首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
Remember this: Age moderation of genetic and environmental contributions to verbal episodic memory from midlife through late adulthood 记住这一点:从中年到成年后期,遗传和环境对言语情景记忆的影响的年龄调节
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101759
Susan E. Luczak , Christopher R. Beam , Shandell Pahlen , Morgan Lynch , Matthew Pilgrim , Chandra A. Reynolds , Matthew S. Panizzon , Vibeke S. Catts , Kaare Christensen , Deborah Finkel , Carol E. Franz , William S. Kremen , Teresa Lee , Matt McGue , Marianne Nygaard , Brenda L. Plassman , Keith E. Whitfield , Nancy L. Pedersen , Margaret Gatz , for the IGEMS Consortium

It is well documented that memory is heritable and that older adults tend to have poorer memory performance than younger adults. However, whether the magnitudes of genetic and environmental contributions to late-life verbal episodic memory ability differ from those at earlier ages remains unresolved. Twins from 12 studies participating in the Interplay of Genes and Environment in Multiple Studies (IGEMS) consortium constituted the analytic sample. Verbal episodic memory was assessed with immediate word list recall (N = 35,204 individuals; 21,792 twin pairs) and prose recall (N = 3805 individuals; 2028 twin pairs), with scores harmonized across studies. Average test performance was lower in successively older age groups for both measures. Twin models found significant age moderation for both measures, with total inter-individual variance increasing significantly with age, although it was not possible definitively to attribute the increase specifically to either genetic or environmental sources. Pooled results across all 12 studies were compared to results where we successively dropped each study (leave-one-out) to assure results were not due to an outlier. We conclude the models indicated an overall increase in variance for verbal episodic memory that was driven by a combination of increases in the genetic and nonshared environmental parameters that were not independently statistically significant. In contrast to reported results for other cognitive domains, differences in environmental exposures are comparatively important for verbal episodic memory, especially word list learning.

有充分的证据表明,记忆力是可遗传的,老年人的记忆力往往比年轻人差。然而,基因和环境对晚年言语情景记忆能力的影响程度是否与早期不同仍悬而未决。来自12项参与多项研究中基因与环境相互作用(IGEMS)联盟的研究的双胞胎构成了分析样本。言语情景记忆采用即时单词列表回忆(N=35204人;21792对双胞胎)和散文回忆(N=3805人;2028对双胞胎)进行评估,各研究的得分一致。两项测量的平均测试成绩在年龄较大的人群中均较低。双生子模型发现,这两项指标都存在显著的年龄调节,个体间总方差随着年龄的增长而显著增加,尽管不可能明确地将这种增加归因于遗传或环境来源。将所有12项研究的汇总结果与我们连续放弃每项研究(遗漏一项)的结果进行比较,以确保结果不是由异常值引起的。我们得出的结论是,这些模型表明,言语情景记忆的方差总体增加,这是由遗传和非共享环境参数的增加共同驱动的,这些参数在统计学上并不独立显著。与其他认知领域的研究结果相反,环境暴露的差异对言语情景记忆,尤其是单词表学习来说相对重要。
{"title":"Remember this: Age moderation of genetic and environmental contributions to verbal episodic memory from midlife through late adulthood","authors":"Susan E. Luczak ,&nbsp;Christopher R. Beam ,&nbsp;Shandell Pahlen ,&nbsp;Morgan Lynch ,&nbsp;Matthew Pilgrim ,&nbsp;Chandra A. Reynolds ,&nbsp;Matthew S. Panizzon ,&nbsp;Vibeke S. Catts ,&nbsp;Kaare Christensen ,&nbsp;Deborah Finkel ,&nbsp;Carol E. Franz ,&nbsp;William S. Kremen ,&nbsp;Teresa Lee ,&nbsp;Matt McGue ,&nbsp;Marianne Nygaard ,&nbsp;Brenda L. Plassman ,&nbsp;Keith E. Whitfield ,&nbsp;Nancy L. Pedersen ,&nbsp;Margaret Gatz ,&nbsp;for the IGEMS Consortium","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101759","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101759","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>It is well documented that memory is heritable and that older adults tend to have poorer memory performance than younger adults. However, whether the magnitudes of genetic and environmental contributions to late-life verbal episodic memory ability differ from those at earlier ages remains unresolved. Twins from 12 studies participating in the Interplay of Genes and Environment in Multiple Studies (IGEMS) consortium constituted the analytic sample. Verbal episodic memory was assessed with immediate word list recall (</span><em>N</em> = 35,204 individuals; 21,792 twin pairs) and prose recall (<em>N</em> = 3805 individuals; 2028 twin pairs), with scores harmonized across studies. Average test performance was lower in successively older age groups for both measures. Twin models found significant age moderation for both measures, with total inter-individual variance increasing significantly with age, although it was not possible definitively to attribute the increase specifically to either genetic or environmental sources. Pooled results across all 12 studies were compared to results where we successively dropped each study (leave-one-out) to assure results were not due to an outlier. We conclude the models indicated an overall increase in variance for verbal episodic memory that was driven by a combination of increases in the genetic and nonshared environmental parameters that were not independently statistically significant. In contrast to reported results for other cognitive domains, differences in environmental exposures are comparatively important for verbal episodic memory, especially word list learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 101759"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10306264/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9746950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
General cognitive ability, as assessed by self-reported ACT scores, is associated with reduced emotional responding: Evidence from a Dynamic Affect Reactivity Task 通过自我报告的ACT分数评估的一般认知能力与情绪反应的减少有关:来自动态影响反应任务的证据
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101760
Michael D. Robinson , Roberta L. Irvin , Todd A. Pringle , Robert J. Klein

