 applied a between-subject design. 227 fifth grade students were recruited and randomly divided into two experimental groups. The groups performed the same figure-matching task, but with different stimulus durations: 400 ms or 1000 ms. The results showed that the 400 ms group had greater arithmetic fluency than the 1000 ms group, and form perception measured only in 400 ms group could explain the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency. Experiment 2 applied a within-subject design. 245 fifth-grade students were recruited and all participants were administered a 400-ms figure-matching task and a 1000-ms figure-matching task. The form perception measured with 400 ms or 1000 ms duration could explain the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency. These findings indicate that form perception speed underlies the association among form perception, non-verbal number sense, and arithmetic fluency.

形式感知对算术流畅性至关重要。形式的视觉处理与算术流畅性之间的关系是一个有待回答的问题。目前的研究假设形式感知速度与算术流畅性相关。实验1采用受试者间设计。招募了227名五年级学生,随机分为两个实验组。这两组都执行了相同的图形匹配任务,但刺激时间不同:400毫秒或1000毫秒。结果表明,400 ms组的算术流畅性高于1000 ms组,仅400 ms组测量的形式知觉可以解释非言语数感与算术流畅性之间的关系。实验2采用受试者内设计。研究人员招募了245名五年级学生,并对所有参与者进行了400毫秒的图形匹配任务和1000毫秒的图形匹配任务。400 ms和1000 ms时间的形式感知可以解释非言语数感与算术流畅性之间的关系。这些发现表明,形式知觉速度是形式知觉、非语言数字感觉和算术流畅性之间联系的基础。
{"title":"Form perception speed is critical for the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency","authors":"Shijia Fang ,&nbsp;Xinlin Zhou","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101704","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101704","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Form perception has been found to be critical for arithmetic fluency. How visual processing of form is related to arithmetic fluency is a question that remains to be answered. The current study hypothesized that form perception speed correlates with arithmetic fluency. Experiment 1 applied a between-subject design. 227 fifth grade students were recruited and randomly divided into two experimental groups. The groups performed the same figure-matching task, but with different stimulus durations: 400 ms or 1000 ms. The results showed that the 400 ms group had greater arithmetic fluency than the 1000 ms group, and form perception measured only in 400 ms group could explain the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency. Experiment 2 applied a within-subject design. 245 fifth-grade students were recruited and all participants were administered a 400-ms figure-matching task and a 1000-ms figure-matching task. The form perception measured with 400 ms or 1000 ms duration could explain the relationship between non-verbal number sense and arithmetic fluency. These findings indicate that form perception speed underlies the association among form perception, non-verbal number sense, and arithmetic fluency.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48902132","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distributions of academic math-verbal tilt and overall academic skill of students specializing in different fields: A study of 1.6 million graduate record examination test takers 不同专业学生学术数学语言倾斜度和综合学术技能的分布——对160万研究生入学考试考生的研究
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101701
Jonathan Wai , Matthew H. Lee , Harrison J. Kell

Using a sample of over 1.6 million scores of U.S. test takers on the Graduate Record Examination 2015–2020, this study broadly replicated prior findings going back over seven decades on overall academic skill and math-verbal tilt as a function of different field specialization. Individuals pursuing STEM degrees and STEM undergraduate backgrounds had stronger quantitative than verbal skills. Individuals pursuing arts/humanities degrees and arts/humanities undergraduate backgrounds had stronger verbal than quantitative skills. However, there were also differences regarding math-verbal tilt in the GRE relative to other samples. Academic skill patterns may be both a cause of or result of educational choices, and deeper consideration of these issues may ultimately have implications for expertise development for students who pursue fields such as the STEM or the arts/humanities.

