首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Impairments of attention in RRMS patients: the role of disease duration. RRMS 患者的注意力障碍:病程的作用。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-27 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421
Devrim Kalkan, Murat Kurt

Introduction: The extent to which different types of attention are affected in RRMS based on disease duration has not been extensively analyzed. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine whether MS patients differ compared to healthy individuals in a homogeneous sample of RRMS patients in terms of attention types and from which year of MS attention deficit starts. Another aim of the study was to examine the effect of MS duration and stimulus onset asynchrony on dual task performance.

Methods: The sample consisted of RRMS patients (n = 53) and healthy participants (n = 30) between the ages of 20-49, who were at least primary school graduates. Healthy participants in the comparison group were reached by snowball sampling technique. Stroop Test, Cancellation Test, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, Coding Test, WMS-R Digit Span and Visual Memory Span subtests were administered to assess attention. Divided attention performance was assessed with a dual task developed based on psychological refractory period paradigm.

Results: The results show that there is a significant difference between RRMS patients and healthy participants in terms of different types of attention (p < 0.05). Focused, sustained and divided attention of RRMS patients and the ability to resist interference showed a significant decline from the 7th year of the disease (p < 0.05); no significant difference was found between healthy participants and patients with 1-6 years of RRMS.

Conclusions: Although the results of the study are consistent with the literature which show that attention deficit develops in MS, it is important in terms of showing that attention deficit changes depending on the duration of the disease. Focused attention, sustained attention, interference resistance and divided attention performance of RRMS patients showed a significant decline after the 7th year of the disease.

导言:关于 RRMS 患者不同类型的注意力受疾病持续时间影响的程度,尚未进行广泛的分析。因此,本研究旨在确定在 RRMS 患者的同质样本中,多发性硬化症患者与健康人相比在注意类型上是否存在差异,以及多发性硬化症患者的注意缺陷是从哪一年开始的。研究的另一个目的是考察多发性硬化症病程和刺激开始不同步对双重任务表现的影响:样本包括 RRMS 患者(53 人)和健康参与者(30 人),年龄在 20-49 岁之间,至少小学毕业。对比组中的健康参与者是通过滚雪球抽样技术获得的。研究人员进行了 Stroop 测试、取消测试、步调听觉连续加法测试、编码测试、WMS-R 数字跨度和视觉记忆跨度子测试,以评估注意力。在心理折射期范式的基础上开发的双重任务评估了注意力分散的表现:结果表明,RRMS 患者和健康参与者在不同类型的注意力方面存在显著差异(p p 结论:虽然研究结果与健康参与者的结果一致,但RRMS 患者和健康参与者在不同类型的注意力方面存在显著差异:尽管研究结果与文献中关于多发性硬化症患者会出现注意力缺陷的观点一致,但重要的是,研究结果表明,注意力缺陷会随着病程的延长而发生变化。RRMS 患者的集中注意力、持续注意力、抗干扰能力和分心能力在病程第 7 年后显著下降。
{"title":"Impairments of attention in RRMS patients: the role of disease duration.","authors":"Devrim Kalkan, Murat Kurt","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>The extent to which different types of attention are affected in RRMS based on disease duration has not been extensively analyzed. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine whether MS patients differ compared to healthy individuals in a homogeneous sample of RRMS patients in terms of attention types and from which year of MS attention deficit starts. Another aim of the study was to examine the effect of MS duration and stimulus onset asynchrony on dual task performance.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The sample consisted of RRMS patients (<i>n</i> = 53) and healthy participants (<i>n</i> = 30) between the ages of 20-49, who were at least primary school graduates. Healthy participants in the comparison group were reached by snowball sampling technique. Stroop Test, Cancellation Test, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, Coding Test, WMS-R Digit Span and Visual Memory Span subtests were administered to assess attention. Divided attention performance was assessed with a dual task developed based on psychological refractory period paradigm.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The results show that there is a significant difference between RRMS patients and healthy participants in terms of different types of attention (<i>p</i> < 0.05). Focused, sustained and divided attention of RRMS patients and the ability to resist interference showed a significant decline from the 7th year of the disease (<i>p</i> < 0.05); no significant difference was found between healthy participants and patients with 1-6 years of RRMS.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Although the results of the study are consistent with the literature which show that attention deficit develops in MS, it is important in terms of showing that attention deficit changes depending on the duration of the disease. Focused attention, sustained attention, interference resistance and divided attention performance of RRMS patients showed a significant decline after the 7th year of the disease.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"891-912"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142728967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A tale of two constructs: confirmatory factor analysis of performance and symptom validity tests. 两个结构的故事:绩效和症状有效性测试的确认性因素分析。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-10 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004
Michael R Basso, Savanna M Tierney, Brad L Roper, Douglas M Whiteside, Dennis R Combs, Eduardo Estevis

Background: Performance validity (PV) and symptom validity (SV) tests assess biased responding that impact scores on neuropsychological tests. The extent to which PV and SV represent overlapping or unique constructs remains incompletely defined, especially among psychiatric patients in a non-forensic setting. The current study investigated this question using confirmatory factor analysis.

Method: Eighty-two inpatients with mood disorders were administered the Word Memory Test, and its primary indices formed a latent variable of PV. From the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 the Fake Bad Scale (FBS), Response Bias Scale (RBS), and Henry-Heilbronner Index (HHI) were employed as a latent SV variable. Two models of the relationship between PV and SV were compared. One freely estimated the shared variance between SV and PV latent constructs. The other assumed the relationship between SV and PV was homogeneous, and covariance was fixed to 1.0.

Results: In the freely estimated model, covariance between PV and SV was -0.18, and model fit was excellent (CFI = 0.098; TLI = 0.096; SRMR = 0.08). For the fixed model, the RBS, HHI, and FBS achieved low loadings on the SV construct, and model fit was poor (CFI = 0.66; TLI = 0.43; SRMR = 0.42).

Conclusions: PV as indexed by the WMT and SV measured by the MMPI-2 are not overlapping constructs among inpatients with mood disorders. These data imply that PV and SV represent distinct constructs in this population. Implications for practice are discussed.

