首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Initial expressed emotion during neuropsychological assessment: investigating motivational dimensions of approach and avoidance. 神经心理学评估过程中的初始情绪表达:研究接近和回避的动机维度。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-23 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2432655
Karlee Patrick, Erin Burke, John Gunstad, Mary Beth Spitznagel

Objective: Prior work indicates that discrete emotions are linked to performance across multiple domains of cognitive function and thus have the potential to impact cognitive profiles in neuropsychological assessment. However, reported presence and magnitude of the relationships between emotion and cognitive test performance are inconsistent. Variable findings in this regard could be due to failure to consider motivations associated with expressed emotion. To better understand the potential impact of expressed emotion on neuropsychological test performance, it may be beneficial to consider approach and avoidance motivation during assessment.

Method: The current cross-sectional study examined associations between cognitive performance and digitally phenotyped facial expressions of discrete emotions on dimensions of approach (i.e. joy, sadness, anger) and avoidance (i.e. fear, disgust) in the context of virtual neuropsychological assessment in 104 adults (ages 55-90).

Results: Initial facial expressions categorized as anger and joy predicted later reduced cognitive performance in aspects of memory and executive function within the virtual session, respectively. Test performance was associated neither with sadness nor with avoidance emotions (i.e. disgust or fear).

Conclusions: Results of the current study did not strongly align with approach/avoidance explanations for links between emotion and cognitive performance; however, results might support an arousal-based explanation, as joy and anger are both high arousal emotions. Additional investigation is needed to understand the intersection of emotion motivation and physiological arousal in the context of neuropsychological assessment.

目的:先前的研究表明,离散情绪与认知功能多个领域的表现有关,因此有可能影响神经心理学评估中的认知概况。然而,关于情绪与认知测试成绩之间关系的存在和程度的报道并不一致。这方面不同的研究结果可能是由于没有考虑到与表达情绪相关的动机。为了更好地了解表达情绪对神经心理学测试成绩的潜在影响,在评估过程中考虑接近和回避动机可能会有所裨益:本横断面研究考察了 104 名成年人(55-90 岁)在虚拟神经心理学评估中的认知表现与数字表型的离散情绪面部表情在接近(即喜悦、悲伤、愤怒)和回避(即恐惧、厌恶)维度上的关联:结果:最初被归类为愤怒和喜悦的面部表情分别预示着后来在虚拟会话中记忆和执行功能方面认知能力的下降。测试成绩既与悲伤情绪无关,也与回避情绪(即厌恶或恐惧)无关:目前的研究结果与情绪和认知表现之间联系的接近/回避解释并不十分吻合;然而,研究结果可能支持基于唤醒的解释,因为喜悦和愤怒都是高唤醒情绪。要了解神经心理学评估中情绪动机和生理唤醒的交叉点,还需要进行更多的调查。
{"title":"Initial expressed emotion during neuropsychological assessment: investigating motivational dimensions of approach and avoidance.","authors":"Karlee Patrick, Erin Burke, John Gunstad, Mary Beth Spitznagel","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2432655","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2432655","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Prior work indicates that discrete emotions are linked to performance across multiple domains of cognitive function and thus have the potential to impact cognitive profiles in neuropsychological assessment. However, reported presence and magnitude of the relationships between emotion and cognitive test performance are inconsistent. Variable findings in this regard could be due to failure to consider motivations associated with expressed emotion. To better understand the potential impact of expressed emotion on neuropsychological test performance, it may be beneficial to consider approach and avoidance motivation during assessment.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The current cross-sectional study examined associations between cognitive performance and digitally phenotyped facial expressions of discrete emotions on dimensions of approach (i.e. joy, sadness, anger) and avoidance (i.e. fear, disgust) in the context of virtual neuropsychological assessment in 104 adults (ages 55-90).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Initial facial expressions categorized as anger and joy predicted later reduced cognitive performance in aspects of memory and executive function within the virtual session, respectively. Test performance was associated neither with sadness nor with avoidance emotions (i.e. disgust or fear).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Results of the current study did not strongly align with approach/avoidance explanations for links between emotion and cognitive performance; however, results might support an arousal-based explanation, as joy and anger are both high arousal emotions. Additional investigation is needed to understand the intersection of emotion motivation and physiological arousal in the context of neuropsychological assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"913-922"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11802313/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142695520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social cognition in acquired brain injury: adaptation and validation of the Brief Assessment of Social Skills (BASS). 获得性脑损伤的社会认知:社会技能简要评估(BASS)的适应与验证。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2441704
Kimberley Wallis, Linda Elisabet Campbell, Skye McDonald, Michelle Kelly

Background: Acquired brain injury (ABI) is associated with social cognitive impairments, yet these impairments are often overlooked during clinical assessments. There are few validated and clinically appropriate measures of social cognition in ABI. The current study examined the validity of the Brief Assessment of Social Skills (BASS) in measuring social cognition following ABI.

Method: Twenty-eight people with ABI were recruited from local brain injury rehabilitation and support services and completed measures of social cognition, general intellectual ability, and social functioning. Twenty-eight controls demographically matched for age, gender, and years of education also performed these measures.

Results: A diagnosis of ABI was significantly associated with poorer performance on five subtests of the BASS. The BASS had moderate correlations with established measures of social cognition and measures characteristics that are distinguishable from general cognition. There was minimal evidence of a relationship between performance on the BASS and social functioning, with a significant relationship between a BASS subscale and informant-reported living skills and total social functioning. Using a series of case studies, the clinical utility of the BASS was emphasized by the development of unique social cognitive profiles across ABI individuals, including impairments in areas not significant at a group level.

Discussion: The BASS is a brief and comprehensive measure that is able to detect social cognition impairments in ABI patients. Given the prevalence of impairment in social cognition following ABI and the implications of these abilities on social functioning, this measure can be used in comprehensive neuropsychological assessment to guide and monitor progress toward rehabilitation goals.

