首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
Seventy years, 1000 samples, and 300,000 SPM scores: A new meta-analysis of Flynn effect patterns 70年,1000个样本,30万个SPM分数:弗林效应模式的新荟萃分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101750
Peera Wongupparaj , Rangsirat Wongupparaj , Robin G. Morris , Veena Kumari

Several studies have investigated and found the gradual rise in IQ over time or the Flynn effect (FE) but inconsistent results on the FEs across types of countries and age groups were reported. The current cross-temporal meta-analysis aimed to examine the temporal correlations between mean IQ scores from Standard Progressive Matrices and year of publication, moderated by types of countries and age groups covering seven decades (1948–2020). The given relationships were weighted by sample sizes. The dataset included 1038 independent samples (N = 299,155) from 72 countries. The results generally supported the FE with the IQ gain of 0.22 points per year, but the magnitudes of the IQ gains depended on types of countries and age groups. Stronger FEs were evident in middle-income countries and younger generations. A multicausal explanatory framework should be utilized to explain the underlying mechanism of the secular IQ gains across factors.

一些研究已经调查并发现智商随着时间的推移逐渐上升或弗林效应(FE),但在不同类型的国家和年龄组中,FEs的结果不一致。当前的跨时间荟萃分析旨在检查标准进步矩阵的平均智商得分与出版年份之间的时间相关性,并根据70年(1948-2020)的国家和年龄组类型进行调节。给定的关系由样本量加权。该数据集包括来自72个国家的1038个独立样本(N = 299155)。结果显示,平均每年的智商提高了0.22分,总体上支持FE,但智商提高的幅度取决于国家和年龄组的类型。中等收入国家和年轻一代的FEs明显更强。应采用多因素解释框架来解释各因素间长期智商提高的潜在机制。
{"title":"Seventy years, 1000 samples, and 300,000 SPM scores: A new meta-analysis of Flynn effect patterns","authors":"Peera Wongupparaj ,&nbsp;Rangsirat Wongupparaj ,&nbsp;Robin G. Morris ,&nbsp;Veena Kumari","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101750","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101750","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Several studies have investigated and found the gradual rise in IQ over time or the Flynn effect (FE) but inconsistent results on the FEs across types of countries and age groups were reported. The current cross-temporal meta-analysis aimed to examine the temporal correlations between mean IQ scores from Standard Progressive Matrices and year of publication, moderated by types of countries and age groups covering seven decades (1948–2020). The given relationships were weighted by sample sizes. The dataset included 1038 independent samples (</span><em>N</em> = 299,155) from 72 countries. The results generally supported the FE with the IQ gain of 0.22 points per year, but the magnitudes of the IQ gains depended on types of countries and age groups. Stronger FEs were evident in middle-income countries and younger generations. A multicausal explanatory framework should be utilized to explain the underlying mechanism of the secular IQ gains across factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42226191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
(Not just) Intelligence stratifies the occupational hierarchy: Ranking 360 professions by IQ and non-cognitive traits (不仅如此)智力对职业等级进行分层:根据智商和非认知特征对360个职业进行排名
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101755
Tobias Wolfram

Occupational sorting, the process of individuals actively selecting into and being selected for different occupations, has significant implications for social stratification and inequality. The psychometric view of occupational differentials in ability emphasizes the importance of intelligence for occupational sorting, as it acts as a necessary condition to enter and remain in certain professions due to their high cognitive demand. The resulting cognitive stratification of the occupational hierarchy leads to strong associations between occupational mean IQ and sociological measures of occupational status and pay. Past research has been criticized for lack of representativeness and small sample sizes. In this study, we both confirm the psychometric view in a large representative sample and extend it to a set of nine non-cognitive traits. We show that the psychometric view holds (on a weaker level) for multiple non-cognitive traits, and using small-area estimation, we provide precise mean estimates and rankings of intelligence and non-cognitive traits for 360 occupations, including rare professions. Keywords: Social Stratification, Occupation, Non-Cognitive Traits.