背景:表现效度(PV)和症状效度(SV)测试评估的是影响神经心理测试得分的偏差反应。表现效度和症状效度在多大程度上代表重叠或独特的结构,目前仍未完全确定,尤其是在非法医环境下的精神病患者中。本研究采用确证因子分析法对这一问题进行了研究:方法:对 82 名患有情绪障碍的住院病人进行了单词记忆测试,其主要指标构成了 PV 的潜变量。明尼苏达多相人格量表-2》中的假恶丑量表(FBS)、反应偏差量表(RBS)和亨利-海尔布隆纳指数(HHI)被用作潜在 SV 变量。比较了 PV 和 SV 之间关系的两个模型。一种是自由估计 SV 和 PV 潜在结构之间的共享方差。另一种假设 SV 和 PV 之间的关系是同质的,协方差被固定为 1.0:在自由估计模型中,PV 和 SV 之间的协方差为-0.18,模型拟合度非常好(CFI = 0.098;TLI = 0.096;SRMR = 0.08)。在固定模型中,RBS、HHI 和 FBS 在 SV 结构上的载荷较低,模型拟合度较差(CFI = 0.66;TLI = 0.43;SRMR = 0.42):结论:在患有情绪障碍的住院患者中,用 WMT 表示的 PV 和用 MMPI-2 测量的 SV 并不是重叠的结构。这些数据表明,在这一人群中,PV 和 SV 代表了不同的结构。本文讨论了对实践的影响。
{"title":"A tale of two constructs: confirmatory factor analysis of performance and symptom validity tests.","authors":"Michael R Basso, Savanna M Tierney, Brad L Roper, Douglas M Whiteside, Dennis R Combs, Eduardo Estevis","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Performance validity (PV) and symptom validity (SV) tests assess biased responding that impact scores on neuropsychological tests. The extent to which PV and SV represent overlapping or unique constructs remains incompletely defined, especially among psychiatric patients in a non-forensic setting. The current study investigated this question using confirmatory factor analysis.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Eighty-two inpatients with mood disorders were administered the Word Memory Test, and its primary indices formed a latent variable of PV. From the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 the Fake Bad Scale (FBS), Response Bias Scale (RBS), and Henry-Heilbronner Index (HHI) were employed as a latent SV variable. Two models of the relationship between PV and SV were compared. One freely estimated the shared variance between SV and PV latent constructs. The other assumed the relationship between SV and PV was homogeneous, and covariance was fixed to 1.0.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In the freely estimated model, covariance between PV and SV was -0.18, and model fit was excellent (CFI = 0.098; TLI = 0.096; SRMR = 0.08). For the fixed model, the RBS, HHI, and FBS achieved low loadings on the SV construct, and model fit was poor (CFI = 0.66; TLI = 0.43; SRMR = 0.42).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PV as indexed by the WMT and SV measured by the MMPI-2 are not overlapping constructs among inpatients with mood disorders. These data imply that PV and SV represent distinct constructs in this population. Implications for practice are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"840-847"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neuropsychology of social cognition: culture, display rules, and emotional expressivity. 社会认知神经心理学:文化、展示规则和情感表达。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-23 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2428728
Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull

Introduction: We investigated the roles of group ethnicity and display rules of emotions in the neuropsychology of social cognition in Asian American and White participants recruited from a majority-minority college campus.

Method: 128 participants (mean age = 24.9 years) completed: 1) Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), which includes separate measures of affect naming of facial expressions and emotional prosody interpretation of audio statements; 2) Display Rule Assessment Inventory (DRAI), a self-report measure of emotional expressivity across four settings (family, close friends, colleagues, and strangers) and in two distinct domains (should/actual) that asks participants what they believe people should do (social value) and what they would actually do (behavioral self-report).

Results: ACS-SP revealed evidence of cultural bias, as reflected by group ethnicity differences, for recognition of emotional prosody but not emotional facial expressions for Asian American versus White participants. The DRAI showed significant cultural differences only for family relationships with White participants endorsing stronger belief in the social value of expressing negative emotions of sadness, aversion, and fear. These AC-SP and DRAI group differences remained significant when covarying for spoken English language, as measured by an oral word reading test. Hierarchical regression results indicated that group ethnicity and family display rules each made specific and significant contributions to neuropsychological performance but did so in very different and distinct ways. Group ethnicity exerted its greatest effect on prosody interpretation whereas family display rules had its most pronounced influence on affect naming.

Conclusions: The current results may help inform and advance culturally responsive neuropsychological models of social cognition.

简介:我们研究了亚裔美国人和白人参与者的群体种族和情绪显示规则在社会认知神经心理学中的作用:我们研究了在少数族裔占多数的大学校园中招募的亚裔美国人和白人参与者的社会认知神经心理学中群体种族和情绪显示规则的作用:128名参与者(平均年龄为24.9岁)完成了以下测试:方法:128名参与者(平均年龄=24.9岁)完成了:1)高级临床解决方案-社会认知(ACS-SP),其中包括对面部表情的情绪命名和音频语句的情绪亲和力解释的单独测量;2)显示规则评估量表(DRAI),这是一种在四个环境(家庭、亲密朋友、同事和陌生人)和两个不同领域(应该/实际)中对情绪表达能力的自我报告测量,询问参与者他们认为人们应该做什么(社会价值)和他们实际上会做什么(行为自我报告):ACS-SP显示了文化偏差的证据,反映在亚裔美国人与白人参与者之间的群体种族差异上,亚裔美国人与白人参与者在识别情绪拟声词方面存在文化偏差,但在识别情绪面部表情方面不存在文化偏差。DRAI 仅在家庭关系方面显示出明显的文化差异,白人参与者更相信表达悲伤、厌恶和恐惧等负面情绪的社会价值。通过口语单词阅读测试对英语口语进行协整后,AC-SP 和 DRAI 的群体差异仍然显著。分层回归结果表明,群体种族和家庭显示规则对神经心理学表现都有具体而显著的影响,但影响方式却截然不同。群体种族对拟声词解释的影响最大,而家庭显示规则对情感命名的影响最明显:目前的研究结果可能有助于为社会认知的文化响应神经心理学模型提供信息并推动其发展。
{"title":"Neuropsychology of social cognition: culture, display rules, and emotional expressivity.","authors":"Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2428728","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2428728","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>We investigated the roles of group ethnicity and display rules of emotions in the neuropsychology of social cognition in Asian American and White participants recruited from a majority-minority college campus.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>128 participants (mean age = 24.9 years) completed: 1) Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), which includes separate measures of affect naming of facial expressions and emotional prosody interpretation of audio statements; 2) Display Rule Assessment Inventory (DRAI), a self-report measure of emotional expressivity across four settings (family, close friends, colleagues, and strangers) and in two distinct domains (should/actual) that asks participants what they believe people should do (social value) and what they would actually do (behavioral self-report).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>ACS-SP revealed evidence of cultural bias, as reflected by group ethnicity differences, for recognition of emotional prosody but not emotional facial expressions for Asian American versus White participants. The DRAI showed significant cultural differences only for family relationships with White participants endorsing stronger belief in the social value of expressing negative emotions of sadness, aversion, and fear. These AC-SP and DRAI group differences remained significant when covarying for spoken English language, as measured by an oral word reading test. Hierarchical regression results indicated that group ethnicity and family display rules each made specific and significant contributions to neuropsychological performance but did so in very different and distinct ways. Group ethnicity exerted its greatest effect on prosody interpretation whereas family display rules had its most pronounced influence on affect naming.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The current results may help inform and advance culturally responsive neuropsychological models of social cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"811-827"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142695535","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological correlates of the good old days bias in mild traumatic brain injury. 轻度脑外伤患者 "过去的美好时光 "偏见的心理相关性。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2426622
Daniel L Schlehofer, Julie A Suhr