背景:获得性脑损伤(ABI)与社会认知障碍有关,然而这些障碍在临床评估中经常被忽视。在ABI中,很少有经过验证和临床适用的社会认知测量方法。本研究考察了社会技能简要评估(BASS)在ABI后社会认知测量中的有效性。方法:从当地脑损伤康复和支持机构招募28例ABI患者,完成社会认知、一般智力和社会功能测试。在年龄、性别和受教育年限上匹配的28个对照组也进行了这些测量。结果:ABI的诊断与BASS的五个亚测试的较差表现显著相关。BASS与社会认知的既定测量和与一般认知不同的测量特征有中度相关性。很少有证据表明BASS的表现与社会功能之间存在关系,而BASS子量表与举报人报告的生活技能和总体社会功能之间存在显著关系。通过一系列的案例研究,通过在ABI个体中发展独特的社会认知概况,包括在群体水平上不显著的领域的损伤,强调了BASS的临床应用。讨论:BASS是一种能够检测ABI患者社会认知障碍的简单而全面的测量方法。鉴于ABI后社会认知障碍的普遍存在以及这些能力对社会功能的影响,该测量可用于综合神经心理学评估,以指导和监测康复目标的进展。
{"title":"Social cognition in acquired brain injury: adaptation and validation of the Brief Assessment of Social Skills (BASS).","authors":"Kimberley Wallis, Linda Elisabet Campbell, Skye McDonald, Michelle Kelly","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2441704","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2441704","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Acquired brain injury (ABI) is associated with social cognitive impairments, yet these impairments are often overlooked during clinical assessments. There are few validated and clinically appropriate measures of social cognition in ABI. The current study examined the validity of the Brief Assessment of Social Skills (BASS) in measuring social cognition following ABI.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Twenty-eight people with ABI were recruited from local brain injury rehabilitation and support services and completed measures of social cognition, general intellectual ability, and social functioning. Twenty-eight controls demographically matched for age, gender, and years of education also performed these measures.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>A diagnosis of ABI was significantly associated with poorer performance on five subtests of the BASS. The BASS had moderate correlations with established measures of social cognition and measures characteristics that are distinguishable from general cognition. There was minimal evidence of a relationship between performance on the BASS and social functioning, with a significant relationship between a BASS subscale and informant-reported living skills and total social functioning. Using a series of case studies, the clinical utility of the BASS was emphasized by the development of unique social cognitive profiles across ABI individuals, including impairments in areas not significant at a group level.</p><p><strong>Discussion: </strong>The BASS is a brief and comprehensive measure that is able to detect social cognition impairments in ABI patients. Given the prevalence of impairment in social cognition following ABI and the implications of these abilities on social functioning, this measure can be used in comprehensive neuropsychological assessment to guide and monitor progress toward rehabilitation goals.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"923-942"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142852932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inspecting the external world: Memory capacity, but not memory self-efficacy, predicts offloading in working memory. 检查外部世界:记忆容量,而不是记忆自我效能,预示着工作记忆的卸载。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2447263
Sanne Böing, Antonia F Ten Brink, Carla Ruis, Zoë A Schielen, Esther Van den Berg, J Matthijs Biesbroek, Tanja C W Nijboer, Stefan Van der Stigchel

Individuals with memory impairments may need to rely often on the external world (i.e. offloading). By memorizing only a fraction of the items at hand, and repeatedly looking back to the remainder of items (i.e. inspecting), they can avoid frailty or effortful memory use. However, individuals with subjective concerns may also prefer to rely on the external world even though their capacity is intact. Crucially, capacity assessment fails to recognize offloading strategies, while inspection assessment may reveal how people choose to deploy memory in everyday life. To disentangle the relative contributions of memory capacity and memory self-efficacy to offloading behavior, we recruited 29 individuals who were referred to a memory clinic and 38 age-matched individuals. We assessed memory capacity using neuropsychological measures, and memory self-efficacy using questionnaires. Inspection behavior was assessed in a copy task that allowed participants to store information to their preferred load or to rely on the external world. Referred individuals had lower capacity scores and lower memory self-efficacy. They inspected as often as controls, but used longer inspections and performed worse. Across all subjects, memory capacity - but not memory self-efficacy - explained inspection frequency and duration, with higher capacity associated with fewer and shorter inspections. Capacity measures thus translate to how people choose to deploy their memory in tasks that do not force full capacity use. However, people generally avoided remembering more than two items per inspection, and thus avoided using their full capacity. Inspection behavior was not further explained by memory self-efficacy, suggesting that inspections are not a sensitive measure of constraints experienced in everyday life. Although we provide support for the predictive value of capacity tasks in tasks with more degrees of freedom, capacity tasks overlook offloading behavior that individuals may employ to avoid using their full memory capacity in everyday life.