这项研究使用了2015-2020年研究生入学考试(Graduate Record Examination)中160多万名美国考生的分数作为样本,广泛地复制了70多年前的研究结果,即整体学术技能和数学语言倾向是不同领域专业的函数。攻读STEM学位和STEM本科背景的人的定量能力强于口头表达能力。追求艺术/人文学位和艺术/人文本科背景的个人口头表达能力强于定量分析能力。然而,与其他样本相比,GRE的数学语言倾斜度也存在差异。学术技能模式既可能是教育选择的原因,也可能是教育选择的结果,对这些问题进行更深入的思考,最终可能会对从事STEM或艺术/人文等领域的学生的专业技能发展产生影响。
{"title":"Distributions of academic math-verbal tilt and overall academic skill of students specializing in different fields: A study of 1.6 million graduate record examination test takers","authors":"Jonathan Wai ,&nbsp;Matthew H. Lee ,&nbsp;Harrison J. Kell","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101701","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101701","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Using a sample of over 1.6 million scores of U.S. test takers on the Graduate Record Examination 2015–2020, this study broadly replicated prior findings going back over seven decades on overall academic skill and math-verbal tilt as a function of different field specialization. Individuals pursuing STEM degrees and STEM undergraduate backgrounds had stronger quantitative than verbal skills. Individuals pursuing arts/humanities degrees and arts/humanities undergraduate backgrounds had stronger verbal than quantitative skills. However, there were also differences regarding math-verbal tilt in the GRE relative to other samples. Academic skill patterns may be both a cause of or result of educational choices, and deeper consideration of these issues may ultimately have implications for expertise development for students who pursue fields such as the STEM or the arts/humanities.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45847110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Flynn effects are biased by differential item functioning over time: A test using overlapping items in Wechsler scales 弗林效应是有偏差的差异项目功能随着时间的推移:一个测试使用重叠项目韦氏量表
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101688
Corentin Gonthier , Jacques Grégoire

The items of intelligence tests can demonstrate differential item functioning across different groups: cross-sample differences in item difficulty or discrimination, independently of any difference of ability. This is also true of comparisons over time: as the cultural context changes, items may increase or decrease in difficulty. This phenomenon is well-known, but its impact on estimates of the Flynn effect has not been systematically investigated. In the current study, we tested differential item functioning in a subset of 111 items common to consecutive versions of the French WAIS-R (1989), WAIS-III (1999) and/or WAIS-IV (2009), using the three normative samples (total N = 2979). Over half the items had significant differential functioning over time, generally becoming more difficult from one version to the next for the same level of ability. The magnitude of differential item functioning tended to be small for each item separately, but the cumulative effect over all items led to underestimating the Flynn effect by about 3 IQ points per decade, a bias close to the expected size of the effect itself. In this case, this bias substantially affected the conclusions, even creating an ersatz negative Flynn effect for the 1999–2009 period, when in fact ability increased (1989–1999) or stagnated (1999–2009) when accounting for differential item functioning. We recommend that studies of the Flynn effect systematically investigate the possibility of differential item functioning to obtain unbiased ability estimates.