Non-neurological factors such as the "expectation as etiology" or the "good old days" bias (EE/GOD bias) may partially explain persistent symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI). What is less clear from existing research is the degree to which EE/GOD bias is related to other psychological correlates of persistent post-concussive symptoms (PPCS). We examined whether the EE/GOD bias was related to illness perception beliefs, intolerance of uncertainty, suggestibility, and domain identification. Participants with MTBI history and without (controls) reported frequency and severity of current PPCS; the MTBI group additionally reported premorbid PPCS. Participants also completed measures of psychological factors potentially associated with PPCS. Consistent with previous studies of the EE/GOD bias, the MTBI group endorsed less premorbid PPCS than current PPCS and when compared to the current symptom report of the control group. The MTBI group also endorsed more current PPCS than the control group. Higher EE/GOD bias was associated with several aspects of illness identity, including belief that symptoms would be more chronic, greater illness-related psychological distress, and greater cogniphobia. Higher EE/GOD bias was also related to higher intolerance of uncertainty and stronger personal identification with memory abilities. Regression showed that perceived symptom timeline, cogniphobia, and domain identification were unique predictors of EE/GOD bias. Findings confirm that the EE/GOD bias is seen in individuals with self-reported history of MTBI and corresponds to other psychological processes that potentially explain ongoing MTBI symptoms, providing greater insight into the potential mechanisms of PPCS. Future studies should examine the EE/GOD bias and associated psychological correlates in a clinical population and also assess for potential neuropsychological correlates. Findings suggest that psychological factors and premorbid symptom report should be considered in clinical assessment and also suggest potential mechanisms of treatment of individuals with acute MTBI or prolonged MTBI symptoms.

非神经学因素,如 "期望作为病因 "或 "过去的美好时光 "偏差(EE/GOD 偏差),可以部分解释轻度脑外伤(MTBI)后的持续症状。现有研究不太清楚的是,EE/GOD 偏差在多大程度上与持续性脑震荡后症状(PPCS)的其他心理相关因素有关。我们研究了 EE/GOD 偏差是否与疾病感知信念、不确定性不容忍、暗示性和领域识别有关。有 MTBI 史和无 MTBI 史的参与者(对照组)报告了当前 PPCS 的频率和严重程度;MTBI 组还报告了病前 PPCS。参与者还完成了可能与 PPCS 相关的心理因素的测量。与之前的 EE/GOD 偏差研究一致,MTBI 组认可的病前 PPCS 比当前 PPCS 少,与对照组的当前症状报告相比也是如此。与对照组相比,MTBI 组对当前 PPCS 的认可度也更高。较高的 EE/GOD 偏差与疾病认同的几个方面有关,包括认为症状会更加慢性化、与疾病相关的心理压力更大以及认知恐惧症更严重。较高的 EE/GOD 偏差还与较高的不确定性不容忍度和较强的个人记忆能力认同有关。回归结果显示,感知症状时间表、认知恐惧症和领域认同是预测 EE/GOD 偏差的独特因素。研究结果证实,EE/GOD偏差出现在自述有MTBI病史的人身上,并与其他可能解释持续MTBI症状的心理过程相对应,从而为PPCS的潜在机制提供了更深入的见解。未来的研究应在临床人群中检验 EE/GOD 偏差和相关的心理相关因素,并评估潜在的神经心理学相关因素。研究结果表明,在临床评估中应考虑心理因素和病前症状报告,并提出了治疗急性 MTBI 或长期 MTBI 症状患者的潜在机制。
{"title":"Psychological correlates of the good old days bias in mild traumatic brain injury.","authors":"Daniel L Schlehofer, Julie A Suhr","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2426622","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2426622","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Non-neurological factors such as the \"expectation as etiology\" or the \"good old days\" bias (EE/GOD bias) may partially explain persistent symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI). What is less clear from existing research is the degree to which EE/GOD bias is related to other psychological correlates of persistent post-concussive symptoms (PPCS). We examined whether the EE/GOD bias was related to illness perception beliefs, intolerance of uncertainty, suggestibility, and domain identification. Participants with MTBI history and without (controls) reported frequency and severity of current PPCS; the MTBI group additionally reported premorbid PPCS. Participants also completed measures of psychological factors potentially associated with PPCS. Consistent with previous studies of the EE/GOD bias, the MTBI group endorsed less premorbid PPCS than current PPCS and when compared to the current symptom report of the control group. The MTBI group also endorsed more current PPCS than the control group. Higher EE/GOD bias was associated with several aspects of illness identity, including belief that symptoms would be more chronic, greater illness-related psychological distress, and greater cogniphobia. Higher EE/GOD bias was also related to higher intolerance of uncertainty and stronger personal identification with memory abilities. Regression showed that perceived symptom timeline, cogniphobia, and domain identification were unique predictors of EE/GOD bias. Findings confirm that the EE/GOD bias is seen in individuals with self-reported history of MTBI and corresponds to other psychological processes that potentially explain ongoing MTBI symptoms, providing greater insight into the potential mechanisms of PPCS. Future studies should examine the EE/GOD bias and associated psychological correlates in a clinical population and also assess for potential neuropsychological correlates. Findings suggest that psychological factors and premorbid symptom report should be considered in clinical assessment and also suggest potential mechanisms of treatment of individuals with acute MTBI or prolonged MTBI symptoms.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"828-839"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social norms in Korsakoff's syndrome and alcohol-related dementia. 科萨科夫综合症和酒精相关痴呆症的社会规范。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-17 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2426826
Robin Boere, Erik Oudman, Albert Postma, Esther van den Berg

