首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Multidimensional assessment of social cognition using non-immersive virtual reality in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: an exploratory study. 利用非沉浸式虚拟现实技术对路易体痴呆症和阿尔茨海默氏症患者的社会认知进行多维度评估:一项探索性研究。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362
Eva-Flore Msika, Marc Verny, Bénédicte Dieudonné, Nathalie Ehrlé, Alexandre Gaston-Bellegarde, Eric Orriols, Pascale Piolino, Pauline Narme

Introduction: Few studies have focused on social cognition in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), even though some brain structures being well known as underlying social cognitive processes are directly impacted in this disease. Furthermore, social cognition processes have been mostly studied independently using evaluations with poor ecological validity. We aimed at studying the ability of a new naturalistic and multidimensional social cognition task to reveal impairments in DLB patients. We chose to compare the profile of these patients with that of Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, for which social cognition is better preserved.

Method: Fifteen patients (DLB: n = 7; AD: n = 8) and 28 healthy controls underwent the REALSoCog task. They encountered several social situations (e.g. control versus transgressions) in a non-immersive virtual city environment allowing the assessment of moral cognition, cognitive and affective theory of mind (ToM), emotional empathy and behavioral intentions.

Results: The main results showed (i) a lower ability to detect transgressions in DLB patients, particularly conventional ones, whereas moral cognition seemed better preserved in AD patients; (ii) a cognitive ToM impairment in both DLB and AD patients, while affective ToM is impaired only in DLB patients; (iii) a decreased emotional empathy specifically observed in DLB patients; (iv) more inappropriate behavioral intentions, mainly in DLB patients, but also in some AD patients.

Conclusions: This study suggests the feasibility and potential interest of the REALSoCog task in revealing social cognition deficits, particularly for DLB patients by showing different social patterns as compared to AD patients. These results offer interesting clinical perspectives to develop more naturalistic tasks in such populations and for clinical differential diagnosis. Limitations and future perspectives are discussed.

引言尽管众所周知,路易体痴呆症(DLB)的某些大脑结构会直接影响患者的社会认知过程,但很少有研究关注这种疾病的社会认知过程。此外,对社会认知过程的研究大多采用生态效度较差的独立评估。我们的目的是研究一种新的自然多维社会认知任务揭示 DLB 患者损伤的能力。我们选择将这些患者的情况与阿尔茨海默病(AD)患者的情况进行比较,后者的社会认知能力保存得更好:15 名患者(DLB:n = 7;AD:n = 8)和 28 名健康对照者接受了 REALSoCog 任务。他们在非沉浸式虚拟城市环境中遇到了几种社会情境(如控制与过失),从而对道德认知、认知和情感心智理论(ToM)、情感共鸣和行为意图进行了评估:主要结果显示:(i) DLB 患者发现越轨行为的能力较低,尤其是传统越轨行为,而注意力缺失症患者的道德认知似乎保存得更好;(ii) DLB 和注意力缺失症患者的认知心智理论(ToM)均受损,而情感心智理论(ToM)仅在 DLB 患者中受损;(iii) DLB 患者的情感共鸣明显降低;(iv) 主要在 DLB 患者中,但在一些注意力缺失症患者中,不当行为意向较多:本研究表明,REALSoCog 任务在揭示社会认知缺陷方面具有可行性和潜在的兴趣,特别是通过显示与注意力缺失症患者不同的社会模式,揭示了 DLB 患者的社会认知缺陷。这些结果为在此类人群中开发更多自然任务和临床鉴别诊断提供了有趣的临床视角。本文还讨论了研究的局限性和未来展望。
{"title":"Multidimensional assessment of social cognition using non-immersive virtual reality in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: an exploratory study.","authors":"Eva-Flore Msika, Marc Verny, Bénédicte Dieudonné, Nathalie Ehrlé, Alexandre Gaston-Bellegarde, Eric Orriols, Pascale Piolino, Pauline Narme","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Few studies have focused on social cognition in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), even though some brain structures being well known as underlying social cognitive processes are directly impacted in this disease. Furthermore, social cognition processes have been mostly studied independently using evaluations with poor ecological validity. We aimed at studying the ability of a new naturalistic and multidimensional social cognition task to reveal impairments in DLB patients. We chose to compare the profile of these patients with that of Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, for which social cognition is better preserved.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Fifteen patients (DLB: <i>n</i> = 7; AD: <i>n</i> = 8) and 28 healthy controls underwent the REALSoCog task. They encountered several social situations (e.g. control versus transgressions) in a non-immersive virtual city environment allowing the assessment of moral cognition, cognitive and affective theory of mind (ToM), emotional empathy and behavioral intentions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The main results showed (i) a lower ability to detect transgressions in DLB patients, particularly conventional ones, whereas moral cognition seemed better preserved in AD patients; (ii) a cognitive ToM impairment in both DLB and AD patients, while affective ToM is impaired only in DLB patients; (iii) a decreased emotional empathy specifically observed in DLB patients; (iv) more inappropriate behavioral intentions, mainly in DLB patients, but also in some AD patients.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study suggests the feasibility and potential interest of the REALSoCog task in revealing social cognition deficits, particularly for DLB patients by showing different social patterns as compared to AD patients. These results offer interesting clinical perspectives to develop more naturalistic tasks in such populations and for clinical differential diagnosis. Limitations and future perspectives are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141174012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Young children's performance on a design fluency task: longitudinal data on total number of designs, clustering and switching, and regression-based norms. 幼儿在设计流畅性任务中的表现:关于设计总数、分组和切换以及基于回归的标准的纵向数据。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744
Christine Resch, Esther Keulers, Rosa Martens, Gerard van Breukelen, Caroline M van Heugten, Wenying Hou, Petra P M Hurks