Dual process theories often contrast a hot, reactive affective system with a cool, reflective cognitive system. The cognitive system permits rationality and reasoning, but may inhibit spontaneous affect. Such frameworks would seem to suggest that individual differences in general cognitive ability, which is linked to abstract forms of reasoning, may impact dynamic components of emotional reactivity. In two studies involving five samples (total N = 631), participants were asked to continuously rate their emotional experiences in response to presented affective images. General cognitive ability, assessed, by proxy, with self-reported ACT scores, was linked to less intense peak reactions, peak reactions that were delayed, and/or to velocities of affect change that were less pronounced. Such relationships tended to be observed regardless of whether images were positive or negative. The findings provide support for dual process theorizing and suggest that general cognitive ability modulates dynamic components of emotional responding.

双过程理论经常将热的、反应性的情感系统与冷的、反思性的认知系统进行对比。认知系统允许理性和推理,但可能抑制自发的情感。这些框架似乎表明,与抽象推理形式相关的一般认知能力的个体差异可能会影响情绪反应的动态组成部分。在两项涉及5个样本(总N = 631)的研究中,参与者被要求对所呈现的情感图像进行持续的情感体验评分。一般认知能力,通过自我报告的ACT分数来评估,与较不强烈的峰值反应、延迟的峰值反应和/或不太明显的情感变化速度有关。无论图像是积极的还是消极的,都倾向于观察到这种关系。这一发现为双过程理论提供了支持,并表明一般认知能力调节情绪反应的动态成分。
{"title":"General cognitive ability, as assessed by self-reported ACT scores, is associated with reduced emotional responding: Evidence from a Dynamic Affect Reactivity Task","authors":"Michael D. Robinson ,&nbsp;Roberta L. Irvin ,&nbsp;Todd A. Pringle ,&nbsp;Robert J. Klein","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101760","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101760","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Dual process theories often contrast a hot, reactive affective system with a cool, reflective cognitive system. The cognitive system permits rationality and reasoning, but may inhibit spontaneous affect. Such frameworks would seem to suggest that individual differences in general cognitive ability, which is linked to abstract forms of reasoning, may impact dynamic components of emotional reactivity. In two studies involving five samples (total </span><em>N</em> = 631), participants were asked to continuously rate their emotional experiences in response to presented affective images. General cognitive ability, assessed, by proxy, with self-reported ACT scores, was linked to less intense peak reactions, peak reactions that were delayed, and/or to velocities of affect change that were less pronounced. Such relationships tended to be observed regardless of whether images were positive or negative. The findings provide support for dual process theorizing and suggest that general cognitive ability modulates dynamic components of emotional responding.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 101760"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42987364","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Is there a g in gunslinger? Cognitive predictors of firearms proficiency 枪手里有g吗?枪械熟练程度的认知预测因素
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101768
Jeffrey M. Cucina , Kimberly J. Wilson , Philip T. Walmsley , Lisa M. Votraw , Theodore L. Hayes

This study addressed a gap in the research literature by evaluating the validity of general mental ability (g) and personality test scores for prediction of firearms proficiency via shooting range performance, an entirely objective task-based criterion. It was hypothesized that mental ability test scores would be positively related to firearms proficiency based on past research in related areas (e.g., g predicts skill acquisition and training performance) and conceptual similarities between firearms proficiency and cognitive tasks. Using 4 datasets with a combined sample size of 22,525 individuals, this hypothesis was confirmed: g had operational validities ranging from .162 to .188 and logical reasoning had operational validities ranging from .179 to .268 after correcting for range restriction and criterion unreliability. Mental ability test scores predicted an entirely psychomotor criterion task: use of firearms to hit targets at a pre-determined level of accuracy. Most of the validity appears to be attributable to g, but a post hoc analysis indicated that writing ability acted as a suppressor (i.e., the validity of g increased when writing ability was included in a regression model). Conscientiousness was hypothesized to have a positive relationship with firearms performance and emotional stability was hypothesized to have positive linear and quadratic relationships. In contrast, it was observed that conscientiousness had a negative operational validity (−.079) and emotional stability lacked validity relative to the firearms proficiency criterion. The implications for individual differences research and practice are discussed.