有记忆障碍的人可能需要经常依赖外部世界(即卸载)。通过只记住手边物品的一小部分,并反复回顾剩余物品(即检查),他们可以避免脆弱或费力的记忆使用。然而,有主观顾虑的个体也可能倾向于依赖外部世界,即使他们的能力是完整的。至关重要的是,容量评估无法识别卸载策略,而检查评估可能会揭示人们在日常生活中如何选择部署内存。为了解开记忆容量和记忆自我效能对卸载行为的相对贡献,我们招募了29名被转介到记忆诊所的个体和38名年龄匹配的个体。我们用神经心理学方法评估记忆容量,用问卷调查评估记忆自我效能。检查行为是在一个复制任务中评估的,该任务允许参与者将信息存储到他们喜欢的负载或依赖外部世界。被推荐个体的能力得分和记忆自我效能都较低。他们检查的频率和对照组一样,但检查时间更长,表现更差。在所有的研究对象中,记忆能力——而不是记忆自我效能——解释了检查的频率和持续时间,记忆能力越强,检查次数越少,时间越短。因此,容量测量可以转化为人们如何选择在不强制使用全部容量的任务中部署他们的内存。然而,人们通常避免每次检查记住两个以上的项目,从而避免使用他们的全部能力。记忆自我效能不能进一步解释检查行为,这表明检查不是日常生活中经历的约束的敏感测量。虽然我们在自由度更高的任务中支持容量任务的预测价值,但容量任务忽略了个体在日常生活中可能采用的卸载行为,以避免使用他们的全部记忆容量。
{"title":"Inspecting the external world: Memory capacity, but not memory self-efficacy, predicts offloading in working memory.","authors":"Sanne Böing, Antonia F Ten Brink, Carla Ruis, Zoë A Schielen, Esther Van den Berg, J Matthijs Biesbroek, Tanja C W Nijboer, Stefan Van der Stigchel","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2447263","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2447263","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals with memory impairments may need to rely often on the external world (i.e. offloading). By memorizing only a fraction of the items at hand, and repeatedly looking back to the remainder of items (i.e. inspecting), they can avoid frailty or effortful memory use. However, individuals with subjective concerns may also prefer to rely on the external world even though their capacity is intact. Crucially, capacity assessment fails to recognize offloading strategies, while inspection assessment may reveal how people choose to deploy memory in everyday life. To disentangle the relative contributions of memory capacity and memory self-efficacy to offloading behavior, we recruited 29 individuals who were referred to a memory clinic and 38 age-matched individuals. We assessed memory capacity using neuropsychological measures, and memory self-efficacy using questionnaires. Inspection behavior was assessed in a copy task that allowed participants to store information to their preferred load or to rely on the external world. Referred individuals had lower capacity scores and lower memory self-efficacy. They inspected as often as controls, but used longer inspections and performed worse. Across all subjects, memory capacity - but not memory self-efficacy - explained inspection frequency and duration, with higher capacity associated with fewer and shorter inspections. Capacity measures thus translate to how people choose to deploy their memory in tasks that do not force full capacity use. However, people generally avoided remembering more than two items per inspection, and thus avoided using their full capacity. Inspection behavior was not further explained by memory self-efficacy, suggesting that inspections are not a sensitive measure of constraints experienced in everyday life. Although we provide support for the predictive value of capacity tasks in tasks with more degrees of freedom, capacity tasks overlook offloading behavior that individuals may employ to avoid using their full memory capacity in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"943-965"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142921812","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analysis of skew, examination of intercorrelations, and determining the optimal threshold for performance invalidity when 10 performance validity tests are administered during a neuropsychological evaluation. 在神经心理学评估中,当进行10项效能效度测试时,分析偏斜,检查相互关系,并确定效能无效的最佳阈值。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-20 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2025.2455074
Mira I Leese, John-Christopher A Finley, Karen S Basurto, Hannah B VanLandingham, Justyna Piszczor, Joseph M Bianco, Matthew S Phillips, Brian M Cerny, Ryan W Schroeder, Jason R Soble

Introduction: This study cross-validates and expands upon previous research by examining the optimal number of PVT failures necessary to determine invalid performance when 10 PVTs are administered during a neuropsychological evaluation. Additionally, the study assessed the degree of skewness of individual PVTs and PVT intercorrelations for the overall sample and by validity group.

Method: Participants were 283 adult neuropsychology outpatients evaluated at an academic medical center. Participants were initially classified as having valid (≤1 PVT failure; n = 225) or invalid (≥2 PVT failures; n = 58; base rate of 20% performance invalidity) performance based on four independent criterion PVTs. Failure rates of 10 additional PVTs were then compared, and sensitivity and specificity were calculated at different thresholds (e.g. ≥1, ≥2, ≥3, ≥4 PVT failures) to determine the optimal threshold for detecting invalid performance while maintaining ≥ 90% specificity.

Results: Findings indicate that failing ≥ 2 PVTs yielded 86% sensitivity/76% specificity, failing ≥ 3 PVTs yielded 69% sensitivity/92% specificity, failing ≥ 4 PVTs yielded 57% sensitivity/96% specificity, failing ≥ 5 PVTs yielded 29% sensitivity/99% specificity, and failing ≥ 6 PVTs yielded 22% sensitivity/100% specificity. PVT intercorrelations were generally small for the overall sample and by validity group. As expected, data were more highly skewed for patients with valid performance.

Conclusions: Findings were consistent with previous research and demonstrate that the three-failure threshold optimally detects invalid performance when 10 PVTs are administered. These findings inform the use of multiple PVTs in clinical settings and aid in the interpretation of PVT results.