智力测验的项目可以证明不同群体的不同项目功能:项目难度或歧视的跨样本差异,独立于任何能力差异。随着时间的推移,比较也是如此:随着文化背景的变化,项目的难度可能会增加或减少。这种现象是众所周知的,但它对弗林效应估计的影响还没有被系统地研究过。在当前的研究中,我们使用三个标准样本(总N = 2979),测试了法语WAIS-R(1989)、WAIS-III(1999)和/或WAIS-IV(2009)连续版本中共有的111个项目的差异项目功能。随着时间的推移,超过一半的项目具有显著的功能差异,对于相同的能力水平,从一个版本到下一个版本通常变得更加困难。每个项目的差异功能的大小往往很小,但所有项目的累积效应导致弗林效应每十年被低估约3个智商点,这一偏差接近于效应本身的预期大小。在这种情况下,这种偏差极大地影响了结论,甚至在1999-2009年期间产生了假的负弗林效应,而实际上,当考虑到差异项目功能时,能力增加了(1989-1999)或停滞了(1999-2009)。我们建议弗林效应的研究系统地调查差异项目功能的可能性,以获得无偏的能力估计。
{"title":"Flynn effects are biased by differential item functioning over time: A test using overlapping items in Wechsler scales","authors":"Corentin Gonthier ,&nbsp;Jacques Grégoire","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101688","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101688","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>The items of intelligence tests can demonstrate differential item functioning across different groups: cross-sample differences in item difficulty or discrimination, independently of any difference of ability. This is also true of comparisons over time: as the cultural context changes, items may increase or decrease in difficulty. This phenomenon is well-known, but its impact on estimates of the Flynn effect has not been systematically investigated. In the current study, we tested differential item functioning in a subset of 111 items common to consecutive versions of the French WAIS-R (1989), WAIS-III (1999) and/or WAIS-IV (2009), using the three normative samples (total </span><em>N</em> = 2979). Over half the items had significant differential functioning over time, generally becoming more difficult from one version to the next for the same level of ability. The magnitude of differential item functioning tended to be small for each item separately, but the cumulative effect over all items led to underestimating the Flynn effect by about 3 IQ points per decade, a bias close to the expected size of the effect itself. In this case, this bias substantially affected the conclusions, even creating an ersatz negative Flynn effect for the 1999–2009 period, when in fact ability increased (1989–1999) or stagnated (1999–2009) when accounting for differential item functioning. We recommend that studies of the Flynn effect systematically investigate the possibility of differential item functioning to obtain unbiased ability estimates.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42823022","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Differentiation of general and specific abilities in intelligence. A bifactor study of age and gender differentiation in 8- to 19-year-olds 智力上的一般能力和特殊能力的区别8- 19岁儿童年龄和性别差异的双因素研究
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101669
Tommaso Feraco, Giorgia Cona

The structure of intelligence is not static. Differentiation hypotheses suggest that the contribution of g and specific abilities on tasks performance varies with age, but most research focused solely on g. Here we sought to test intelligence differentiation in both its general and specific components using a bifactor modelling approach that – previously ignored in literature – should be better suited to this aim. In parallel, the possible differences in intelligence differentiation between males and females are explored for the first time. A population of 8866 youths (8–19 years old) of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopment Cohort completed 12 tasks measuring four components of cognition (complex cognition, executive control, episodic memory, and social cognition) and the WRAT-4. Using bootstrap and meta-regression analysis, the bifactor-(S·I - 1) model shows that the variance explained differently changes over time according to peculiar development patterns of specific components. Executive functions lose specificity with age and their variance is explained only by g at the end of adolescence, episodic memory is increasingly explained by g, and complex cognition is explained more by g in males – and less in females – when age increases, also suggesting a possible role of gender in intelligence differentiation. We conclude discussing the importance of using adequate statistical models and we proposed studying differentiation also at the level of specific abilities to account for the complexity of the developmental phenomenon that could be better described by development priority theories.

智力的结构不是一成不变的。分化假设表明,g和特定能力对任务表现的贡献随年龄而变化,但大多数研究只关注g。在这里,我们试图使用一种双因素建模方法来测试智力分化的一般和特定成分,这种方法以前在文献中被忽视,但应该更适合于这一目标。同时,首次探讨了男性和女性在智力分化方面可能存在的差异。8866名8-19岁的费城神经发育队列青年完成了12项任务,测量了认知的四个组成部分(复杂认知、执行控制、情景记忆和社会认知)和WRAT-4。采用bootstrap和meta回归分析,双因子-(S·I - 1)模型表明,根据特定成分的特殊发展模式,方差随时间的变化具有不同的解释。执行功能随着年龄的增长而失去特异性,它们的差异只能在青春期结束时用g来解释,情景记忆越来越多地用g来解释,随着年龄的增长,复杂的认知在男性中更多地用g来解释,而在女性中则更少,这也表明性别在智力分化中可能起着作用。最后,我们讨论了使用适当的统计模型的重要性,并建议在特定能力的层面上研究差异,以解释发展现象的复杂性,而发展优先理论可以更好地描述这种复杂性。
{"title":"Differentiation of general and specific abilities in intelligence. A bifactor study of age and gender differentiation in 8- to 19-year-olds","authors":"Tommaso Feraco,&nbsp;Giorgia Cona","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101669","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101669","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The structure of intelligence is not static. Differentiation hypotheses suggest that the contribution of <em>g</em> and specific abilities on tasks performance varies with age, but most research focused solely on <em>g</em>. Here we sought to test intelligence differentiation in both its general and specific components using a bifactor modelling approach that – previously ignored in literature – should be better suited to this aim. In parallel, the possible differences in intelligence differentiation between males and females are explored for the first time. A population of 8866 youths (8–19 years old) of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopment Cohort completed 12 tasks measuring four components of cognition (complex cognition, executive control, episodic memory, and social cognition) and the WRAT-4. Using bootstrap and meta-regression analysis, the bifactor-(S·I - 1) model shows that the variance explained differently changes over time according to peculiar development patterns of specific components. Executive functions lose specificity with age and their variance is explained only by <em>g</em> at the end of adolescence, episodic memory is increasingly explained by <em>g</em>, and complex cognition is explained more by <em>g</em> in males – and less in females – when age increases, also suggesting a possible role of gender in intelligence differentiation. We conclude discussing the importance of using adequate statistical models and we proposed studying differentiation also at the level of specific abilities to account for the complexity of the developmental phenomenon that could be better described by development priority theories.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48766980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Cognitive ability has powerful, widespread and robust effects on social stratification: Evidence from the 1979 and 1997 US National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth 认知能力对社会分层具有强大、广泛和稳健的影响:来自1979年和1997年美国全国青年纵向调查的证据
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101686
Gary N. Marks