Objective: Social cognition is essential for individuals to perceive, process, and interpret social information that enables them to function effectively in society. Korsakoff's syndrome (KS) and alcohol-related dementia (ARD) are alcohol-related cognitive disorders that are likely to impair social cognition. This study aimed to investigate the ability to judge social norms in KS and ARD in comparison to healthy subjects.

Method: The study included 30 patients with KS, 10 patients with ARD, and 74 age-, sex-, and education-matched control participants. The Social Norms Questionnaire - Dutch version (SNQ-NL) was used to measure social cognition, and standardized tests were used to examine the association between social cognition and executive functioning.

Results: Both KS and ARD patients performed worse in judging social norms when compared to healthy controls, but there was no significant difference between KS and ARD. Both KS and ARD patients performed worse on the SNQ-NL, judging appropriate behavior as inappropriate (Overadhere errors), than controls. When compared to control participants, patients with KS demonstrated significantly more Break Errors, while the differences between the ARD group and the control group did not reach statistical significance. There were no significant correlations between the SNQ-NL variables and performances on standardized tests for executive functioning in both the KS and control group.

Conclusions: This study aimed to investigate the ability to judge social norms in KS and ARD in comparison to healthy subjects. The results show that compared to a control group, both KS and ARD patients performed worse in judging social norms. KS and ARD patients scored equally low on social norms identification. Overall, these findings can further help us understand the difficulties in social behavior as experienced by patients and care staff, since problems in accurately judging social norms could possibly contribute to the severe behavioral issues as observed in alcohol-related disorders.

目的:社会认知对个人感知、处理和解释社会信息至关重要,它能使个人在社会中有效发挥作用。科萨科夫综合征(KS)和酒精相关性痴呆(ARD)是与酒精相关的认知障碍,很可能会损害社会认知能力。本研究旨在调查 KS 和 ARD 与健康受试者相比对社会规范的判断能力:研究对象包括 30 名 KS 患者、10 名 ARD 患者和 74 名年龄、性别和教育程度匹配的对照组参与者。社会规范问卷--荷兰语版(SNQ-NL)用于测量社会认知,标准化测试用于研究社会认知与执行功能之间的关联:结果:与健康对照组相比,KS和ARD患者在判断社会规范方面的表现都较差,但KS和ARD之间没有显著差异。与对照组相比,KS和ARD患者在SNQ-NL中将适当行为判定为不适当行为(过度错误)的表现均较差。与对照组相比,KS 患者的 "突破错误 "明显增多,而 ARD 组与对照组之间的差异未达到统计学意义上的显著性。在KS组和对照组中,SNQ-NL变量与执行功能标准化测试成绩之间没有明显的相关性:本研究旨在调查 KS 和 ARD 与健康受试者相比对社会规范的判断能力。结果显示,与对照组相比,KS 和 ARD 患者在判断社会规范方面的表现都较差。KS 和 ARD 患者在社会规范识别方面的得分同样较低。总之,这些研究结果可以进一步帮助我们了解患者和护理人员在社交行为方面遇到的困难,因为准确判断社会规范的问题可能是酒精相关障碍患者出现严重行为问题的原因之一。
{"title":"Social norms in Korsakoff's syndrome and alcohol-related dementia.","authors":"Robin Boere, Erik Oudman, Albert Postma, Esther van den Berg","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2426826","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2426826","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Social cognition is essential for individuals to perceive, process, and interpret social information that enables them to function effectively in society. Korsakoff's syndrome (KS) and alcohol-related dementia (ARD) are alcohol-related cognitive disorders that are likely to impair social cognition. This study aimed to investigate the ability to judge social norms in KS and ARD in comparison to healthy subjects.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The study included 30 patients with KS, 10 patients with ARD, and 74 age-, sex-, and education-matched control participants. The Social Norms Questionnaire - Dutch version (SNQ-NL) was used to measure social cognition, and standardized tests were used to examine the association between social cognition and executive functioning.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Both KS and ARD patients performed worse in judging social norms when compared to healthy controls, but there was no significant difference between KS and ARD. Both KS and ARD patients performed worse on the SNQ-NL, judging appropriate behavior as inappropriate (Overadhere errors), than controls. When compared to control participants, patients with KS demonstrated significantly more Break Errors, while the differences between the ARD group and the control group did not reach statistical significance. There were no significant correlations between the SNQ-NL variables and performances on standardized tests for executive functioning in both the KS and control group.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study aimed to investigate the ability to judge social norms in KS and ARD in comparison to healthy subjects. The results show that compared to a control group, both KS and ARD patients performed worse in judging social norms. KS and ARD patients scored equally low on social norms identification. Overall, these findings can further help us understand the difficulties in social behavior as experienced by patients and care staff, since problems in accurately judging social norms could possibly contribute to the severe behavioral issues as observed in alcohol-related disorders.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"868-877"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142643725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Subjective time dilation in abstinent patients with alcohol use disorder. 戒酒障碍患者的主观时间膨胀。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2427320
Jing Wu, Yu Liu, Xiangjuan Kong, Dapeng Zhang, Wei Hao, Zheng Ye

Introduction: Patients with alcohol use disorder (AUD) may have distortions in time perception. This study investigated subjective time dilation (the tendency to perceive a time interval longer than it is) and its association with craving and impulsivity in AUD.

Method: Thirty abstinent male inpatients with AUD (age 29-60 years) and thirty sex-, age-, and education-matched healthy controls completed a temporal generalization task, which assessed the preference (point of subjective equality, PSE) and sensitivity of time perception in the second range. Craving for alcohol was assessed using the Alcohol Urge Questionnaire. Impulsivity was assessed using a delay discounting task and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11. A comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests was used to measure executive function (flanker task, symbol digit modalities test, trail-making test-A/B), negative emotionality (Beck Depression Inventory-II, Self-rating Anxiety Scale), and incentive salience (monetary incentive delay task) following the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment (ANA) framework.