本研究通过评估一般心理能力(g)和人格测试分数在通过射击场表现(一个完全客观的基于任务的标准)预测枪支熟练程度方面的有效性,解决了研究文献中的空白。基于过去相关领域的研究(例如,g预测技能习得和训练表现)以及枪械熟练程度与认知任务之间的概念相似性,假设心理能力测试分数与枪械熟练程度呈正相关。使用4个数据集,总样本量为22,525人,这一假设得到证实:g的操作效度范围为0.162至0.188,逻辑推理的操作效度范围为0.179至0.268,校正了范围限制和标准不可靠性。心理能力测试分数预测了一个完全精神运动的标准任务:使用枪支以预先确定的精度击中目标。大部分效度似乎归因于g,但事后分析表明,写作能力起着抑制作用(即,当写作能力被纳入回归模型时,g的效度增加)。假设严谨性与枪械性能存在正相关关系,假设情绪稳定性与枪械性能存在正线性和二次关系。相对于枪械熟练度标准,严谨性的操作效度为负(- 0.079),情绪稳定性的效度为负。讨论了个体差异研究和实践的意义。
{"title":"Is there a g in gunslinger? Cognitive predictors of firearms proficiency","authors":"Jeffrey M. Cucina ,&nbsp;Kimberly J. Wilson ,&nbsp;Philip T. Walmsley ,&nbsp;Lisa M. Votraw ,&nbsp;Theodore L. Hayes","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101768","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101768","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study addressed a gap in the research literature by evaluating the validity of general mental ability (<em>g</em>) and personality test scores for prediction of firearms proficiency via shooting range performance, an entirely objective task-based criterion. It was hypothesized that mental ability test scores would be positively related to firearms proficiency based on past research in related areas (e.g., <em>g</em> predicts skill acquisition and training performance) and conceptual similarities between firearms proficiency and cognitive tasks. Using 4 datasets with a combined sample size of 22,525 individuals, this hypothesis was confirmed: <em>g</em> had operational validities ranging from .162 to .188 and logical reasoning had operational validities ranging from .179 to .268 after correcting for range restriction and criterion unreliability. Mental ability test scores predicted an entirely psychomotor criterion task: use of firearms to hit targets at a pre-determined level of accuracy. Most of the validity appears to be attributable to <em>g</em>, but a post hoc analysis indicated that writing ability acted as a suppressor (i.e., the validity of <em>g</em> increased when writing ability was included in a regression model). Conscientiousness was hypothesized to have a positive relationship with firearms performance and emotional stability was hypothesized to have positive linear and quadratic relationships. In contrast, it was observed that conscientiousness had a negative operational validity (−.079) and emotional stability lacked validity relative to the firearms proficiency criterion. The implications for individual differences research and practice are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 101768"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44673808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The latent structure of emerging cognitive abilities: An infant twin study 新兴认知能力的潜在结构:一项婴儿双胞胎研究
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101771
Giorgia Bussu , Mark Taylor , Kristiina Tammimies , Angelica Ronald , Terje Falck-Ytter

It is well known that genetic factors account for up to 70% of variability in cognition from childhood to adulthood. However, less is known about the first year of life. This study investigated the etiological factors influencing individual variability in different domains of emerging cognitive and motor abilities in early infancy, and to what extent genetic and environmental influences are unique or shared across different domains. We compared multivariate twin models built on scores from the five scales of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) in a community sample of monozygotic and dizygotic twins at 5 months of age (n=567). The results indicated a hierarchical etiological structure whereby a general genetic latent factor accounted for 54% of variance underlying the different domains of emerging cognitive and motor abilities (A=0.54, confidence interval CI=[0; 0.82]). We also found additional genetic influences that were specific to early motor and language development. Unlike previous findings on older children, we did not find significant influences of shared environment on the shared factor (C=0, CI=[0, 0.57]), or any specific scale. Furthermore, influences of unique environment, which include measurement error, were moderate and statistically significant (E=0.46, CI=0.18; 0.81]). This study provides strong evidence for a unitary hierarchical structure across different domains of emerging cognition. Evidence that a single common etiological factor, which we term infant g, contributes to a range of different abilities supports the view that in young infants, intrinsic and general neurodevelopmental processes are key drivers of observable behavioural differences in specific domains.