职业分类是个体主动选择进入和被选择不同职业的过程,对社会分层和不平等具有重要意义。职业能力差异的心理测量学观点强调了智力对职业分类的重要性,因为由于某些职业的高认知需求,智力是进入和留在该行业的必要条件。由此产生的职业等级的认知分层导致职业平均智商与职业地位和薪酬的社会学测量之间的强烈关联。过去的研究因缺乏代表性和样本量小而受到批评。在这项研究中,我们在一个大的代表性样本中证实了心理测量学的观点,并将其扩展到一组九种非认知特征。我们的研究表明,心理测量学观点(在较弱的水平上)适用于多种非认知特征,并使用小区域估计,我们提供了360种职业(包括稀有职业)的智力和非认知特征的精确均值估计和排名。关键词:社会分层,职业,非认知特质
{"title":"(Not just) Intelligence stratifies the occupational hierarchy: Ranking 360 professions by IQ and non-cognitive traits","authors":"Tobias Wolfram","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101755","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101755","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Occupational sorting, the process of individuals actively selecting into and being selected for different occupations, has significant implications for social stratification and inequality. The psychometric view of occupational differentials in ability emphasizes the importance of intelligence for occupational sorting, as it acts as a necessary condition to enter and remain in certain professions due to their high cognitive demand. The resulting cognitive stratification of the occupational hierarchy leads to strong associations between occupational mean IQ and sociological measures of occupational status and pay. Past research has been criticized for lack of representativeness and small sample sizes. In this study, we both confirm the psychometric view in a large representative sample and extend it to a set of nine non-cognitive traits. We show that the psychometric view holds (on a weaker level) for multiple non-cognitive traits, and using small-area estimation, we provide precise mean estimates and rankings of intelligence and non-cognitive traits for 360 occupations, including rare professions. Keywords: Social Stratification, Occupation, Non-Cognitive Traits.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46766406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looking for Flynn effects in a recent online U.S. adult sample: Examining shifts within the SAPA Project 在最近的美国成年人在线样本中寻找弗林效应:研究SAPA项目中的变化
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101734
Elizabeth M. Dworak , William Revelle , David M. Condon

Compared to European countries, research is limited regarding if the Flynn effect, or its reversal, is a current phenomenon in the United States. Though recent research on the United States suggests that a Flynn effect could still be present, or partially present, among child and adolescent samples, few studies have explored differences of cognitive ability scores among US adults. Thirteen years of cross-sectional data from a subsample of adults (n = 394,378) were obtained from the Synthetic Aperture Personality Assessment Project (SAPA Project) to examine if cognitive ability scores changed within the United States from 2006 to 2018. Responses to an overlapping set of 35 (collected 2006–2018) and 60 (collected 2011–2018) items from the open-source multiple choice intelligence assessment International Cognitive Ability Resource (ICAR) were used to examine the trends in standardized average composite cognitive ability scores and domain scores of matrix reasoning, letter and number series, verbal reasoning, and three-dimensional rotation. Composite ability scores from 35 items and domain scores (matrix reasoning; letter and number series) showed a pattern consistent with a reversed Flynn effect from 2006 to 2018 when stratified across age, education, or gender. Slopes for verbal reasoning scores, however, failed to meet or exceed an annual threshold of |0.02| SD. A reversed Flynn effect was also present from 2011 to 2018 for composite ability scores from 60 items across age, education, and gender. Despite declining scores across age and demographics in other domains of cognitive ability, three-dimensional rotation scores showed evidence of a Flynn effect with the largest slopes occurring across age stratified regressions.