Results: AUD patients exhibited a smaller PSE than healthy controls, perceiving a time interval 8% longer than it was. AUD patients with a smaller PSE showed a greater craving for alcohol but not greater impulsivity. Exploratory factor analysis incorporating the PSE and ANA measures revealed four latent factors. The PSE loaded highly onto a factor reflecting time perception but not three other factors reflecting executive function, negative emotionality, and incentive salience.

Conclusions: AUD patients exhibit a pathological form of subjective time dilation, which is associated with a greater craving for alcohol. Time perception may be an independent functional dimension for understanding addictive behaviors in AUD.

导言:酒精使用障碍(AUD)患者可能会出现时间知觉失真。本研究调查了主观时间扩张(感知时间间隔比实际时间长的倾向)及其与 AUD 患者渴求和冲动的关系:方法:30 名戒酒的男性 AUD 住院患者(29-60 岁)和 30 名性别、年龄和教育程度相匹配的健康对照者完成了一项时间概括任务,该任务评估了第二区间时间感知的偏好(主观相等点,PSE)和敏感性。对酒精的渴望程度通过酒精冲动问卷进行评估。冲动性通过延迟折现任务和 Barratt 冲动量表-11 进行评估。根据成瘾性神经临床评估(ANA)框架,采用了一整套神经心理学测试来测量执行功能(侧翼任务、符号数字模型测试、追踪测试-A/B)、负性情绪(贝克抑郁量表-II、焦虑自评量表)和激励显著性(货币激励延迟任务):结果:与健康对照组相比,AUD 患者的 PSE 更小,他们认为时间间隔比实际时间间隔长 8%。PSE 较小的 AUD 患者对酒精的渴望程度更高,但冲动程度却不高。结合 PSE 和 ANA 测量进行的探索性因子分析发现了四个潜在因子。PSE与一个反映时间感知的因子高度相关,但与其他三个反映执行功能、负面情绪性和激励显著性的因子无关:结论:AUD 患者表现出一种病态的主观时间膨胀,这与他们对酒精的更大渴望有关。时间知觉可能是了解 AUD 患者成瘾行为的一个独立功能维度。
{"title":"Subjective time dilation in abstinent patients with alcohol use disorder.","authors":"Jing Wu, Yu Liu, Xiangjuan Kong, Dapeng Zhang, Wei Hao, Zheng Ye","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427320","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Patients with alcohol use disorder (AUD) may have distortions in time perception. This study investigated subjective time dilation (the tendency to perceive a time interval longer than it is) and its association with craving and impulsivity in AUD.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty abstinent male inpatients with AUD (age 29-60 years) and thirty sex-, age-, and education-matched healthy controls completed a temporal generalization task, which assessed the preference (point of subjective equality, PSE) and sensitivity of time perception in the second range. Craving for alcohol was assessed using the Alcohol Urge Questionnaire. Impulsivity was assessed using a delay discounting task and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11. A comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests was used to measure executive function (flanker task, symbol digit modalities test, trail-making test-A/B), negative emotionality (Beck Depression Inventory-II, Self-rating Anxiety Scale), and incentive salience (monetary incentive delay task) following the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment (ANA) framework.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>AUD patients exhibited a smaller PSE than healthy controls, perceiving a time interval 8% longer than it was. AUD patients with a smaller PSE showed a greater craving for alcohol but not greater impulsivity. Exploratory factor analysis incorporating the PSE and ANA measures revealed four latent factors. The PSE loaded highly onto a factor reflecting time perception but not three other factors reflecting executive function, negative emotionality, and incentive salience.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>AUD patients exhibit a pathological form of subjective time dilation, which is associated with a greater craving for alcohol. Time perception may be an independent functional dimension for understanding addictive behaviors in AUD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"878-890"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142647703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How have neck muscle vibration effects on visuospatial behavior and spatial neglect been explored? A scoping review. 如何研究颈部肌肉振动对视觉空间行为和空间忽视的影响?范围综述。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-28 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2432663
Noémie C Duclos, Eric Sorita, Frédérique Poncet, Cyril Duclos, Karim Jamal

Introduction: Neck muscle vibration (NMV) has been proposed as a bottom-up intervention to enhance visuospatial exploration in post-stroke patients with unilateral spatial neglect (USN). While some studies report enlarged visual exploration during NMV application, others find no significant impact, highlighting inconsistencies in the literature. The diversity in NMV application methods and the variation in visuospatial outcome measures may contribute to these conflicting findings. This study aimed to overview the methodological approaches used to investigate NMV's effects on visuospatial behavior in USN patients, focusing on aspects beyond sample size and study design.

Methods: Among the seven databases, studies that applied NMV and assessed visual or perceptual outcomes were included in the analysis. Two independent reviewers screened titles and abstracts to select the studies to analyze. Data about the publication, population, modalities of application, and outcomes were extracted and synthesized.

Results: Twenty-five publications from 1988 to 2023 were included, involving a total of 512 participants, of whom 114 (22.3%) had USN. The presence of USN was assessed based on paper-and-pencil tests. The frequency of NMV was set between 80 and 125 hz. In 22 studies, NMV were applied on the left side of the neck muscles. Six studies proposed multiple NMV sessions, lasting between 5 and 50 minutes/day, 3-5 times/week, for 2-4 weeks. One study included a follow-up period of up to 1.4 years. The tasks during NMV often involved indicating the subjective straight ahead (SSA, n = 8), pointing out targets, or no specific activity (n = 7, each). The SSA and cancellation tests were the most frequent outcomes.

Conclusions: NMV application modalities varied widely across studies, with only vibration frequency showing consistency. The tasks performed during NMV and the outcome measures were diverse and generally unrelated to activities of daily living. Therefore, NMV effects during more ecologically valid tasks should be explored.