众所周知,从童年到成年,遗传因素占认知变化的70%。然而,人们对生命的第一年知之甚少。本研究调查了影响婴儿早期认知和运动能力不同领域个体差异的病因学因素,以及遗传和环境影响在不同领域的独特或共有程度。我们比较了建立在马伦早期学习量表(MSEL)五个量表得分上的多变量双胞胎模型,该模型建立在5个月大的同卵双胞胎和异卵双胞胎社区样本中(n=567)。结果显示了一个分层的病因结构,其中一般的遗传潜在因素占54%的变异,潜在的不同领域的新兴认知和运动能力(a =0.54,置信区间CI=[0;0.82])。我们还发现了额外的遗传影响,这是早期运动和语言发展所特有的。与先前对年龄较大的儿童的研究结果不同,我们没有发现共享环境对共享因素(C=0, CI=[0,0.57])或任何特定量表的显著影响。此外,包括测量误差在内的独特环境的影响是中等且具有统计学意义(E=0.46, CI=0.18;0.81])。这项研究为跨新兴认知不同领域的统一层次结构提供了强有力的证据。有证据表明,一个单一的常见病因(我们称之为婴儿g)会导致一系列不同的能力,这支持了这样一种观点,即在年幼婴儿中,内在和一般的神经发育过程是特定领域可观察到的行为差异的关键驱动因素。
{"title":"The latent structure of emerging cognitive abilities: An infant twin study","authors":"Giorgia Bussu ,&nbsp;Mark Taylor ,&nbsp;Kristiina Tammimies ,&nbsp;Angelica Ronald ,&nbsp;Terje Falck-Ytter","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101771","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101771","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It is well known that genetic factors account for up to 70% of variability in cognition from childhood to adulthood. However, less is known about the first year of life. This study investigated the etiological factors influencing individual variability in different domains of emerging cognitive and motor abilities in early infancy, and to what extent genetic and environmental influences are unique or shared across different domains. We compared multivariate twin models built on scores from the five scales of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) in a community sample of monozygotic and dizygotic twins at 5 months of age (n=567). The results indicated a hierarchical etiological structure whereby a general genetic latent factor accounted for 54% of variance underlying the different domains of emerging cognitive and motor abilities (A=0.54, confidence interval CI=[0; 0.82]). We also found additional genetic influences that were specific to early motor and language development. Unlike previous findings on older children, we did not find significant influences of shared environment on the shared factor (C=0, CI=[0, 0.57]), or any specific scale. Furthermore, influences of unique environment, which include measurement error, were moderate and statistically significant (E=0.46, CI=0.18; 0.81]). This study provides strong evidence for a unitary hierarchical structure across different domains of emerging cognition. Evidence that a single common etiological factor, which we term <em>infant g</em>, contributes to a range of different abilities supports the view that in young infants, intrinsic and general neurodevelopmental processes are key drivers of observable behavioural differences in specific domains.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 101771"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47287484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Generational intelligence tests score changes in Spain: Are we asking the right question? 西班牙的代际智力测验成绩变化:我们问对问题了吗?
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101772
Roberto Colom, Luis F. García, Pei Chun Shih, Francisco J. Abad

Generational intelligence test score gains have been documented worldwide in the twentieth century. However, recent evidence suggests these increased scores are coming to an end in some world regions. Here we compare two cohorts of university freshmen. The first cohort (n = 311) was assessed in 1991, whereas the second cohort (n = 349) was assessed thirty years later (2022). These cohorts completed the same intelligence battery including eight standardized speeded and power tests tapping reasoning (abstract and quantitative), language (vocabulary, verbal comprehension, and verbal meanings), rote calculation, and visuospatial relations. The results revealed a global gain of 3.5 IQ points but also upward and downward changes at the test level. The 2022 cohort outperformed the 1991 cohort on reasoning (abstract and quantitative), verbal comprehension, and vocabulary, whereas the 1991 cohort outscored the 2022 cohort on rote calculation, visuospatial relations (mental rotation and identical figures), and verbal meanings. These findings are thought to support one key claim made by James Flynn: generational changes on the specific cognitive abilities and skills tapped by standardized tests should be expected without appreciable or substantive changes in the structure of the intelligence construct identified within generations. This main conclusion is discussed with respect to theoretical causal implications putatively derived from current intelligence psychometric models.

在20世纪,世界各地都有记录表明,代际智力测试成绩的增长。然而,最近的证据表明,在世界上的一些地区,这种增长的分数正在结束。这里我们比较了两组大学新生。第一组(n = 311)在1991年进行评估,而第二组(n = 349)在30年后(2022年)进行评估。这些人完成了同样的智力测试,包括8项标准化的速度和能力测试,涉及推理(抽象和定量)、语言(词汇、口头理解和口头意义)、死记硬背计算和视觉空间关系。结果显示,总体而言,他们的智商增加了3.5分,但测试水平也有上下变化。2022年出生的人在推理(抽象和定量)、语言理解和词汇方面的表现优于1991年出生的人,而1991年出生的人在死记硬背的计算、视觉空间关系(心理旋转和相同的数字)和口头意义方面的表现优于2022年出生的人。这些发现被认为支持了詹姆斯·弗林(James Flynn)提出的一个关键观点:标准化测试所揭示的特定认知能力和技能的代际变化应该是预料之中的,而不会在几代人之间确定的智力结构发生明显或实质性的变化。这一主要结论是讨论了相对于理论的因果影响,从目前的智力心理测量模型推定。
{"title":"Generational intelligence tests score changes in Spain: Are we asking the right question?","authors":"Roberto Colom,&nbsp;Luis F. García,&nbsp;Pei Chun Shih,&nbsp;Francisco J. Abad","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101772","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101772","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Generational intelligence test score gains have been documented worldwide in the twentieth century. However, recent evidence suggests these increased scores are coming to an end in some world regions. Here we compare two cohorts of university freshmen. The first cohort (<em>n</em> = 311) was assessed in 1991, whereas the second cohort (<em>n</em><span> = 349) was assessed thirty years later (2022). These cohorts completed the same intelligence battery including eight standardized speeded and power tests tapping reasoning (abstract and quantitative), language (vocabulary, verbal comprehension, and verbal meanings), rote calculation, and visuospatial relations. The results revealed a global gain of 3.5 IQ points but also upward and downward changes at the test level. The 2022 cohort outperformed the 1991 cohort on reasoning (abstract and quantitative), verbal comprehension, and vocabulary, whereas the 1991 cohort outscored the 2022 cohort on rote calculation, visuospatial relations (mental rotation and identical figures), and verbal meanings. These findings are thought to support one key claim made by James Flynn: generational changes on the specific cognitive abilities and skills tapped by standardized tests should be expected without appreciable or substantive changes in the structure of the intelligence construct identified within generations. This main conclusion is discussed with respect to theoretical causal implications putatively derived from current intelligence psychometric models.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"99 ","pages":"Article 101772"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45513024","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
All (tilt) models are wrong, but some are useful: A reply to Sorjonen et al.’s (2023) critique of tilt 所有(倾斜)模型都是错误的,但有些是有用的:回复Sorjonen等人(2023)对倾斜的批评
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101749
Thomas R. Coyle