与欧洲国家相比,关于弗林效应或其逆转是否在美国是一种当前现象的研究有限。尽管最近对美国的研究表明,在儿童和青少年样本中,弗林效应可能仍然存在,或部分存在,但很少有研究探讨美国成年人认知能力得分的差异。从合成孔径人格评估项目(SAPA项目)中获得了来自成人亚样本(n = 394,378)的13年横断面数据,以检查2006年至2018年美国境内的认知能力评分是否发生了变化。本研究利用国际认知能力资源(ICAR)中35项(收集于2006-2018年)和60项(收集于2011-2018年)的重叠答题,考察了矩阵推理、字母和数字序列、言语推理和三维旋转的标准化平均复合认知能力得分和领域得分的变化趋势。35项综合能力得分和领域得分(矩阵推理;字母和数字序列)显示出与2006年至2018年的反弗林效应一致的模式,即按年龄、教育程度或性别分层。然而,言语推理分数的斜率未能达到或超过年度阈值|0.02| SD。从2011年到2018年,不同年龄、教育程度和性别的60个项目的综合能力得分也出现了相反的弗林效应。尽管在其他认知能力领域的得分在年龄和人口统计学上有所下降,但三维旋转得分显示出弗林效应的证据,在年龄分层回归中出现最大的斜率。
{"title":"Looking for Flynn effects in a recent online U.S. adult sample: Examining shifts within the SAPA Project","authors":"Elizabeth M. Dworak ,&nbsp;William Revelle ,&nbsp;David M. Condon","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101734","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101734","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Compared to European countries, research is limited regarding if the Flynn effect, or its reversal, is a current phenomenon in the United States. Though recent research on the United States suggests that a Flynn effect could still be present, or partially present, among child and adolescent samples, few studies have explored differences of cognitive ability scores among US adults. Thirteen years of cross-sectional data from a subsample of adults (<em>n</em> = 394,378) were obtained from the Synthetic Aperture Personality Assessment Project (SAPA Project) to examine if cognitive ability scores changed within the United States from 2006 to 2018. Responses to an overlapping set of 35 (collected 2006–2018) and 60 (collected 2011–2018) items from the open-source multiple choice intelligence assessment International Cognitive Ability Resource (ICAR) were used to examine the trends in standardized average composite cognitive ability scores and domain scores of matrix reasoning, letter and number series, verbal reasoning, and three-dimensional rotation. Composite ability scores from 35 items and domain scores (matrix reasoning; letter and number series) showed a pattern consistent with a reversed Flynn effect from 2006 to 2018 when stratified across age, education, or gender. Slopes for verbal reasoning scores, however, failed to meet or exceed an annual threshold of |0.02| SD. A reversed Flynn effect was also present from 2011 to 2018 for composite ability scores from 60 items across age, education, and gender. Despite declining scores across age and demographics in other domains of cognitive ability, three-dimensional rotation scores showed evidence of a Flynn effect with the largest slopes occurring across age stratified regressions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44036341","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
A synthetic theory to integrate and explain the causes of the Flynn effect: The Parental Executive Model 一个综合理论来整合和解释弗林效应的原因:父母-高管模型
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101740
Joseph Lee Rodgers, Patrick O'Keefe

The Flynn effect is one of the most interesting puzzles in psychology. Measured intelligence increased worldwide for a century, primarily in the fluid domain. Some increases may be flattening and/or reversing. Around 20 theories have been developed to explain the Flynn effect; most have empirical or conceptual weaknesses, though they also have strengths and face validity. The role of parents has been given only slight attention in past theorizing. We propose a new synthetic theory, the Parental Executive Model (the PEM) that integrates, organizes, and takes advantage of most previous theories. To structure our paper, we begin with a description of synthetic theories, and how to evaluate them (which has unique and difficult challenges). Following, we review the Flynn effect, and briefly summarize the new theory. Next, recent research is reviewed suggesting the value of re-conceptualizing the location of the Flynn effect to focus on families, and parents. Following, we describe the PEM, which states that parents – and teachers, authors, friends, etc. – use their agency to manage children's intellectual growth. The PEM posits that many (not all) parents subjectively optimize their children's intellectual outcomes, taking advantage of some subset of available resources – nutrition, education, technology, health care, etc. Cross-generational momentum occurs as parenting improves across generations. Prior research motivating the PEM is reviewed, and methods to evaluate the PEM are discussed. Finally, one recent empirical evaluation of the PEM is reviewed, that the cognitive home environment has been improving over time.