本研究交叉验证并扩展了先前的研究,通过检查在神经心理学评估中使用10个PVT来确定无效表现所需的PVT失败的最佳次数。此外,该研究评估了整体样本和有效性组的个体PVT和PVT相互关联的偏度程度。方法:研究对象为283名在某学术医疗中心接受评估的成年神经心理学门诊患者。参与者最初被分类为有效(≤1次PVT失败;n = 225)或无效(PVT失败≥2次;n = 58;基于四个独立标准pvt的性能。然后比较另外10个PVT的失败率,并在不同阈值(例如≥1、≥2、≥3、≥4 PVT失败)下计算灵敏度和特异性,以确定检测无效性能的最佳阈值,同时保持≥90%的特异性。结果:研究结果表明,PVTs≥2失败为86%敏感性/76%特异性,pts≥3失败为69%敏感性/92%特异性,pts≥4失败为57%敏感性/96%特异性,pts≥5失败为29%敏感性/99%特异性,pts≥6失败为22%敏感性/100%特异性。总体样本和效度组的PVT相互关系通常较小。正如预期的那样,对于表现良好的患者,数据偏差更大。结论:研究结果与先前的研究一致,并表明当给予10个pvt时,三失败阈值可以最佳地检测无效性能。这些发现为临床中多次PVT的使用提供了信息,并有助于解释PVT结果。
{"title":"Analysis of skew, examination of intercorrelations, and determining the optimal threshold for performance invalidity when 10 performance validity tests are administered during a neuropsychological evaluation.","authors":"Mira I Leese, John-Christopher A Finley, Karen S Basurto, Hannah B VanLandingham, Justyna Piszczor, Joseph M Bianco, Matthew S Phillips, Brian M Cerny, Ryan W Schroeder, Jason R Soble","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2455074","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2455074","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>This study cross-validates and expands upon previous research by examining the optimal number of PVT failures necessary to determine invalid performance when 10 PVTs are administered during a neuropsychological evaluation. Additionally, the study assessed the degree of skewness of individual PVTs and PVT intercorrelations for the overall sample and by validity group.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants were 283 adult neuropsychology outpatients evaluated at an academic medical center. Participants were initially classified as having valid (≤1 PVT failure; <i>n</i> = 225) or invalid (≥2 PVT failures; <i>n</i> = 58; base rate of 20% performance invalidity) performance based on four independent criterion PVTs. Failure rates of 10 additional PVTs were then compared, and sensitivity and specificity were calculated at different thresholds (e.g. ≥1, ≥2, ≥3, ≥4 PVT failures) to determine the optimal threshold for detecting invalid performance while maintaining ≥ 90% specificity.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Findings indicate that failing ≥ 2 PVTs yielded 86% sensitivity/76% specificity, failing ≥ 3 PVTs yielded 69% sensitivity/92% specificity, failing ≥ 4 PVTs yielded 57% sensitivity/96% specificity, failing ≥ 5 PVTs yielded 29% sensitivity/99% specificity, and failing ≥ 6 PVTs yielded 22% sensitivity/100% specificity. PVT intercorrelations were generally small for the overall sample and by validity group. As expected, data were more highly skewed for patients with valid performance.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings were consistent with previous research and demonstrate that the three-failure threshold optimally detects invalid performance when 10 PVTs are administered. These findings inform the use of multiple PVTs in clinical settings and aid in the interpretation of PVT results.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"989-1000"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143006111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-language neuropsychological measures increase sensitivity of identifying language reorganization in patients with epilepsy: a pilot study. 非语言神经心理学测量增加识别癫痫患者语言重组的敏感性:一项试点研究。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-15 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2025.2451320
Marielle Nagele, Zerrin Yetkin, Kenneth Chase Bailey, David Denney, Thomas O'neil, Sasha Alick, Roderick McColl, Jason A D Smith

Objective: To examine neuropsychological characteristic differences between typical and atypical language dominance in adult persons with epilepsy (PWE) and mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS), including exploring the impact of selected clinical variables on detection of atypical language and neuropsychological performance.

Methods: Adults with intractable epilepsy and MTS (n = 39) underwent comprehensive, pre-surgical evaluation including fMRI and neuropsychological assessment. Participants with concordant lateralization of MTS and seizure onset were included. Participants were grouped by dichotomized typical or atypical language lateralization based on fMRI results. Neuropsychological performance and other relevant clinical variables of the aforementioned groups were then compared.

Results: Those with atypical language demonstrated poorer performance across neuropsychological tasks as compared to those with typical language lateralization. Although, typical neuropsychological measures used to evaluate language lateralization were not among those significantly different between the groups. Differences in neuropsychological performance were particularly pronounced on TMT A, TMT B, Stroop (Color), GPB (Dominant), and GPB (Non-Dominant). ROC Curve was provided to evaluate reproducibility at different thresholds.

Conclusion: This pilot study revealed those with atypical language lateralization demonstrated greater cognitive dysfunction across neuropsychological tasks than those with typical language lateralization. Neuropsychological measures outside of the domain of language tests detected subtle changes of functional neuroanatomical reorganization while language domain tasks revealed no significant differences between aforementioned groups in pre-surgical evaluation of PWE. While these preliminary results require further replication, these are important implications for diagnostic and prognostic evaluation.

目的:探讨成人癫痫(PWE)和中颞叶硬化症(MTS)患者典型和非典型语言优势的神经心理特征差异,包括部分临床变量对非典型语言和神经心理表现检测的影响。方法:对39例成人顽固性癫痫合并MTS患者进行术前综合评估,包括功能磁共振成像(fMRI)和神经心理学评估。包括MTS侧化和癫痫发作一致的参与者。参与者根据功能磁共振成像结果分为典型或非典型语言侧化。然后比较上述两组的神经心理表现和其他相关临床变量。结果:那些非典型语言的人在神经心理任务中的表现比那些典型语言偏侧的人差。然而,用于评估语言侧化的典型神经心理学测量在两组之间并没有显著差异。在TMT A、TMT B、Stroop (Color)、GPB (Dominant)和GPB (Non-Dominant)上,神经心理表现的差异尤为明显。提供ROC曲线评价不同阈值下的再现性。结论:本初步研究显示非典型语言侧化患者在神经心理任务中表现出比典型语言侧化患者更大的认知功能障碍。语言测试领域外的神经心理学测量检测到功能神经解剖重组的细微变化,而语言领域任务显示上述组在术前评估PWE方面没有显着差异。虽然这些初步结果需要进一步的复制,但这些对诊断和预后评估具有重要意义。
{"title":"Non-language neuropsychological measures increase sensitivity of identifying language reorganization in patients with epilepsy: a pilot study.","authors":"Marielle Nagele, Zerrin Yetkin, Kenneth Chase Bailey, David Denney, Thomas O'neil, Sasha Alick, Roderick McColl, Jason A D Smith","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2451320","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2451320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To examine neuropsychological characteristic differences between typical and atypical language dominance in adult persons with epilepsy (PWE) and mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS), including exploring the impact of selected clinical variables on detection of atypical language and neuropsychological performance.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Adults with intractable epilepsy and MTS (<i>n</i> = 39) underwent comprehensive, pre-surgical evaluation including fMRI and neuropsychological assessment. Participants with concordant lateralization of MTS and seizure onset were included. Participants were grouped by dichotomized typical or atypical language lateralization based on fMRI results. Neuropsychological performance and other relevant clinical variables of the aforementioned groups were then compared.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Those with atypical language demonstrated poorer performance across neuropsychological tasks as compared to those with typical language lateralization. Although, typical neuropsychological measures used to evaluate language lateralization were not among those significantly different between the groups. Differences in neuropsychological performance were particularly pronounced on TMT A, TMT B, Stroop (Color), GPB (Dominant), and GPB (Non-Dominant). ROC Curve was provided to evaluate reproducibility at different thresholds.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>This pilot study revealed those with atypical language lateralization demonstrated greater cognitive dysfunction across neuropsychological tasks than those with typical language lateralization. Neuropsychological measures outside of the domain of language tests detected subtle changes of functional neuroanatomical reorganization while language domain tasks revealed no significant differences between aforementioned groups in pre-surgical evaluation of PWE. While these preliminary results require further replication, these are important implications for diagnostic and prognostic evaluation.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"978-988"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143006113","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between self-monitoring and cognitive strategy use in midlife and older adults. 中老年人自我监控与认知策略使用的关系。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-20 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2025.2451315
Nicole Whiteley, Brooke F Beech, Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe

Introduction: Self-monitoring abilities, both in the moment (online) and general self-knowledge (offline) of one's errors, are crucial to implementing modification to tasks to support healthy, independent aging. Cognitive strategies (CS) aid in functional, physical, and cognitive abilities, but without recognition of their need, individuals may struggle to complete daily tasks. The current study examined whether higher levels of self-monitoring would predict higher use and quality of real-world cognitive strategies in older adults.

Methods: Participants included 80 community-dwelling midlife and older adults. Participants completed a remote battery of neuropsychological tasks, including a computerized go-no-go task that evaluated online self-monitoring, and a self-reported questionnaire to measure offline self-monitoring (Cognitive Self-Efficacy Questionnaire). To assess CS, a count score (CS Quantity) and utility score (CS Quality) were computed based on strategies utilized in completion of real-world prospective memory tasks.

Results: Online self-monitoring was not significantly related to offline self-monitoring (r(77) = -.07, p = .52). A hierarchical regression revealed that while offline self-monitoring significantly predicted 7% of the variance in CS Quality, above and beyond age, global cognition, and premorbid functioning (ΔR2 = .07, ΔF = 6.23, p = .02), the addition of online self-monitoring did not contribute significant incremental validity (ΔR2 = .001, ΔF = 0.12, p = .73). The second hierarchical regression revealed that neither online nor offline self-monitoring significantly predicted CS Quantity, after controlling for sex (ΔR2 = .004, ΔF = 0.29, p = .60).

Conclusion: The results support the distinction between online and offline self-monitoring concepts and their assessment. For community-dwelling midlife and older adults without dementia, clinicians may consider an individual's perceptions of their ability to self-monitor when working to facilitate the use of cognitive strategies.

自我监控能力,无论是在当下(在线)还是对自己错误的一般自我认识(离线),对于实施任务修改以支持健康、独立的老龄化至关重要。认知策略(CS)有助于提高功能、身体和认知能力,但如果不认识到它们的需求,个体可能难以完成日常任务。目前的研究调查了更高水平的自我监控是否预示着老年人对现实世界认知策略的更高使用和质量。方法:参与者包括80名居住在社区的中年和老年人。参与者完成了一系列远程神经心理学任务,包括评估在线自我监控的计算机化go-no-go任务,以及测量离线自我监控的自我报告问卷(认知自我效能问卷)。为了评估CS,计数得分(CS数量)和效用得分(CS质量)是基于完成现实世界前瞻性记忆任务所使用的策略计算的。结果:在线自我监测与离线自我监测无显著相关(r(77) = -)。07, p = .52)。分层回归显示,离线自我监测显著预测7%的CS质量方差,高于和超过年龄、整体认知和病前功能(ΔR2 =。07, ΔF = 6.23, p = .02),增加在线自我监测对增加效度没有显著贡献(ΔR2 =。001, ΔF = 0.12, p = 0.73)。第二次层次回归显示,在控制性别(ΔR2 =)后,在线和离线自我监控都不能显著预测CS数量。004, ΔF = 0.29, p = 0.60)。结论:研究结果支持线上与线下自我监控概念的区分及其评价。对于居住在社区的中年人和没有痴呆症的老年人,临床医生在促进认知策略的使用时,可能会考虑个人对自我监控能力的感知。
{"title":"The relationship between self-monitoring and cognitive strategy use in midlife and older adults.","authors":"Nicole Whiteley, Brooke F Beech, Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2451315","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2451315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Self-monitoring abilities, both in the moment (online) and general self-knowledge (offline) of one's errors, are crucial to implementing modification to tasks to support healthy, independent aging. Cognitive strategies (CS) aid in functional, physical, and cognitive abilities, but without recognition of their need, individuals may struggle to complete daily tasks. The current study examined whether higher levels of self-monitoring would predict higher use and quality of real-world cognitive strategies in older adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Participants included 80 community-dwelling midlife and older adults. Participants completed a remote battery of neuropsychological tasks, including a computerized go-no-go task that evaluated online self-monitoring, and a self-reported questionnaire to measure offline self-monitoring (Cognitive Self-Efficacy Questionnaire). To assess CS, a count score (CS Quantity) and utility score (CS Quality) were computed based on strategies utilized in completion of real-world prospective memory tasks.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Online self-monitoring was not significantly related to offline self-monitoring (<i>r</i>(77) = -.07, <i>p</i> = .52). A hierarchical regression revealed that while offline self-monitoring significantly predicted 7% of the variance in CS Quality, above and beyond age, global cognition, and premorbid functioning (Δ<i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .07, Δ<i>F</i> = 6.23, <i>p</i> = .02), the addition of online self-monitoring did not contribute significant incremental validity (Δ<i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .001, Δ<i>F</i> = 0.12, <i>p</i> = .73). The second hierarchical regression revealed that neither online nor offline self-monitoring significantly predicted CS Quantity, after controlling for sex (Δ<i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .004, Δ<i>F</i> = 0.29, <i>p</i> = .60).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The results support the distinction between online and offline self-monitoring concepts and their assessment. For community-dwelling midlife and older adults without dementia, clinicians may consider an individual's perceptions of their ability to self-monitor when working to facilitate the use of cognitive strategies.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"966-977"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11802286/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143006115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Detecting noncredible symptomology in ADHD evaluations using machine learning.
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-25 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2025.2458547
John-Christopher A Finley, Matthew S Phillips, Jason R Soble, Violeta J Rodriguez