Tilt refers to a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences in two abilities (e.g., math and verbal), yielding an ability profile with strengths and weaknesses. A common type of tilt is ability tilt based on math and verbal scores, yielding math tilt (math>verbal) and verbal tilt (verbal>math). This article responds to Sorjonen, Ingre, Nilsonne, and Melin's (2023, this issue) critique of tilt and investment theories. In their critique, Sorjonen et al. claim that tilt results are spurious and that investment theories cannot explain tilt. Contra Sorjonen et al.'s claims, the current article argues that the nomological network of tilt relations is not spurious but is parsimonious, falsifiable, and supports the predictions of investment theories. The nomological network indicates that (a) tilt levels increase with age in adolescence and are mediated by processing speed; (b) males show math tilt, whereas females show verbal tilt; and (c) math tilt correlates positively with analogous criteria (e.g., science and math) and negatively with competing criteria (e.g., verbal), whereas verbal tilt shows the opposite pattern. It is argued that these (and other) findings are not spurious but can be parsimoniously explained by investment theories, which assume that investment in a particular domain (e.g., STEM; science, technology, engineering, math) boosts the development of analogous abilities (e.g., math) and inhibits the development of competing abilities (e.g., verbal). The article concludes with a discussion of future research on tilt, focusing on factors that may affect the development of tilt and its predictive power (e.g., trait complexes and developmental dedifferentiation).

倾斜是指一种特定能力的模式,基于两种能力(如数学和语言)的学科内差异,产生一个有优点和缺点的能力档案。一种常见的倾斜类型是基于数学和语言成绩的能力倾斜,产生数学倾斜(数学>;语言)和语言倾斜(语言>;数学)。本文回应了Sorjonen、Ingre、Nilsone和Melin(2023年,本期)对倾斜和投资理论的批评。在他们的批评中,Sorjonen等人声称倾斜结果是虚假的,投资理论无法解释倾斜。与Sorjonen等人的说法相反,本文认为倾斜关系的法理网络不是虚假的,而是吝啬的、可证伪的,并支持投资理论的预测。法理网络表明:(a)青春期的倾斜水平随着年龄的增长而增加,并由处理速度介导;(b) 男性表现出数学倾向,而女性表现出言语倾向;(c)数学倾向与类似标准(如科学和数学)呈正相关,与竞争标准(如语言)呈负相关,而语言倾向则表现出相反的模式。有人认为,这些(和其他)发现并非虚假,但可以用投资理论来简单解释,投资理论认为,对特定领域(如STEM;科学、技术、工程、数学)的投资促进了类似能力(如数学)的发展,并抑制了竞争能力(如语言)的发展。文章最后讨论了未来对倾斜的研究,重点讨论了可能影响倾斜发展及其预测能力的因素(如性状复合体和发育去分化)。
{"title":"All (tilt) models are wrong, but some are useful: A reply to Sorjonen et al.’s (2023) critique of tilt","authors":"Thomas R. Coyle","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101749","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101749","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><em>Tilt</em> refers to a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences in two abilities (e.g., math and verbal), yielding an ability profile with strengths and weaknesses. A common type of tilt is ability tilt based on math and verbal scores, yielding math tilt (math&gt;verbal) and verbal tilt (verbal&gt;math). This article responds to Sorjonen, Ingre, Nilsonne, and Melin's (2023, this issue) critique of tilt and investment theories. In their critique, Sorjonen et al. claim that tilt results are spurious and that investment theories cannot explain tilt. Contra Sorjonen et al.'s claims, the current article argues that the nomological network of tilt relations is not spurious but is parsimonious, falsifiable, and supports the predictions of investment theories. The nomological network indicates that (a) tilt levels increase with age in adolescence and are mediated by processing speed; (b) males show math tilt, whereas females show verbal tilt; and (c) math tilt correlates positively with analogous criteria (e.g., science and math) and negatively with competing criteria (e.g., verbal), whereas verbal tilt shows the opposite pattern. It is argued that these (and other) findings are not spurious but can be parsimoniously explained by investment theories, which assume that investment in a particular domain (e.g., STEM; science, technology, engineering, math) boosts the development of analogous abilities (e.g., math) and inhibits the development of competing abilities (e.g., verbal). The article concludes with a discussion of future research on tilt, focusing on factors that may affect the development of tilt and its predictive power (e.g., trait complexes and developmental dedifferentiation).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101749"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49718402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relations between intelligence index score discrepancies and psychopathology symptoms in the EDEN mother-child birth cohort EDEN母婴出生队列智力指数得分差异与精神病理症状的关系
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101753
Victoria Shevchenko , Ghislaine Labouret , Ava Guez , Sylvana Côté , Barbara Heude , Hugo Peyre , Franck Ramus