弗林效应是心理学中最有趣的谜题之一。一个世纪以来,测量智力在全球范围内不断增长,主要是在流体领域。一些增长可能趋于平缓和/或逆转。人们已经发展出大约20种理论来解释弗林效应;大多数都有经验或概念上的弱点,尽管它们也有优势和有效性。在过去的理论中,父母的作用很少受到重视。我们提出了一个新的综合理论,即父母执行模型(PEM),它整合、组织并利用了大多数先前的理论。为了构建我们的论文,我们首先描述了综合理论,以及如何评估它们(这具有独特和困难的挑战)。接下来,我们回顾了弗林效应,并简要总结了这一新理论。接下来,回顾了最近的研究,表明重新定义弗林效应的位置以关注家庭和父母的价值。接下来,我们将描述PEM,它指出父母——以及老师、作者、朋友等——使用他们的代理来管理孩子的智力成长。PEM假设许多(不是所有)父母主观地优化他们孩子的智力结果,利用一些可用资源的子集-营养,教育,技术,医疗保健等。随着育儿方式在几代人之间的改善,代际动力就会出现。综述了国内外对PEM的研究进展,并对PEM的评价方法进行了讨论。最后,一个最近的实证评估的PEM审查,认知家庭环境已经改善随着时间的推移。
{"title":"A synthetic theory to integrate and explain the causes of the Flynn effect: The Parental Executive Model","authors":"Joseph Lee Rodgers,&nbsp;Patrick O'Keefe","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101740","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101740","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>The Flynn effect is one of the most interesting puzzles in psychology. Measured intelligence increased worldwide for a century, primarily in the fluid domain. Some increases may be flattening and/or reversing. Around 20 theories have been developed to explain the Flynn effect; most have empirical or conceptual weaknesses, though they also have strengths and </span>face validity<span>. The role of parents has been given only slight attention in past theorizing. We propose a new synthetic theory, the Parental Executive Model (the PEM) that integrates, organizes, and takes advantage of most previous theories. To structure our paper, we begin with a description of synthetic theories, and how to evaluate them (which has unique and difficult challenges). Following, we review the Flynn effect, and briefly summarize the new theory. Next, recent research is reviewed suggesting the value of re-conceptualizing the location of the Flynn effect to focus on families, and parents. Following, we describe the PEM, which states that parents – and teachers, authors, friends, etc. – use their agency to manage children's intellectual growth. The PEM posits that many (not all) parents subjectively optimize their children's intellectual outcomes, taking advantage of some subset of available resources – nutrition, education, technology, health care, etc. Cross-generational momentum occurs as parenting improves across generations. Prior research motivating the PEM is reviewed, and methods to evaluate the PEM are discussed. Finally, one recent empirical evaluation of the PEM is reviewed, that the cognitive home environment has been improving over time.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49529395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The relationship of divergent thinking with broad retrieval ability and processing speed: A meta-analysis 发散思维与广义检索能力和加工速度的关系:一项元分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101739
Kirill G. Miroshnik , Boris Forthmann , Maciej Karwowski , Mathias Benedek

The present study aimed to integrate evidence on the relationship among broad retrieval ability (Gr), processing speed (Gs), and divergent thinking (DT) with a three-level meta-analytic approach. The analysis was conducted on 560 effect sizes obtained from 47 studies with an overall sample of 10,391 participants. Results indicated moderate mean correlations for both the Gr–DT (r = 0.47, 95% CI: [0.38, 0.54]) and the Gs–DT relationship (r = 0.31, 95% CI: [0.20, 0.41]). Notably, the correlation between DT and Gr was significantly higher than between DT and Gs, and the former remained significant even after controlling for the Gr–Gs correlation (r = 0.35, 95% CI: [0.26, 0.44]). Moderation analyses revealed that the Gr–DT link was moderated by the modality of DT tests and type of DT indicator, whereas the Gs–DT link was moderated by the modality of DT tests and type of DT instruction. Overall, these findings support the claim on the essential role of broad retrieval ability and processing speed in creative idea production.

本研究旨在运用三层次元分析方法,整合广义检索能力(Gr)、处理速度(Gs)与发散思维(DT)之间的关系。这项分析是从47项研究中获得的560个效应值进行的,总共有10391名参与者。结果显示,Gr-DT (r = 0.47, 95% CI:[0.38, 0.54])和Gs-DT (r = 0.31, 95% CI:[0.20, 0.41])的平均相关性中等。值得注意的是,DT与Gr之间的相关性显著高于DT与Gs之间的相关性,即使在控制了Gr - Gs相关性后,DT与Gr之间的相关性仍然显著(r = 0.35, 95% CI:[0.26, 0.44])。调节性分析发现,Gr-DT联系受DT测验形式和DT指标类型的调节,而Gs-DT联系受DT测验形式和DT教学类型的调节。总的来说,这些发现支持了广义检索能力和处理速度在创意想法产生中的重要作用。
{"title":"The relationship of divergent thinking with broad retrieval ability and processing speed: A meta-analysis","authors":"Kirill G. Miroshnik ,&nbsp;Boris Forthmann ,&nbsp;Maciej Karwowski ,&nbsp;Mathias Benedek","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101739","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101739","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>The present study aimed to integrate evidence on the relationship among broad retrieval ability (Gr), processing speed (Gs), and divergent thinking (DT) with a three-level meta-analytic approach. The analysis was conducted on 560 effect sizes obtained from 47 studies with an overall sample of 10,391 participants. Results indicated moderate mean correlations for both the Gr–DT (</span><em>r</em> = 0.47, 95% CI: [0.38, 0.54]) and the Gs–DT relationship (<em>r</em> = 0.31, 95% CI: [0.20, 0.41]). Notably, the correlation between DT and Gr was significantly higher than between DT and Gs, and the former remained significant even after controlling for the Gr–Gs correlation (<em>r</em><span> = 0.35, 95% CI: [0.26, 0.44]). Moderation analyses revealed that the Gr–DT link was moderated by the modality of DT tests and type of DT indicator, whereas the Gs–DT link was moderated by the modality of DT tests and type of DT instruction. Overall, these findings support the claim on the essential role of broad retrieval ability and processing speed in creative idea production.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46787631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Maternal supportiveness is predictive of childhood general intelligence 母亲的支持可以预测儿童的一般智力
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101754
Curtis S. Dunkel , Dimitri van der Linden , Tetsuya Kawamoto