Few issues in the social sciences are as controversial as the role of cognitive ability for educational and subsequent socioeconomic attainments. There are a variety of arguments raised to dismiss, discount or discredit the role of cognitive ability: socioeconomic background is the dominant influence; if cognitive ability appears important, that is only because important predictors have been omitted; the relative importance of socioeconomic background and cognitive ability cannot be ascertained; and cognitive ability is simply a function of socioeconomic background and, for post-education socioeconomic attainments, education. This study analyses the effects of cognitive ability and socioeconomic background on a chronological sequence of social stratification outcomes - school grades, SAT and ACT scores, educational and occupational attainment, income and wealth - in data from the 1979 and 1997 National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth. The coefficients for cognitive ability decline marginally with the addition of socioeconomic background measures, including family-of-origin income averaged over several years, and wealth. In contrast, socioeconomic background coefficients decline substantially with the addition of cognitive ability. Net of educational attainment, cognitive ability has sizable effects on occupational attainment and income. Net of socioeconomic background, education and occupation, a one-standard-deviation difference in ability corresponds to a sizable 43% difference in positive wealth at around age 35 in the older cohort and a 25% increase in the younger cohort. Therefore, contrary to dominant narratives, cognitive ability is important to a range of social stratification outcomes, and its effects cannot be attributed to socioeconomic background or educational attainment.