简介:颈部肌肉振动(NMV颈部肌肉振动(NMV)被认为是一种自下而上的干预方法,可增强中风后单侧空间忽略(USN)患者的视觉空间探索能力。一些研究报告称,在应用 NMV 时,视觉探索能力有所增强,而另一些研究则认为没有显著影响,这凸显了文献中的不一致性。NMV应用方法的多样性和视觉空间结果测量方法的差异可能是造成这些相互矛盾的研究结果的原因。本研究旨在概述用于研究 NMV 对 USN 患者视觉空间行为影响的方法,重点关注样本大小和研究设计以外的方面:在七个数据库中,应用 NMV 并评估视觉或知觉结果的研究均被纳入分析范围。两名独立审稿人对标题和摘要进行筛选,选出需要分析的研究。提取并综合了有关出版物、人群、应用方式和结果的数据:共纳入了 1988 年至 2023 年间的 25 篇出版物,涉及 512 名参与者,其中 114 人(22.3%)患有 USN。根据纸笔测试评估是否存在 USN。NMV 频率设定在 80 到 125 赫兹之间。在 22 项研究中,对颈部左侧肌肉施加了 NMV。六项研究建议进行多次 NMV 治疗,每天 5 至 50 分钟,每周 3 至 5 次,持续 2 至 4 周。其中一项研究的随访期长达 1.4 年。NMV 期间的任务通常包括指示主观正前方(SSA,n = 8)、指出目标或无特定活动(n = 7,每项)。SSA和取消测试是最常见的结果:结论:不同研究中的 NMV 应用模式差异很大,只有振动频率显示出一致性。在非挤压振动过程中执行的任务和结果测量多种多样,一般与日常生活活动无关。因此,应探索非机械振动对更多生态有效任务的影响。
{"title":"How have neck muscle vibration effects on visuospatial behavior and spatial neglect been explored? A scoping review.","authors":"Noémie C Duclos, Eric Sorita, Frédérique Poncet, Cyril Duclos, Karim Jamal","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2432663","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2432663","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Neck muscle vibration (NMV) has been proposed as a bottom-up intervention to enhance visuospatial exploration in post-stroke patients with unilateral spatial neglect (USN). While some studies report enlarged visual exploration during NMV application, others find no significant impact, highlighting inconsistencies in the literature. The diversity in NMV application methods and the variation in visuospatial outcome measures may contribute to these conflicting findings. This study aimed to overview the methodological approaches used to investigate NMV's effects on visuospatial behavior in USN patients, focusing on aspects beyond sample size and study design.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Among the seven databases, studies that applied NMV and assessed visual or perceptual outcomes were included in the analysis. Two independent reviewers screened titles and abstracts to select the studies to analyze. Data about the publication, population, modalities of application, and outcomes were extracted and synthesized.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Twenty-five publications from 1988 to 2023 were included, involving a total of 512 participants, of whom 114 (22.3%) had USN. The presence of USN was assessed based on paper-and-pencil tests. The frequency of NMV was set between 80 and 125 hz. In 22 studies, NMV were applied on the left side of the neck muscles. Six studies proposed multiple NMV sessions, lasting between 5 and 50 minutes/day, 3-5 times/week, for 2-4 weeks. One study included a follow-up period of up to 1.4 years. The tasks during NMV often involved indicating the subjective straight ahead (SSA, <i>n</i> = 8), pointing out targets, or no specific activity (<i>n</i> = 7, each). The SSA and cancellation tests were the most frequent outcomes.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>NMV application modalities varied widely across studies, with only vibration frequency showing consistency. The tasks performed during NMV and the outcome measures were diverse and generally unrelated to activities of daily living. Therefore, NMV effects during more ecologically valid tasks should be explored.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"848-867"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142739588","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Money versus performance feedback: money associated with lower feelings of cognitive fatigue. 金钱与表现反馈:金钱与较低的认知疲劳感相关。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2424533
Fareshte Erani, Harrison Stoll, Darshan Patel, Maria T Schultheis, John D Medaglia

Objective: Prior research suggests that effort and reward are central to cognitive fatigue. To better understand the influence of reward on cognitive fatigue, this study examined the effect of reward type and frequency on cognitive fatigue.

Participants and methods: In an online between-subjects study, 400 participants completed a computerized switching task and were randomly sorted into one of the five possible groups based on reward condition: [1] infrequent money, [2] frequent money, [3] infrequent performance-feedback, [4] frequent performance feedback, and [5] no-reward. Cognitive fatigue was assessed using the Visual Analog Scale for Fatigue (VAS-F) during the task. Mixed effects models were used to estimate the influence of reward type and frequency on task performance and cognitive fatigue.

Results: We found that participants in the monetary groups were significantly faster (p < .001) compared to participants in the feedback and no-reward groups. We also found that participants in the frequent-money group were significantly faster than those in the infrequent-money group (p < .001). We found that the group receiving infrequent-money was associated with a decrease in VAS-F scores compared to no-reward (p = .04).

Conclusions: The current study supports the role of reward in cognitive fatigue. Our results confirm well-established findings that money positively influences on-task behavior, especially when money is provided frequently. In a cognitively healthy sample, there is some evidence to suggest that money provided infrequently could decrease feelings of fatigue. Continued work is needed to understand how, and which, specific behavioral reward manipulations reduce fatigue, especially in clinical populations most affected by fatigue.