Data from the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project (N = 1075) were used to test the hypothesis that maternal supportiveness (measured at three waves from 14 to 36 months) is positively and prospectively associated with a child's general intelligence (measured at five waves from 14 months to 10 years). Bivariate correlations showed that maternal supportiveness was consistently and positively associated with a child's general intelligence. For example, maternal supportiveness as measured at 14 months was correlated with a child's general intelligence at age 10; r = 0.35. Results of autoregressive cross-lagged panel models showed maternal supportiveness directly predicted future general intelligence through age four and indirectly, via age four general intelligence, up to age 10. Additional analyses verified that the effect of maternal supportiveness was on general intelligence and not specific abilities. The results point to the importance of maternal supportiveness on general intelligence in the first decade of life.

来自早期开端研究和评估项目(N = 1075)的数据被用来检验母亲的支持(从14个月到36个月的三波测量)与孩子的一般智力(从14个月到10岁的五波测量)呈正相关的假设。双变量相关性表明,母亲的支持与孩子的一般智力始终呈正相关。例如,14个月大时测量的母亲支持与孩子10岁时的一般智力相关;r = 0.35。自回归交叉滞后面板模型的结果显示,母亲的支持直接预测到4岁的孩子未来的一般智力,并通过4岁的一般智力间接预测到10岁。另外的分析证实,母亲的支持对一般智力的影响,而不是特定的能力。研究结果表明,在孩子出生后的头十年里,母亲的支持对孩子的一般智力有重要影响。
{"title":"Maternal supportiveness is predictive of childhood general intelligence","authors":"Curtis S. Dunkel ,&nbsp;Dimitri van der Linden ,&nbsp;Tetsuya Kawamoto","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101754","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101754","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Data from the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project (<em>N</em> = 1075) were used to test the hypothesis that maternal supportiveness (measured at three waves from 14 to 36 months) is positively and prospectively associated with a child's general intelligence (measured at five waves from 14 months to 10 years). Bivariate correlations showed that maternal supportiveness was consistently and positively associated with a child's general intelligence. For example, maternal supportiveness as measured at 14 months was correlated with a child's general intelligence at age 10; <em>r</em> = 0.35. Results of autoregressive cross-lagged panel models showed maternal supportiveness directly predicted future general intelligence through age four and indirectly, via age four general intelligence, up to age 10. Additional analyses verified that the effect of maternal supportiveness was on general intelligence and not specific abilities. The results point to the importance of maternal supportiveness on general intelligence in the first decade of life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45529857","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On group differences in the heritability of intelligence: A reply to Giangrande and Turkheimer (2022) 智力遗传力的群体差异:对Giangrande和Turkheimer(2022)的回答
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101737
Bryan J. Pesta , Jan te Nijenhuis , Jordan Lasker , Emil O.W. Kirkegaard , John G.R. Fuerst

Here we reply to Giangrande and Turkheimer's (2022; G&T) recent critique of a meta-analysis we published in Intelligence regarding the Scarr-Rowe Hypothesis and the apparent lack of putative race/ethnic group differences in the heritability of intelligence (Pesta et al., 2020). Our rebuttal is divided into three sections that address ubiquitous misstatements in their critique: Section 1 focuses on conceptual and theoretical points. Section 2 addresses methodological, statistical, and interpretative points. Section 3 provides new analyses suggested by G&T that support our original interpretations. We note that G&T published their critique in Perspectives on Psychological Science (PoPS), which did not invite us to respond before their paper was published and our subsequent submission of a rebuttal was not accepted. Our unsuccessful appeal of these events based on possible ethics violations is detailed here (Appendix E). We recognize that this is a controversial area of research with legitimate disagreements and hope our responses maintain a degree of rigor and professionalism that others can emulate.