在社会科学中,很少有问题像认知能力在教育和随后的社会经济成就中的作用那样有争议。人们提出了各种各样的论点来驳斥、贬低或质疑认知能力的作用:社会经济背景是主要的影响;如果认知能力看起来很重要,那只是因为重要的预测因素被忽略了;社会经济背景和认知能力的相对重要性无法确定;认知能力只是社会经济背景的函数,对于教育后的社会经济成就来说,是教育的函数。本研究分析了认知能力和社会经济背景对社会分层结果按时间顺序的影响——学校成绩、SAT和ACT分数、教育和职业成就、收入和财富——数据来自1979年和1997年全国青年纵向调查。随着社会经济背景指标的增加,认知能力的系数略有下降,社会经济背景指标包括几年来的平均家庭收入和财富。相反,社会经济背景系数随着认知能力的增加而显著下降。除受教育程度外,认知能力对职业成就和收入有相当大的影响。除去社会经济背景、教育和职业因素,能力上一个标准差的差异对应的是,年龄较大的人群在35岁左右的正财富会有43%的巨大差异,而年龄较小的人群则会增加25%。因此,与主流叙事相反,认知能力对一系列社会分层结果很重要,其影响不能归因于社会经济背景或教育程度。
{"title":"Cognitive ability has powerful, widespread and robust effects on social stratification: Evidence from the 1979 and 1997 US National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth","authors":"Gary N. Marks","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101686","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101686","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Few issues in the social sciences are as controversial as the role of cognitive ability for educational and subsequent socioeconomic attainments. There are a variety of arguments raised to dismiss, discount or discredit the role of cognitive ability: socioeconomic background is the dominant influence; if cognitive ability appears important, that is only because important predictors have been omitted; the relative importance of socioeconomic background and cognitive ability cannot be ascertained; and cognitive ability is simply a function of socioeconomic background and, for post-education socioeconomic attainments, education. This study analyses the effects of cognitive ability and socioeconomic background on a chronological sequence of social stratification outcomes - school grades, SAT and ACT scores, educational and occupational attainment, income and wealth - in data from the 1979 and 1997 National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth. The coefficients for cognitive ability decline marginally with the addition of socioeconomic background measures, including family-of-origin income averaged over several years, and wealth. In contrast, socioeconomic background coefficients decline substantially with the addition of cognitive ability. Net of educational attainment, cognitive ability has sizable effects on occupational attainment and income. Net of socioeconomic background, education and occupation, a one-standard-deviation difference in ability corresponds to a sizable 43% difference in positive wealth at around age 35 in the older cohort and a 25% increase in the younger cohort. Therefore, contrary to dominant narratives, cognitive ability is important to a range of social stratification outcomes, and its effects cannot be attributed to socioeconomic background or educational attainment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42784732","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
IQ in adolescence and cognition over 50 years later: The mediating role of adult personality 青春期智商与50年后认知:成人人格的中介作用
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101682
Yannick Stephan , Angelina R. Sutin , Martina Luchetti , Damaris Aschwanden , Antonio Terracciano

There is substantial evidence for the association between higher early life IQ and better cognition in late life. To advance knowledge on potential pathways, the present study tested whether Five-Factor Model personality traits in adulthood mediate the association between adolescent IQ and later-life cognition. Participants were from the Graduate sample of the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study on Aging (WLS; N = 3585). IQ was assessed in 1957 (about age 17), personality was assessed in 2003–2005 (age = 64), and cognition was assessed in 2011 (age = 71). Controlling for demographic factors, higher IQ in adolescence was related to higher openness, lower neuroticism, lower extraversion, lower agreeableness and lower conscientiousness in adulthood. Higher openness partially mediated the association between higher IQ and better cognition. Additional analyses indicated that the pattern of associations between IQ, personality and cognition was similar when the polygenic score for cognition was included as an additional covariate. Although effect size were small, this study provides new evidence that openness in adulthood is on the pathway between early life IQ and later-life cognition.

有大量证据表明,早期较高的智商与晚年较好的认知能力之间存在关联。为了进一步了解潜在的途径,本研究测试了五因素模型人格特征在成年期是否介导青少年智商和晚年认知之间的关联。参与者来自威斯康星老龄化纵向研究(WLS;n = 3585)。1957年(大约17岁)进行了智商测试,2003-2005年(年龄= 64)进行了人格测试,2011年(年龄= 71)进行了认知测试。在人口统计学因素控制下,青春期高智商与成年期开放性、神经质性、外向性、宜人性、尽责性显著相关。较高的开放性部分介导了高智商和更好的认知之间的关联。进一步的分析表明,当将认知的多基因得分作为一个额外的协变量时,智商、人格和认知之间的关联模式是相似的。虽然效应量很小,但这项研究提供了新的证据,证明成年期的开放性是早期生活智商和晚年认知之间的途径。
{"title":"IQ in adolescence and cognition over 50 years later: The mediating role of adult personality","authors":"Yannick Stephan ,&nbsp;Angelina R. Sutin ,&nbsp;Martina Luchetti ,&nbsp;Damaris Aschwanden ,&nbsp;Antonio Terracciano","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101682","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101682","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>There is substantial evidence for the association between higher early life IQ and better cognition in late life. To advance knowledge on potential pathways, the present study tested whether Five-Factor Model personality traits in adulthood mediate the association between adolescent IQ and later-life cognition. Participants were from the Graduate sample of the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study on Aging (WLS; <em>N</em> = 3585). IQ was assessed in 1957 (about age 17), personality was assessed in 2003–2005 (age = 64), and cognition was assessed in 2011 (age = 71). Controlling for demographic factors, higher IQ in adolescence was related to higher openness, lower neuroticism, lower extraversion, lower agreeableness and lower conscientiousness in adulthood. Higher openness partially mediated the association between higher IQ and better cognition. Additional analyses indicated that the pattern of associations between IQ, personality and cognition was similar when the polygenic score for cognition was included as an additional covariate. Although effect size were small, this study provides new evidence that openness in adulthood is on the pathway between early life IQ and later-life cognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10806914","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing speed mediates the development of tech tilt and academic tilt in adolescence 处理速度对青少年技术倾向和学业倾向的发展起中介作用
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101648
Thomas R. Coyle