The aim of this study was to provide a comprehensive understanding of the potential linkages between intelligence and psychopathology across the full IQ range, while considering both absolute IQ scores and discrepancies between them. We drew data from the EDEN mother-child birth cohort, gathered at two time points: 5.5 and 11.5 years of age. We examined three instruments assessing psychopathology: the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, the Child Behavior Checklist, and the Mental Health and Social Inadaptation Assessment for Adolescents. We focused on four distinct scales: internalizing disorder, conduct disorder, social problems, and ADHD symptoms.

Our analyses first examined correlations between Full-scale IQ, Verbal IQ, Performance IQ, and psychopathology scores. Subsequently, we explored correlations between absolute and relative verbal-performance IQ discrepancies and psychopathology scores. In general, we found that relations between intelligence index scores and psychopathology scales were generally null or negative (high IQ associated with fewer psychopathology symptoms). Our results do not lend support to the hypothesis that high intelligence or index score discrepancies are risk factors for psychopathology in children and adolescents.

本研究的目的是在考虑绝对智商分数和两者之间的差异的同时,全面了解整个智商范围内智力和精神病理之间的潜在联系。我们从EDEN的母婴出生队列中收集了两个时间点的数据:5.5岁和11.5岁。我们检查了三种评估精神病理学的工具:力量和困难问卷、儿童行为检查表和青少年心理健康和社会不适应评估。我们专注于四个不同的尺度:内化障碍、行为障碍、社会问题和多动症症状。我们的分析首先检查了全面智商、语言智商、表现智商和精神病理学分数之间的相关性。随后,我们探讨了绝对和相对语言表现智商差异与精神病理学分数之间的相关性。总的来说,我们发现智力指数得分与精神病理量表之间的关系通常为零或负相关(高智商与较少的精神病理症状相关)。我们的结果不支持高智力或指数得分差异是儿童和青少年精神病理的危险因素的假设。
{"title":"Relations between intelligence index score discrepancies and psychopathology symptoms in the EDEN mother-child birth cohort","authors":"Victoria Shevchenko ,&nbsp;Ghislaine Labouret ,&nbsp;Ava Guez ,&nbsp;Sylvana Côté ,&nbsp;Barbara Heude ,&nbsp;Hugo Peyre ,&nbsp;Franck Ramus","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101753","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101753","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>The aim of this study was to provide a comprehensive understanding of the potential linkages between intelligence and psychopathology across the full IQ range, while considering both absolute IQ scores and discrepancies between them. We drew data from the EDEN mother-child birth cohort, gathered at two time points: 5.5 and 11.5 years of age. We examined three instruments assessing psychopathology: the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, the Child Behavior Checklist, and the </span>Mental Health and Social Inadaptation Assessment for Adolescents. We focused on four distinct scales: internalizing disorder, conduct disorder, social problems, and ADHD symptoms.</p><p>Our analyses first examined correlations between Full-scale IQ, Verbal IQ, Performance IQ, and psychopathology scores. Subsequently, we explored correlations between absolute and relative verbal-performance IQ discrepancies and psychopathology scores. In general, we found that relations between intelligence index scores and psychopathology scales were generally null or negative (high IQ associated with fewer psychopathology symptoms). Our results do not lend support to the hypothesis that high intelligence or index score discrepancies are risk factors for psychopathology in children and adolescents.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101753"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44675383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Gene-environment interplay in early life cognitive development 早期认知发展中的基因-环境相互作用
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101748
Sophie von Stumm , Radhika Kandaswamy , Jessye Maxwell

Children's differences in early life cognitive development are driven by the interplay of genetic and environmental factors, but identifying replicable gene-environment interactions (GxE) has proven difficult. We systematically tested GxE effects in the prediction of cognitive development from 2 to 4 years, using polygenic scores (PGS) for years spent in education and 39 measures of the home and neighborhood environment. Data came from up to 6973 unrelated individuals from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a UK population-representative cohort. The environmental measures accounted together for 20.6% of the variance in cognitive development, while the PGS accounted for 0.5% (p < .001). We observed substantial gene-environment correlations but found no conclusive evidence for GxE effects. While associations between PGS and cognitive development were weak, genetic and environmental factors had direct and additive (i.e., main effects) rather than interactive influences on early life cognitive development.