Introduction: Diagnostic evaluations for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are becoming increasingly complicated by the number of adults who fabricate or exaggerate symptoms. Novel methods are needed to improve the assessment process required to detect these noncredible symptoms. The present study investigated whether unsupervised machine learning (ML) could serve as one such method, and detect noncredible symptom reporting in adults undergoing ADHD evaluations.

Method: Participants were 623 adults who underwent outpatient ADHD evaluations. Patients' scores from symptom validity tests embedded in two self-report questionnaires were examined in an unsupervised ML model. The model, called "sidClustering," is based on a clustering and random forest algorithm. The model synthesized the raw scores (without cutoffs) from the symptom validity tests into an unspecified number of groups. The groups were then compared to predetermined ratings of credible versus noncredible symptom reporting. The noncredible symptom ratings were defined by either two or three or more symptom validity test elevations.

Results: The model identified two groups that were significantly (p < .001) and meaningfully associated with the predetermined ratings of credible or noncredible symptom reporting, regardless of the number of elevations used to define noncredible reporting. The validity test assessing overreporting of various types of psychiatric symptoms was most influential in determining group membership; but symptom validity tests regarding ADHD-specific symptoms were also contributory.

Conclusion: These findings suggest that unsupervised ML can effectively identify noncredible symptom reporting using scores from multiple symptom validity tests without predetermined cutoffs. The ML-derived groups also support the use of two validity test elevations to identify noncredible symptom reporting. Collectively, these findings serve as a proof of concept that unsupervised ML can improve the process of detecting noncredible symptoms during ADHD evaluations. With additional research, unsupervised ML may become a useful supplementary tool for quickly and accurately detecting noncredible symptoms during these evaluations.

{"title":"Detecting noncredible symptomology in ADHD evaluations using machine learning.","authors":"John-Christopher A Finley, Matthew S Phillips, Jason R Soble, Violeta J Rodriguez","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2458547","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2458547","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Diagnostic evaluations for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are becoming increasingly complicated by the number of adults who fabricate or exaggerate symptoms. Novel methods are needed to improve the assessment process required to detect these noncredible symptoms. The present study investigated whether unsupervised machine learning (ML) could serve as one such method, and detect noncredible symptom reporting in adults undergoing ADHD evaluations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants were 623 adults who underwent outpatient ADHD evaluations. Patients' scores from symptom validity tests embedded in two self-report questionnaires were examined in an unsupervised ML model. The model, called \"sidClustering,\" is based on a clustering and random forest algorithm. The model synthesized the raw scores (without cutoffs) from the symptom validity tests into an unspecified number of groups. The groups were then compared to predetermined ratings of credible versus noncredible symptom reporting. The noncredible symptom ratings were defined by either two or three or more symptom validity test elevations.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The model identified two groups that were significantly (<i>p</i> < .001) and meaningfully associated with the predetermined ratings of credible or noncredible symptom reporting, regardless of the number of elevations used to define noncredible reporting. The validity test assessing overreporting of various types of psychiatric symptoms was most influential in determining group membership; but symptom validity tests regarding ADHD-specific symptoms were also contributory.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These findings suggest that unsupervised ML can effectively identify noncredible symptom reporting using scores from multiple symptom validity tests without predetermined cutoffs. The ML-derived groups also support the use of two validity test elevations to identify noncredible symptom reporting. Collectively, these findings serve as a proof of concept that unsupervised ML can improve the process of detecting noncredible symptoms during ADHD evaluations. With additional research, unsupervised ML may become a useful supplementary tool for quickly and accurately detecting noncredible symptoms during these evaluations.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1015-1025"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143039026","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of noise exposure, time pressure, and cognitive load on objective task performance and subjective sensory overload and fatigue.
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-27 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2025.2458539
Marilien C Marzolla, Lex Borghans, Juliëtte Ebus, Martyna Gwiazda, Caroline van Heugten, Petra Hurks

Introduction: Sensory hypersensitivity (SHS) refers to an increased sensitivity to sensory stimuli, often leading to sensory overload and adversely affecting daily functioning and well-being. This study examined the effects of three situational triggers - noise, time pressure, and cognitive load - on task performance, sensory overload, and fatigue. Additionally, we sought to explore the associations between these effects and SHS, while accounting for other influencing factors such as personality, coping mechanisms, and anxiety.

Method: We experimentally tested 105 university students, employing a visuospatial task (the Paper Folding Test, PFT) under eight different conditions, manipulating the three situational triggers. The measured outcomes included task accuracy, average response time, sensory overload, and fatigue. Participants also completed several questionnaires: Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS), Multi-Modal Evaluation of Sensory Sensitivity (MESSY), State and Trait Anxiety Index, Big Five Inventory, and COPE Easy.