在此,我们对詹格兰德和图克海默(2022;G&T)最近对我们发表在《情报》上的关于Scarr-Rowe假设的荟萃分析的批评,以及智力遗传能力明显缺乏假定的种族/民族差异(Pesta et al., 2020)。我们的反驳分为三个部分,以解决他们批评中普遍存在的错误陈述:第1部分侧重于概念和理论要点。第2节讨论了方法、统计和解释要点。第3节提供了G&T提出的支持我们原始解释的新分析。我们注意到,G&T在《心理科学展望》(PoPS)上发表了他们的批评,在他们的论文发表之前没有邀请我们回应,我们随后提交的反驳也没有被接受。基于可能违反道德规范的原因,我们对这些事件的失败申诉详见此处(附录E)。我们认识到,这是一个有争议的研究领域,存在合理的分歧,希望我们的回应保持一定程度的严谨性和专业性,以使其他人可以效仿。
{"title":"On group differences in the heritability of intelligence: A reply to Giangrande and Turkheimer (2022)","authors":"Bryan J. Pesta ,&nbsp;Jan te Nijenhuis ,&nbsp;Jordan Lasker ,&nbsp;Emil O.W. Kirkegaard ,&nbsp;John G.R. Fuerst","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101737","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101737","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Here we reply to Giangrande and Turkheimer's (2022; G&amp;T) recent critique of a meta-analysis we published in <em>Intelligence</em><span> regarding the Scarr-Rowe Hypothesis and the apparent lack of putative race/ethnic group differences in the heritability of intelligence (Pesta et al., 2020). Our rebuttal is divided into three sections that address ubiquitous misstatements in their critique: Section 1 focuses on conceptual and theoretical points. Section 2 addresses methodological, statistical, and interpretative points. Section 3 provides new analyses suggested by G&amp;T that support our original interpretations. We note that G&amp;T published their critique in </span><em>Perspectives on Psychological Science</em> (PoPS), which did not invite us to respond before their paper was published and our subsequent submission of a rebuttal was not accepted. Our unsuccessful appeal of these events based on possible ethics violations is detailed here (Appendix E). We recognize that this is a controversial area of research with legitimate disagreements and hope our responses maintain a degree of rigor and professionalism that others can emulate.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46335231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood: A meta-analysis 成年晚期的智力和预期寿命:一项荟萃分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738
Macarena Sánchez-Izquierdo , Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros , Elizabeth Lucía Valeriano-Lorenzo , Juan Botella

In an aging society, it is crucial to understand why some people live long and others do not. There has been a proliferation of studies in recent years that highlight the importance of psycho-behavioural factors in the ways of aging, one of those psychological components is intelligence. In this meta-analysis, the association between intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood is analysed through the Hazard Ratio (HR). Our objectives are: (i) to update Calvin's meta-analysis, especially the estimate of the association between survival and intelligence; and (ii) to evaluate the role of some moderators, especially the age of the participants, to explore intelligence–mortality throughout adulthood and old age. The results show a positive relationship between intelligence and survival (HR: 0.79; 95% CI: 0.81–0.76). This association is significantly moderated by the years of follow-up, the effect size being smaller the more years elapse between the intelligence assessment and the recording of the outcome. Intelligence is a protective factor to reach middle-high age, but from then on survival depends less and less on intelligence and more on other factors.