儿童早期认知发展的差异是由遗传和环境因素的相互作用驱动的,但确定可复制的基因-环境相互作用(GxE)已被证明是困难的。我们系统地测试了GxE在预测2 - 4岁认知发展中的作用,使用了教育年限的多基因分数(PGS)和家庭和社区环境的39种测量方法。数据来自双胞胎早期发展研究(TEDS)中多达6973名不相关的个体,这是一个英国人口代表性队列。环境措施合计占认知发展差异的20.6%,而PGS只占0.5% (p <措施)。我们观察到大量的基因与环境相关,但没有发现GxE效应的确凿证据。虽然PGS与认知发展之间的关联较弱,但遗传和环境因素对早期生活认知发展的影响是直接和附加的(即主要影响),而不是相互作用的。
{"title":"Gene-environment interplay in early life cognitive development","authors":"Sophie von Stumm ,&nbsp;Radhika Kandaswamy ,&nbsp;Jessye Maxwell","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101748","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101748","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children's differences in early life cognitive development are driven by the interplay of genetic and environmental factors, but identifying replicable gene-environment interactions (GxE) has proven difficult. We systematically tested GxE effects in the prediction of cognitive development from 2 to 4 years, using polygenic scores (PGS) for years spent in education and 39 measures of the home and neighborhood environment. Data came from up to 6973 unrelated individuals from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a UK population-representative cohort. The environmental measures accounted together for 20.6% of the variance in cognitive development, while the PGS accounted for 0.5% (<em>p</em> &lt; .001). We observed substantial gene-environment correlations but found no conclusive evidence for GxE effects. While associations between PGS and cognitive development were weak, genetic and environmental factors had direct and additive (i.e., main effects) rather than interactive influences on early life cognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101748"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45119359","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cross-cultural generalizability of cognitive ability measures: A systematic literature review. 认知能力测量的跨文化概括性:系统的文献综述。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101751
Christopher J. Wilson , Stephen C. Bowden , Linda K. Byrne , Nicole R. Joshua , Wolfgang Marx , Lawrence G. Weiss

Examining factorial invariance provides the strongest test of the generalizability of psychological constructs across populations and should be investigated prior to cross-cultural interpretation of cognitive assessments. The aim of this systematic review was to critically evaluate the current evidence regarding the factorial invariance and the generalizability of cognition models across cultures. The review was structured using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. The literature search identified 57 original studies examining the factorial invariance of cognitive ability assessments across cultures. The results were strongly supportive of the cross-cultural generalizability of the underlying cognitive model. Ten studies found configural invariance, 20 studies found weak or partial weak factorial invariance, 12 found strong or partial strong factorial invariance, and 13 found strict factorial invariance. However, the quality of the factorial invariance analyses varied between studies, with some analyses not adopting the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance analysis, leading to ambiguous results. No study that provided interpretable results in terms of the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance found a lack of factorial invariance. Overall, the results of this review suggest that i) the factor analytic models of cognitive abilities generalize across cultures, ii) the use of the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance is likely to find strong or strict factorial invariance, iii) the results are compatible with well-established Cattell-Horn-Carroll constructs being invariant across cultures. Future research into factorial invariance should follow the hierarchical analytic approach so as not to misestimate factorial invariance. Studies should also use the Cattell-Horn-Carroll taxonomy to systematize intelligence research.

检查析因不变性是对心理构念在人群中的普遍性的最有力的检验,应该在认知评估的跨文化解释之前进行调查。本系统综述的目的是批判性地评估关于跨文化认知模型的析因不变性和概括性的现有证据。本综述采用系统评价和荟萃分析首选报告项目(PRISMA)指南进行组织。文献检索确定了57项原始研究,这些研究考察了不同文化间认知能力评估的因子不变性。研究结果强烈支持了潜在认知模型的跨文化概括性。10项研究发现构型不变性,20项研究发现弱或部分弱因子不变性,12项研究发现强或部分强因子不变性,13项研究发现严格因子不变性。然而,各研究的析因不变性分析的质量各不相同,有些分析没有采用层次分析法进行析因不变性分析,导致结果不明确。在阶乘不变性的层次方法方面,没有研究提供可解释的结果,发现缺乏阶乘不变性。总的来说,这篇综述的结果表明,i)认知能力的因素分析模型在不同文化中都是通用的,ii)使用分层方法来分析因子不变性可能会发现强的或严格的因子不变性,iii)结果与已经建立的cattel - horn - carroll结构在不同文化中都是不变的相一致。未来对析因不变性的研究应遵循层次分析法,以免误读析因不变性。研究还应该使用卡特尔-霍恩-卡罗尔分类法来系统化智力研究。
{"title":"The cross-cultural generalizability of cognitive ability measures: A systematic literature review.","authors":"Christopher J. Wilson ,&nbsp;Stephen C. Bowden ,&nbsp;Linda K. Byrne ,&nbsp;Nicole R. Joshua ,&nbsp;Wolfgang Marx ,&nbsp;Lawrence G. Weiss","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101751","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101751","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Examining factorial invariance provides the strongest test of the generalizability of psychological constructs across populations and should be investigated prior to cross-cultural interpretation of cognitive assessments. The aim of this systematic review was to critically evaluate the current evidence regarding the factorial invariance and the generalizability of cognition models across cultures. The review was structured using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. The literature search identified 57 original studies examining the factorial invariance of cognitive ability assessments across cultures. The results were strongly supportive of the cross-cultural generalizability of the underlying cognitive model. Ten studies found configural invariance, 20 studies found weak or partial weak factorial invariance, 12 found strong or partial strong factorial invariance, and 13 found strict factorial invariance. However, the quality of the factorial invariance analyses varied between studies, with some analyses not adopting the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance analysis, leading to ambiguous results. No study that provided interpretable results in terms of the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance found a lack of factorial invariance. Overall, the results of this review suggest that i) the factor analytic models of cognitive abilities generalize across cultures, ii) the use of the hierarchical approach to factorial invariance is likely to find strong or strict factorial invariance, iii) the results are compatible with well-established Cattell-Horn-Carroll constructs being invariant across cultures. Future research into factorial invariance should follow the hierarchical analytic approach so as not to misestimate factorial invariance. Studies should also use the Cattell-Horn-Carroll taxonomy to systematize intelligence research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101751"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43241142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Bilingualism and intelligence in children exposed to poverty environments: A Raven's error pattern analysis using a generalized propensity score method 贫困环境下儿童的双语能力与智力:基于广义倾向评分法的雷文错误模式分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101758
Leopoldo Laborda , Juan Mejalenko , Isabel Gómez-Veiga