目的:先前的研究表明,努力和奖励是认知疲劳的核心。为了更好地了解奖励对认知疲劳的影响,本研究考察了奖励类型和频率对认知疲劳的影响。参与者和方法:在一项在线受试者之间的研究中,400名参与者完成了一项计算机化的切换任务,并根据奖励条件随机分为五组:[1]不经常得到钱,[2]经常得到钱,[3]不经常得到表现反馈,[4]经常得到表现反馈,[5]没有奖励。使用视觉模拟疲劳量表(VAS-F)评估任务期间的认知疲劳。采用混合效应模型估计奖励类型和频率对任务绩效和认知疲劳的影响。结果:我们发现货币组的参与者明显更快(p p p = .04)。结论:本研究支持奖励在认知疲劳中的作用。我们的研究结果证实了既定的发现,即金钱对任务行为有积极影响,尤其是在经常提供金钱的情况下。在一个认知健康的样本中,有一些证据表明,不经常提供金钱可以减少疲劳的感觉。需要继续研究如何以及哪些特定的行为奖励操作可以减少疲劳,特别是在受疲劳影响最大的临床人群中。
{"title":"Money versus performance feedback: money associated with lower feelings of cognitive fatigue.","authors":"Fareshte Erani, Harrison Stoll, Darshan Patel, Maria T Schultheis, John D Medaglia","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2424533","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2424533","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Prior research suggests that effort and reward are central to cognitive fatigue. To better understand the influence of reward on cognitive fatigue, this study examined the effect of reward type and frequency on cognitive fatigue.</p><p><strong>Participants and methods: </strong>In an online between-subjects study, 400 participants completed a computerized switching task and were randomly sorted into one of the five possible groups based on reward condition: [1] infrequent money, [2] frequent money, [3] infrequent performance-feedback, [4] frequent performance feedback, and [5] no-reward. Cognitive fatigue was assessed using the Visual Analog Scale for Fatigue (VAS-F) during the task. Mixed effects models were used to estimate the influence of reward type and frequency on task performance and cognitive fatigue.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found that participants in the monetary groups were significantly faster (<i>p</i> < .001) compared to participants in the feedback and no-reward groups. We also found that participants in the frequent-money group were significantly faster than those in the infrequent-money group (<i>p</i> < .001). We found that the group receiving infrequent-money was associated with a decrease in VAS-F scores compared to no-reward (<i>p</i> = .04).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The current study supports the role of reward in cognitive fatigue. Our results confirm well-established findings that money positively influences on-task behavior, especially when money is provided frequently. In a cognitively healthy sample, there is some evidence to suggest that money provided infrequently could decrease feelings of fatigue. Continued work is needed to understand how, and which, specific behavioral reward manipulations reduce fatigue, especially in clinical populations most affected by fatigue.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"794-809"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142750969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analyzing the relationship between processing speed impairment and Rey-15 item test performance. 分析处理速度障碍与 Rey-15 项目测试成绩之间的关系。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2406241
Brian Ramanauskas, Tana M Nixon, John-Christopher A Finley, Hannah B VanLandingham, Mira I Leese, Devin M Ulrich, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Brian M Cerny, Matthew S Phillips, Jason R Soble, Anthony D Robinson

Objective: This study investigated the relationship between processing speed impairment severity and performance on the Rey 15-Item Test (RFIT) and RFIT + Recognition.

Method: Cross-sectional data from 285 examinees (228 valid/57 invalid) referred for neuropsychological assessment who were administered the RFIT, Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) Processing Speed Index (PSI), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and three independent criterion PVTs were included. PSI bands were operationalized as Intact (≥85SS; n = 163), Reduced/Possibly Impaired (77-84SS; n = 36), or Impaired (≤76 SS; n = 29). Receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curve analyses tested the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition's classification accuracy for detecting invalid performance for the overall sample and by PSI impairment status.

Results: Those with intact processing speed performed significantly better on the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition than those with reduced/possibly impaired and impaired processing speed. Though verbal/visual memory predicted RFIT scores independently, PSI contributed additional variance. ROC curves for RFIT and RFIT + Recognition were significant (AUC=.64-.84). Optimal cut-scores yielded modest sensitivity (30%-63%) and high specificity (89%-93%) among those with intact and reduced processing speed but yielded unacceptable accuracy in those with impaired speed (AUC=.59-.62).

Conclusions: Although the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition demonstrated acceptable classification accuracy in those with intact processing speed, accuracy diminished with increasing speed impairment. This finding was more pronounced for RFIT + Recognition compared to the traditional RFIT. As such, the RFIT may have limited clinical utility in examinees with more significant processing speed deficits.

研究目的本研究探讨了处理速度障碍的严重程度与雷伊15项测验(RFIT)和RFIT+识别测验成绩之间的关系:方法:研究人员收集了285名接受神经心理学评估的受试者(228名有效/57名无效)的横断面数据,这些受试者分别接受了RFIT、韦氏成人智力量表-第四版(WAIS-IV)处理速度指数(PSI)、简明视觉空间记忆测试-修订版、Rey听觉言语学习测试以及三项独立标准PVT的测试。PSI 分级分为完好(≥85SS;n = 163)、降低/可能受损(77-84SS;n = 36)或受损(≤76 SS;n = 29)。通过接收操作者特征曲线(ROC)分析,测试了 RFIT 和 RFIT + Recognition 在检测总体样本和 PSI 损伤状态下的无效表现时的分类准确性:结果:在 RFIT 和 RFIT + 识别测试中,处理速度完好者的表现明显优于处理速度减弱/可能受损者和处理速度受损者。虽然言语/视觉记忆可独立预测 RFIT 分数,但 PSI 也会带来额外的差异。RFIT 和 RFIT + Recognition 的 ROC 曲线具有显著性(AUC=.64-.84)。在处理速度完好和降低的人群中,最佳切分分数产生了适度的灵敏度(30%-63%)和较高的特异性(89%-93%),但在处理速度受损的人群中却产生了不可接受的准确性(AUC=.59-.62):尽管 RFIT 和 RFIT + Recognition 对处理速度正常者的分类准确度可以接受,但准确度会随着速度受损程度的增加而降低。与传统的 RFIT 相比,RFIT + Recognition 的这一结果更为明显。因此,RFIT 对处理速度有明显缺陷的受试者的临床实用性可能有限。
{"title":"Analyzing the relationship between processing speed impairment and Rey-15 item test performance.","authors":"Brian Ramanauskas, Tana M Nixon, John-Christopher A Finley, Hannah B VanLandingham, Mira I Leese, Devin M Ulrich, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Brian M Cerny, Matthew S Phillips, Jason R Soble, Anthony D Robinson","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2406241","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2406241","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study investigated the relationship between processing speed impairment severity and performance on the Rey 15-Item Test (RFIT) and RFIT + Recognition.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Cross-sectional data from 285 examinees (228 valid/57 invalid) referred for neuropsychological assessment who were administered the RFIT, Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) Processing Speed Index (PSI), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and three independent criterion PVTs were included. PSI bands were operationalized as Intact (≥85SS; <i>n</i> = 163), Reduced/Possibly Impaired (77-84SS; <i>n</i> = 36), or Impaired (≤76 SS; <i>n</i> = 29). Receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curve analyses tested the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition's classification accuracy for detecting invalid performance for the overall sample and by PSI impairment status.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Those with intact processing speed performed significantly better on the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition than those with reduced/possibly impaired and impaired processing speed. Though verbal/visual memory predicted RFIT scores independently, PSI contributed additional variance. ROC curves for RFIT and RFIT + Recognition were significant (AUC=.64-.84). Optimal cut-scores yielded modest sensitivity (30%-63%) and high specificity (89%-93%) among those with intact and reduced processing speed but yielded unacceptable accuracy in those with impaired speed (AUC=.59-.62).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Although the RFIT and RFIT + Recognition demonstrated acceptable classification accuracy in those with intact processing speed, accuracy diminished with increasing speed impairment. This finding was more pronounced for RFIT + Recognition compared to the traditional RFIT. As such, the RFIT may have limited clinical utility in examinees with more significant processing speed deficits.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"707-717"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142347563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social and leisure engagement moderates the association between care partner distress and cognitive status of care recipients with dementia. 社交和休闲活动可调节痴呆症患者护理伴侣的痛苦与认知状况之间的关系。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2406586
Julie Gorenko, Cynthia McDowell, Nicholas Tamburri, Carren Dujela, André P Smith, Debra J Sheets, Stuart W S MacDonald