在老龄化社会中,理解为什么有些人长寿而有些人不长寿是至关重要的。近年来,越来越多的研究强调了心理行为因素在衰老过程中的重要性,其中一个心理因素就是智力。在本荟萃分析中,通过风险比(HR)分析了成年后期智力与预期寿命之间的关系。我们的目标是:(i)更新Calvin的元分析,特别是对生存和智力之间关系的估计;(ii)评估一些调节因素的作用,特别是参与者的年龄,以探索整个成年期和老年期的智力死亡率。结果表明,智力与生存呈正相关(HR: 0.79;95% ci: 0.81-0.76)。这种关联被随访年份显著地缓和了,在智力评估和结果记录之间的时间间隔越长,效果越小。智力是达到中老年的保护因素,但从那时起,生存对智力的依赖越来越少,而更多地依赖于其他因素。
{"title":"Intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood: A meta-analysis","authors":"Macarena Sánchez-Izquierdo ,&nbsp;Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros ,&nbsp;Elizabeth Lucía Valeriano-Lorenzo ,&nbsp;Juan Botella","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In an aging society, it is crucial to understand why some people live long and others do not. There has been a proliferation of studies in recent years that highlight the importance of psycho-behavioural factors in the ways of aging, one of those psychological components is intelligence. In this meta-analysis, the association between intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood is analysed through the Hazard Ratio (HR). Our objectives are: (i) to update Calvin's meta-analysis, especially the estimate of the association between survival and intelligence; and (ii) to evaluate the role of some moderators, especially the age of the participants, to explore intelligence–mortality throughout adulthood and old age. The results show a positive relationship between intelligence and survival (HR<sub>•</sub>: 0.79; 95% CI: 0.81–0.76). This association is significantly moderated by the years of follow-up, the effect size being smaller the more years elapse between the intelligence assessment and the recording of the outcome. Intelligence is a protective factor to reach middle-high age, but from then on survival depends less and less on intelligence and more on other factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49373951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Further arguments that ability tilt correlations are spurious: A reply to Coyle (2022) 关于能力倾斜相关性的进一步论证是错误的:对科伊尔的回复(2022)
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706
Kimmo Sorjonen , Michael Ingre , Gustav Nilsonne , Bo Melin

Ability tilt refers to a within-individual difference between two abilities, e.g. a difference between math and verbal ability. Coyle and colleagues have demonstrated correlations between ability tilts and measures of the constituent abilities. We have previously pointed out that such measures may be spurious as the tilt variable is dependent on the constituent abilities. We have further shown that reported tilt associations are inconsistent with simulations including non-spurious tilt-effects, and concluded that tilt-correlations demonstrated by Coyle and colleagues are spurious. In a recent paper, Coyle responded with a series of arguments, including that the validity of tilt correlations is supported by their agreement with theoretical predictions, and that the analyses we used in our previous critique (regression effects) differ from tilt-correlations. Here, we advance the discussion by responding to the arguments put forward by Coyle. We show that the difference between regression effects and correlations is not material to the validity of our argument. Furthermore, we discuss the relation of tilt correlations to theory, and show that many empirical tilt-correlations, e.g. between the birth rate – death rate difference and fertility in US states, can be observed although such correlations can hardly be explained by differential investment theories. Therefore, we maintain that tilt correlations are spurious and that they offer little support for theories concerning the development of intelligence.

能力倾斜指的是两种能力之间的个体内部差异,例如数学和语言能力之间的差异。科伊尔和他的同事已经证明了能力倾斜度和构成能力的测量之间的相关性。我们以前曾指出,这些措施可能是虚假的,因为倾斜变量取决于组成能力。我们进一步表明,报告的倾斜关联与模拟不一致,包括非虚假倾斜效应,并得出结论,Coyle及其同事所证明的倾斜相关性是虚假的。在最近的一篇论文中,Coyle用一系列的论点来回应,包括倾斜相关性的有效性得到了它们与理论预测一致的支持,以及我们在之前的批评中使用的分析(回归效应)不同于倾斜相关性。在这里,我们通过回应科伊尔提出的论点来推进讨论。我们表明,回归效应和相关性之间的差异对我们论证的有效性并不重要。此外,我们讨论了倾斜相关性与理论的关系,并表明可以观察到许多经验倾斜相关性,例如美国各州的出生率-死亡率差异与生育率之间的相关性,尽管这种相关性很难用差分投资理论来解释。因此,我们认为倾斜相关性是虚假的,它们对智力发展的理论提供了很少的支持。
{"title":"Further arguments that ability tilt correlations are spurious: A reply to Coyle (2022)","authors":"Kimmo Sorjonen ,&nbsp;Michael Ingre ,&nbsp;Gustav Nilsonne ,&nbsp;Bo Melin","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Ability tilt refers to a within-individual difference between two abilities, e.g. a difference between math and verbal ability. Coyle and colleagues have demonstrated correlations between ability tilts and measures of the constituent abilities. We have previously pointed out that such measures may be spurious as the tilt variable is dependent on the constituent abilities. We have further shown that reported tilt associations are inconsistent with simulations including non-spurious tilt-effects, and concluded that tilt-correlations demonstrated by Coyle and colleagues are spurious. In a recent paper, Coyle responded with a series of arguments, including that the validity of tilt correlations is supported by their agreement with theoretical predictions, and that the analyses we used in our previous critique (regression effects) differ from tilt-correlations. Here, we advance the discussion by responding to the arguments put forward by Coyle. We show that the difference between regression effects and correlations is not material to the validity of our argument. Furthermore, we discuss the relation of tilt correlations to theory, and show that many empirical tilt-correlations, e.g. between the birth rate – death rate difference and fertility in US states, can be observed although such correlations can hardly be explained by differential investment theories. Therefore, we maintain that tilt correlations are spurious and that they offer little support for theories concerning the development of intelligence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42936370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
General intelligence in middle school students from different Russian regions: Results of PISA-like tests 俄罗斯不同地区中学生的一般智力:类似pisa测试的结果
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756
Nikita Kolachev , Galina Kovaleva