The main objective of this study is to compare the quantitative (correct answers) and qualitative (error types) performance of children belonging to different linguistic groups on a non-verbal reasoning test, Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices Test, after being matched based on level of exposure to poverty, certain individual characteristics and test performance. The sample is representative for Mexico at a population level and consists of children aged 5 to 12 (n = 4644), of which 671 are bilingual in Spanish and an indigenous language, 3970 are monolingual in Spanish and 78 are monolingual in an indigenous language. The results show significant quantitative differences with a lower overall performance in the Raven's test by bilingual children as compared to their monolingual (Spanish only) peers, but no qualitative differences when analyzing their error types. When considering each linguistic group individually, the relative frequency of three error types (Repetition, Wrong Principle, and Incomplete Correlate) is similar in children aged 5 to 8 and in those aged 9 to 12. However, considering the two age cohorts, the results reveal how the intragroup differences in each linguistic group, are only statistically significant in the case of Difference errors, in the group of monolingual children in Spanish.

In addition to practical use that may be potentially derived from this empirical evidence, these results may also be encouraging from a methodological point of view. They demonstrate how the method used, in addition to permitting greater comparison between the experimental groups of a representative sample at a population level, does not present high sensitivity, either for the model used to estimate the Generalized Propensity Score method, or for the specific estimator used.

本研究的主要目的是比较属于不同语言群体的儿童在非语言推理测试(Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices test)中的定量(正确答案)和定性(错误类型)表现,这些儿童是根据贫困程度、某些个人特征和测试表现进行匹配的。样本在人口水平上代表了墨西哥,由5至12岁的儿童组成(n = 4644),其中671人会说西班牙语和一种土著语言的双语,3970人会说一种西班牙语,78人会说一种土著语言。结果显示,双语儿童与单语儿童(仅西班牙语)相比,在Raven’s测试中的总体表现较低,这在数量上存在显著差异,但在分析他们的错误类型时,没有质的差异。当单独考虑每个语言群体时,5至8岁儿童和9至12岁儿童的三种错误类型(重复,错误原则和不完全相关)的相对频率相似。然而,考虑到两个年龄队列,结果揭示了各组语言的组内差异如何仅在西班牙语单语儿童组中出现差异错误的情况下才具有统计学意义。除了可能从这些经验证据中获得的实际用途之外,从方法论的角度来看,这些结果也可能令人鼓舞。他们证明,除了允许在总体水平上对代表性样本的实验组进行更大的比较外,该方法如何使用,无论是用于估计广义倾向评分方法的模型,还是用于使用的特定估计器,都没有表现出高灵敏度。
{"title":"Bilingualism and intelligence in children exposed to poverty environments: A Raven's error pattern analysis using a generalized propensity score method","authors":"Leopoldo Laborda ,&nbsp;Juan Mejalenko ,&nbsp;Isabel Gómez-Veiga","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101758","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101758","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The main objective of this study is to compare the quantitative (correct answers) and qualitative (error types) performance of children belonging to different linguistic groups on a non-verbal reasoning test, Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices Test, after being matched based on level of exposure to poverty, certain individual characteristics and test performance. The sample is representative for Mexico at a population level and consists of children aged 5 to 12 (<em>n</em> = 4644), of which 671 are bilingual in Spanish and an indigenous language, 3970 are monolingual in Spanish and 78 are monolingual in an indigenous language. The results show significant quantitative differences with a lower overall performance in the Raven's test by bilingual children as compared to their monolingual (Spanish only) peers, but no qualitative differences when analyzing their error types. When considering each linguistic group individually, the relative frequency of three error types (Repetition, Wrong Principle, and Incomplete Correlate) is similar in children aged 5 to 8 and in those aged 9 to 12. However, considering the two age cohorts, the results reveal how the intragroup differences in each linguistic group, are only statistically significant in the case of Difference errors, in the group of monolingual children in Spanish.</p><p>In addition to practical use that may be potentially derived from this empirical evidence, these results may also be encouraging from a methodological point of view. They demonstrate how the method used, in addition to permitting greater comparison between the experimental groups of a representative sample at a population level, does not present high sensitivity, either for the model used to estimate the Generalized Propensity Score method, or for the specific estimator used.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101758"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43539961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1