Tilt represents a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences between two abilities, indicating strength in one ability (e.g., technical) and weakness in another ability (e.g., academic). The current study examined age differences in tilt, processing speed, and general intelligence (g) in adolescence for 13- to 17-year-olds. Tilt was measured using the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery and was based on differences in technical (mechanical and electrical) and academic abilities (math or verbal). These differences yielded tech tilt (technical > academic) and academic tilt (academic > technical). Older ages correlated with greater tech tilt and academic tilt and with faster processing speed. In addition, processing speed strongly mediated age-tilt relations. The mediating effects of processing speed (on age-tilt relations) were partly related to g. The results supported investment theories, which assume that age-related increases in speed accelerate the acquisition of specific abilities that produce tilt. Adding g to the model supported cascade theories, which assume that age-related increases in speed boost general abilities (e.g., g), which in turn accelerate the acquisition of specific abilities related to tilt. Future research should examine different types of tilt and factors that influence tilt such as developmental period and ability specialization.

倾斜代表了一种特定能力的模式,基于两种能力之间的学科差异,表明一种能力的优势(如技术)和另一种能力的劣势(如学术)。目前的研究调查了13至17岁青少年在倾斜度、处理速度和一般智力(g)方面的年龄差异。倾斜度是用武装部队职业能力测试来测量的,基于技术(机械和电气)和学术能力(数学或语言)的差异。这些差异产生了技术倾斜。学术性)和学术性倾向(Academic >技术)。年龄越大,技术倾向和学术倾向越大,处理速度越快。此外,加工速度在年龄倾向关系中起中介作用。处理速度(对年龄倾斜关系)的中介作用部分与g有关。结果支持投资理论,该理论假设年龄相关的速度增加加速了产生倾斜的特定能力的习得。将g添加到模型中支持级联理论,该理论假设与年龄相关的速度增加会提高一般能力(例如g),这反过来又会加速与倾斜相关的特定能力的获得。未来的研究应进一步探讨不同类型的倾斜以及影响倾斜的因素,如发育时期和能力专业化。
{"title":"Processing speed mediates the development of tech tilt and academic tilt in adolescence","authors":"Thomas R. Coyle","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101648","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101648","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><em>Tilt</em> represents a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences between two abilities, indicating strength in one ability (e.g., technical) and weakness in another ability (e.g., academic). The current study examined age differences in tilt, processing speed, and general intelligence (<em>g</em>) in adolescence for 13- to 17-year-olds. Tilt was measured using the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery and was based on differences in technical (mechanical and electrical) and academic abilities (math or verbal). These differences yielded tech tilt (technical &gt; academic) and academic tilt (academic &gt; technical). Older ages correlated with greater tech tilt and academic tilt and with faster processing speed. In addition, processing speed strongly mediated age-tilt relations. The mediating effects of processing speed (on age-tilt relations) were partly related to <em>g</em>. The results supported investment theories, which assume that age-related increases in speed accelerate the acquisition of specific abilities that produce tilt. Adding <em>g</em> to the model supported cascade theories, which assume that age-related increases in speed boost general abilities (e.g., <em>g</em>), which in turn accelerate the acquisition of specific abilities related to tilt. Future research should examine different types of tilt and factors that influence tilt such as developmental period and ability specialization.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42521017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1