This study is aimed at investigating the contribution of the general intelligence factor if six PISA domains (reading, mathematical, scientific, financial literacies, global competence, and creative thinking) are combined in one measurement instrument. For achieving our goal, items based on the PISA frameworks are developed, students in grades 5–8 from three different Russian regions are assessed, and three IRT models (unidimensional, multidimensional, and bifactor) are applied to process the data. In addition, the correlations from the multidimensional model are estimated to examine the degree of cognitive specificity and mixture modeling is implemented to investigate ability differentiation across grades. Statistical analysis reveals that the bifactor model comprising one general and six specific factors, has a better fit in each grade. Based on this model, we compute the variance explained by the general factor, with the estimates varying between 60% and 70%. In general, the pure variance explained by specific factors does not exceed 10%. The correlations are above 0.40 in each grade and the averaged associations tend to increase from 6th to 8th grade, although they are smaller in years 6 and 7 compared to year 5. The general ability differentiation effect is observed in grades 6 to 8 and is not present in grade 5. Specific ability differentiation is more pronounced in reading literacy, especially in grade 5 to 7. The results obtained are discussed from the perspective of the ability and developmental differentiation/dedifferentiation problem.

本研究旨在探讨如果将六个PISA领域(阅读、数学、科学、金融素养、全球能力和创造性思维)结合在一个测量工具中,一般智力因素的贡献。为了实现我们的目标,我们开发了基于PISA框架的项目,对来自俄罗斯三个不同地区的5-8年级学生进行了评估,并应用了三个IRT模型(一维、多维和双因素)来处理数据。此外,通过估算多维模型的相关性来检验认知特异性程度,并采用混合模型来研究年级间的能力差异。统计分析表明,由1个一般因子和6个特殊因子组成的双因子模型在各等级均具有较好的拟合效果。基于该模型,我们计算了由一般因素解释的方差,估计值在60%到70%之间变化。一般来说,由特定因素解释的纯方差不超过10%。各年级的相关系数均在0.40以上,从6年级到8年级,平均相关性有增加的趋势,尽管6年级和7年级的相关性比5年级小。一般的能力分化效应在6至8年级观察到,而在5年级不存在。特异能力分化在阅读素养中更为明显,特别是在5 - 7年级。从能力和发育分化/去分化问题的角度对所得结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"General intelligence in middle school students from different Russian regions: Results of PISA-like tests","authors":"Nikita Kolachev ,&nbsp;Galina Kovaleva","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study is aimed at investigating the contribution of the general intelligence factor if six PISA domains (reading, mathematical, scientific, financial literacies, global competence, and creative thinking) are combined in one measurement instrument. For achieving our goal, items based on the PISA frameworks are developed, students in grades 5–8 from three different Russian regions are assessed, and three IRT models (unidimensional, multidimensional, and bifactor) are applied to process the data. In addition, the correlations from the multidimensional model are estimated to examine the degree of cognitive specificity and mixture modeling is implemented to investigate ability differentiation across grades. Statistical analysis reveals that the bifactor model comprising one general and six specific factors, has a better fit in each grade. Based on this model, we compute the variance explained by the general factor, with the estimates varying between 60% and 70%. In general, the pure variance explained by specific factors does not exceed 10%. The correlations are above 0.40 in each grade and the averaged associations tend to increase from 6th to 8th grade, although they are smaller in years 6 and 7 compared to year 5. The general ability differentiation effect is observed in grades 6 to 8 and is not present in grade 5. Specific ability differentiation is more pronounced in reading literacy, especially in grade 5 to 7. The results obtained are discussed from the perspective of the ability and developmental differentiation/dedifferentiation problem.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47614191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1