Results: Our findings indicated that sensory overload increased as more situational triggers were introduced, with noise having the most significant impact. However, this increase in sensory overload did not correspond to changes in objective performance measures, such as accuracy and average response time on the PFT, which were primarily influenced by cognitive load (i.e. easy versus difficult items). Additionally, individuals with higher levels of SHS (HSPS and MESSY) reported greater overall sensory overload and fatigue. Nonetheless, the impact of the triggers on sensory overload and fatigue was not exclusive to those with high SHS, and neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, and trait anxiety were significant predictors of SHS, more so than task-related outcomes.

Conclusions: Feelings of sensory overload may not necessarily impair cognitive performance, and the impact of situational triggers can be similar for individuals with and without SHS. This implies that the burden of SHS and overall sensory overload may be influenced by other underlying factors leading to an elevation of baseline sensory overload, warranting further investigation.

{"title":"The impact of noise exposure, time pressure, and cognitive load on objective task performance and subjective sensory overload and fatigue.","authors":"Marilien C Marzolla, Lex Borghans, Juliëtte Ebus, Martyna Gwiazda, Caroline van Heugten, Petra Hurks","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2458539","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2025.2458539","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Sensory hypersensitivity (SHS) refers to an increased sensitivity to sensory stimuli, often leading to sensory overload and adversely affecting daily functioning and well-being. This study examined the effects of three situational triggers - noise, time pressure, and cognitive load - on task performance, sensory overload, and fatigue. Additionally, we sought to explore the associations between these effects and SHS, while accounting for other influencing factors such as personality, coping mechanisms, and anxiety.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We experimentally tested 105 university students, employing a visuospatial task (the Paper Folding Test, PFT) under eight different conditions, manipulating the three situational triggers. The measured outcomes included task accuracy, average response time, sensory overload, and fatigue. Participants also completed several questionnaires: Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS), Multi-Modal Evaluation of Sensory Sensitivity (MESSY), State and Trait Anxiety Index, Big Five Inventory, and COPE Easy.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Our findings indicated that sensory overload increased as more situational triggers were introduced, with noise having the most significant impact. However, this increase in sensory overload did not correspond to changes in objective performance measures, such as accuracy and average response time on the PFT, which were primarily influenced by cognitive load (i.e. easy versus difficult items). Additionally, individuals with higher levels of SHS (HSPS and MESSY) reported greater overall sensory overload and fatigue. Nonetheless, the impact of the triggers on sensory overload and fatigue was not exclusive to those with high SHS, and neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, and trait anxiety were significant predictors of SHS, more so than task-related outcomes.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Feelings of sensory overload may not necessarily impair cognitive performance, and the impact of situational triggers can be similar for individuals with and without SHS. This implies that the burden of SHS and overall sensory overload may be influenced by other underlying factors leading to an elevation of baseline sensory overload, warranting further investigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1001-1014"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143046496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impairments of attention in RRMS patients: the role of disease duration. RRMS 患者的注意力障碍:病程的作用。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-27 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421
Devrim Kalkan, Murat Kurt

Introduction: The extent to which different types of attention are affected in RRMS based on disease duration has not been extensively analyzed. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine whether MS patients differ compared to healthy individuals in a homogeneous sample of RRMS patients in terms of attention types and from which year of MS attention deficit starts. Another aim of the study was to examine the effect of MS duration and stimulus onset asynchrony on dual task performance.

Methods: The sample consisted of RRMS patients (n = 53) and healthy participants (n = 30) between the ages of 20-49, who were at least primary school graduates. Healthy participants in the comparison group were reached by snowball sampling technique. Stroop Test, Cancellation Test, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, Coding Test, WMS-R Digit Span and Visual Memory Span subtests were administered to assess attention. Divided attention performance was assessed with a dual task developed based on psychological refractory period paradigm.

Results: The results show that there is a significant difference between RRMS patients and healthy participants in terms of different types of attention (p < 0.05). Focused, sustained and divided attention of RRMS patients and the ability to resist interference showed a significant decline from the 7th year of the disease (p < 0.05); no significant difference was found between healthy participants and patients with 1-6 years of RRMS.

Conclusions: Although the results of the study are consistent with the literature which show that attention deficit develops in MS, it is important in terms of showing that attention deficit changes depending on the duration of the disease. Focused attention, sustained attention, interference resistance and divided attention performance of RRMS patients showed a significant decline after the 7th year of the disease.

导言:关于 RRMS 患者不同类型的注意力受疾病持续时间影响的程度,尚未进行广泛的分析。因此,本研究旨在确定在 RRMS 患者的同质样本中,多发性硬化症患者与健康人相比在注意类型上是否存在差异,以及多发性硬化症患者的注意缺陷是从哪一年开始的。研究的另一个目的是考察多发性硬化症病程和刺激开始不同步对双重任务表现的影响:样本包括 RRMS 患者(53 人)和健康参与者(30 人),年龄在 20-49 岁之间,至少小学毕业。对比组中的健康参与者是通过滚雪球抽样技术获得的。研究人员进行了 Stroop 测试、取消测试、步调听觉连续加法测试、编码测试、WMS-R 数字跨度和视觉记忆跨度子测试,以评估注意力。在心理折射期范式的基础上开发的双重任务评估了注意力分散的表现:结果表明,RRMS 患者和健康参与者在不同类型的注意力方面存在显著差异(p p 结论:虽然研究结果与健康参与者的结果一致,但RRMS 患者和健康参与者在不同类型的注意力方面存在显著差异:尽管研究结果与文献中关于多发性硬化症患者会出现注意力缺陷的观点一致,但重要的是,研究结果表明,注意力缺陷会随着病程的延长而发生变化。RRMS 患者的集中注意力、持续注意力、抗干扰能力和分心能力在病程第 7 年后显著下降。
{"title":"Impairments of attention in RRMS patients: the role of disease duration.","authors":"Devrim Kalkan, Murat Kurt","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2427421","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>The extent to which different types of attention are affected in RRMS based on disease duration has not been extensively analyzed. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine whether MS patients differ compared to healthy individuals in a homogeneous sample of RRMS patients in terms of attention types and from which year of MS attention deficit starts. Another aim of the study was to examine the effect of MS duration and stimulus onset asynchrony on dual task performance.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The sample consisted of RRMS patients (<i>n</i> = 53) and healthy participants (<i>n</i> = 30) between the ages of 20-49, who were at least primary school graduates. Healthy participants in the comparison group were reached by snowball sampling technique. Stroop Test, Cancellation Test, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, Coding Test, WMS-R Digit Span and Visual Memory Span subtests were administered to assess attention. Divided attention performance was assessed with a dual task developed based on psychological refractory period paradigm.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The results show that there is a significant difference between RRMS patients and healthy participants in terms of different types of attention (<i>p</i> < 0.05). Focused, sustained and divided attention of RRMS patients and the ability to resist interference showed a significant decline from the 7th year of the disease (<i>p</i> < 0.05); no significant difference was found between healthy participants and patients with 1-6 years of RRMS.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Although the results of the study are consistent with the literature which show that attention deficit develops in MS, it is important in terms of showing that attention deficit changes depending on the duration of the disease. Focused attention, sustained attention, interference resistance and divided attention performance of RRMS patients showed a significant decline after the 7th year of the disease.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"891-912"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142728967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A tale of two constructs: confirmatory factor analysis of performance and symptom validity tests. 两个结构的故事:绩效和症状有效性测试的确认性因素分析。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-10 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004
Michael R Basso, Savanna M Tierney, Brad L Roper, Douglas M Whiteside, Dennis R Combs, Eduardo Estevis