Objectives: Despite the established impact of care recipient cognitive impairment on care partner (CP) distress, limited quantitative research has explored how social and leisure engagement may moderate this relationship, offering a potential avenue for enhancing well-being in both care partners and recipients. The current study therefore examined the between- and within-dyad associations between cognitive impairment of persons with dementia (PwD) and their family CP's distress, and whether social and leisure activity moderated this relationship.

Methods: Data were utilized from dyads (PwD, n = 33, and their CPs, n = 34) engaged in the Voices in Motion project, a social-cognitive choral intervention for PwD and their family CPs. Measures indexing PwD cognitive status, CP distress, and PwD and CP social and leisure engagement were assessed using an intensive repeated-measures burst design, with multilevel models of change employed to disaggregate between- and within-person effects.

Results: Diminished cognitive functioning in PwD was associated with increased CP distress (p < 0.01) between-dyads; however, this relationship was not significant within-dyads. The between-dyad association was significantly moderated by the extent of social and leisure engagement of both CPs (p < .001) and PwD (p = .04). Follow-up simple slopes demonstrated that, at mean- and high-levels (+1SD) of social and leisure engagement for PwD and/or CP, increased PwD cognitive function significantly predicted lower CP distress.

Conclusion: The significant moderating influence of social and leisure engagement of dyads underscores the protective role of such engagement for reducing care-related distress. Activity engagement for CPs and PwD may help modulate the deleterious impact of PwD cognitive impairment and attenuate CP distress. These findings highlight the potential for dyadic interventions that promote social and leisure activities to mitigate caregiving challenges and enhance quality of life for both CPs and PwD.

目的:尽管已确定受照护者的认知障碍会对照护伴侣(CP)的痛苦产生影响,但很少有定量研究探讨社交和休闲活动如何缓和这种关系,从而为提高照护伴侣和受照护者的幸福感提供潜在的途径。因此,本研究探讨了痴呆症患者(PwD)的认知障碍与其家庭护理伙伴(CP)的苦恼之间的组间和组内关系,以及社交和休闲活动是否调节了这种关系:方法: 数据来自参与 "动感之声 "项目的二人组(痴呆症患者,33 人;痴呆症患者家属,34 人),该项目是一项针对痴呆症患者及其家属的社会认知合唱干预项目。该项目采用密集重复测量突发设计,通过多层次变化模型来分解人与人之间和人与人之间的影响,对残疾人的认知状况、CP 痛苦以及残疾人和 CP 的社交和休闲参与进行了评估:结果:残疾人认知功能的减弱与慢性阻塞性肺病患者痛苦的增加相关(p p p = .04)。后续的简单斜率表明,在平均水平和高水平(+1SD)的残疾人和/或慢性阻塞性肺病患者的社交和休闲参与中,残疾人认知功能的提高可显著预测慢性阻塞性肺病患者的痛苦程度降低:结论:二人组的社交和休闲参与具有重要的调节作用,强调了社交和休闲参与对减少护理相关困扰的保护作用。CP 和 PwD 的活动参与可能有助于调节 PwD 认知功能障碍的有害影响,并减轻 CP 的困扰。这些研究结果凸显了促进社交和休闲活动的双亲干预措施在减轻护理挑战和提高 CP 和 PwD 生活质量方面的潜力。
{"title":"Social and leisure engagement moderates the association between care partner distress and cognitive status of care recipients with dementia.","authors":"Julie Gorenko, Cynthia McDowell, Nicholas Tamburri, Carren Dujela, André P Smith, Debra J Sheets, Stuart W S MacDonald","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2406586","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2406586","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objectives: </strong>Despite the established impact of care recipient cognitive impairment on care partner (CP) distress, limited quantitative research has explored how social and leisure engagement may moderate this relationship, offering a potential avenue for enhancing well-being in both care partners and recipients. The current study therefore examined the between- and within-dyad associations between cognitive impairment of persons with dementia (PwD) and their family CP's distress, and whether social and leisure activity moderated this relationship.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Data were utilized from dyads (PwD, <i>n</i> = 33, and their CPs, <i>n</i> = 34) engaged in the Voices in Motion project, a social-cognitive choral intervention for PwD and their family CPs. Measures indexing PwD cognitive status, CP distress, and PwD and CP social and leisure engagement were assessed using an intensive repeated-measures burst design, with multilevel models of change employed to disaggregate between- and within-person effects.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Diminished cognitive functioning in PwD was associated with increased CP distress (<i>p</i> < 0.01) between-dyads; however, this relationship was not significant within-dyads. The between-dyad association was significantly moderated by the extent of social and leisure engagement of both CPs (<i>p</i> < .001) and PwD (<i>p</i> = .04). Follow-up simple slopes demonstrated that, at mean- and high-levels (+1<i>SD</i>) of social and leisure engagement for PwD and/or CP, increased PwD cognitive function significantly predicted lower CP distress.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The significant moderating influence of social and leisure engagement of dyads underscores the protective role of such engagement for reducing care-related distress. Activity engagement for CPs and PwD may help modulate the deleterious impact of PwD cognitive impairment and attenuate CP distress. These findings highlight the potential for dyadic interventions that promote social and leisure activities to mitigate caregiving challenges and enhance quality of life for both CPs and PwD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"742-754"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142380954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1