Background: Performance validity (PV) and symptom validity (SV) tests assess biased responding that impact scores on neuropsychological tests. The extent to which PV and SV represent overlapping or unique constructs remains incompletely defined, especially among psychiatric patients in a non-forensic setting. The current study investigated this question using confirmatory factor analysis.

Method: Eighty-two inpatients with mood disorders were administered the Word Memory Test, and its primary indices formed a latent variable of PV. From the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 the Fake Bad Scale (FBS), Response Bias Scale (RBS), and Henry-Heilbronner Index (HHI) were employed as a latent SV variable. Two models of the relationship between PV and SV were compared. One freely estimated the shared variance between SV and PV latent constructs. The other assumed the relationship between SV and PV was homogeneous, and covariance was fixed to 1.0.

Results: In the freely estimated model, covariance between PV and SV was -0.18, and model fit was excellent (CFI = 0.098; TLI = 0.096; SRMR = 0.08). For the fixed model, the RBS, HHI, and FBS achieved low loadings on the SV construct, and model fit was poor (CFI = 0.66; TLI = 0.43; SRMR = 0.42).

Conclusions: PV as indexed by the WMT and SV measured by the MMPI-2 are not overlapping constructs among inpatients with mood disorders. These data imply that PV and SV represent distinct constructs in this population. Implications for practice are discussed.

背景:表现效度(PV)和症状效度(SV)测试评估的是影响神经心理测试得分的偏差反应。表现效度和症状效度在多大程度上代表重叠或独特的结构,目前仍未完全确定,尤其是在非法医环境下的精神病患者中。本研究采用确证因子分析法对这一问题进行了研究:方法:对 82 名患有情绪障碍的住院病人进行了单词记忆测试,其主要指标构成了 PV 的潜变量。明尼苏达多相人格量表-2》中的假恶丑量表(FBS)、反应偏差量表(RBS)和亨利-海尔布隆纳指数(HHI)被用作潜在 SV 变量。比较了 PV 和 SV 之间关系的两个模型。一种是自由估计 SV 和 PV 潜在结构之间的共享方差。另一种假设 SV 和 PV 之间的关系是同质的,协方差被固定为 1.0:在自由估计模型中,PV 和 SV 之间的协方差为-0.18,模型拟合度非常好(CFI = 0.098;TLI = 0.096;SRMR = 0.08)。在固定模型中,RBS、HHI 和 FBS 在 SV 结构上的载荷较低,模型拟合度较差(CFI = 0.66;TLI = 0.43;SRMR = 0.42):结论:在患有情绪障碍的住院患者中,用 WMT 表示的 PV 和用 MMPI-2 测量的 SV 并不是重叠的结构。这些数据表明,在这一人群中,PV 和 SV 代表了不同的结构。本文讨论了对实践的影响。
{"title":"A tale of two constructs: confirmatory factor analysis of performance and symptom validity tests.","authors":"Michael R Basso, Savanna M Tierney, Brad L Roper, Douglas M Whiteside, Dennis R Combs, Eduardo Estevis","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2425004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Performance validity (PV) and symptom validity (SV) tests assess biased responding that impact scores on neuropsychological tests. The extent to which PV and SV represent overlapping or unique constructs remains incompletely defined, especially among psychiatric patients in a non-forensic setting. The current study investigated this question using confirmatory factor analysis.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Eighty-two inpatients with mood disorders were administered the Word Memory Test, and its primary indices formed a latent variable of PV. From the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 the Fake Bad Scale (FBS), Response Bias Scale (RBS), and Henry-Heilbronner Index (HHI) were employed as a latent SV variable. Two models of the relationship between PV and SV were compared. One freely estimated the shared variance between SV and PV latent constructs. The other assumed the relationship between SV and PV was homogeneous, and covariance was fixed to 1.0.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In the freely estimated model, covariance between PV and SV was -0.18, and model fit was excellent (CFI = 0.098; TLI = 0.096; SRMR = 0.08). For the fixed model, the RBS, HHI, and FBS achieved low loadings on the SV construct, and model fit was poor (CFI = 0.66; TLI = 0.43; SRMR = 0.42).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PV as indexed by the WMT and SV measured by the MMPI-2 are not overlapping constructs among inpatients with mood disorders. These data imply that PV and SV represent distinct constructs in this population. Implications for practice are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"840-847"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1