Introduction: Design fluency (DF) tasks are commonly used to assess executive functions such as attentional control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and strategy use. Next to the total number of correct designs, the standard outcome of a DF task, clustering and switching can help disentangle the processes underlying DF performance. We present the first longitudinal study of 4-8-year-old children's developmental DF trajectories.

Method: At initial enrollment, children (n = 228) were aged between 4.05 and 6.88 years (M = 5.18, SD = 0.59) and attended Dutch primary schools. The DF task was administered at three time points, each time point separated by approximately 1 year. Data were analyzed using mixed regression for total number of correct designs and switching, and mixed logistic regression analysis for clustering.

Results: The total number of correct designs increased linearly across the three time points. Across all time points, children made very few clusters, and most clusters consisted of only 3 designs. Clustering only increased at the third assessment compared to the two previous assessments. Switching increased up to the second assessment, but not after that. The number of switches was highly correlated with the total number of correct designs at all time points (r = 0.78 to r = 0.85). These developmental trajectories were similar for all children regardless of their baseline age. Normative data are given for the total number of correct designs and switching.

Conclusions: Children as of age 4 onwards can perform a DF task. For children as young as 4-8 years old, computing clustering, and switching measures is of limited value to study cognitive processes underlying DF performance, next to the total number of correct designs. There were no sex differences on any of the DF outcomes. Level of parental education (LPE) was positively associated with the total number of correct designs and switching.

简介:设计流畅性(DF)任务通常用于评估注意力控制、认知灵活性、自我监控和策略使用等执行功能:设计流畅性(DF)任务通常用于评估注意力控制、认知灵活性、自我监控和策略使用等执行功能。设计流畅性任务的标准结果是正确设计的总数,除此以外,聚类和转换也有助于厘清设计流畅性表现的基本过程。我们首次对 4-8 岁儿童的 DF 发展轨迹进行了纵向研究:最初入学时,儿童(n = 228)的年龄在 4.05 到 6.88 岁之间(M = 5.18,SD = 0.59),就读于荷兰小学。DF 任务在三个时间点进行,每个时间点相隔约 1 年。数据采用混合回归法对正确设计总数和转换进行分析,并采用混合逻辑回归分析法对数据进行聚类分析:结果:在三个时间点上,正确设计的总数呈线性增长。在所有时间点上,孩子们的聚类都很少,大多数聚类都只有 3 个设计。与前两次评估相比,聚类只在第三次评估时才有所增加。转换次数在第二次评估前有所增加,但之后就没有了。在所有时间点上,切换次数与正确设计总数都高度相关(r = 0.78 到 r = 0.85)。无论基线年龄如何,所有儿童的发展轨迹都相似。我们还给出了正确设计总数和转换的标准数据:结论:4 岁以上的儿童可以完成 DF 任务。对于 4-8 岁的儿童来说,计算聚类和转换测量对于研究 DF 表现的认知过程价值有限,仅次于正确设计总数。在任何DF结果上都没有性别差异。父母的教育水平(LPE)与设计正确总数和转换呈正相关。
{"title":"Young children's performance on a design fluency task: longitudinal data on total number of designs, clustering and switching, and regression-based norms.","authors":"Christine Resch, Esther Keulers, Rosa Martens, Gerard van Breukelen, Caroline M van Heugten, Wenying Hou, Petra P M Hurks","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Design fluency (DF) tasks are commonly used to assess executive functions such as attentional control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and strategy use. Next to the total number of correct designs, the standard outcome of a DF task, clustering and switching can help disentangle the processes underlying DF performance. We present the first longitudinal study of 4-8-year-old children's developmental DF trajectories.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>At initial enrollment, children (<i>n</i> = 228) were aged between 4.05 and 6.88 years (<i>M</i> = 5.18, SD = 0.59) and attended Dutch primary schools. The DF task was administered at three time points, each time point separated by approximately 1 year. Data were analyzed using mixed regression for total number of correct designs and switching, and mixed logistic regression analysis for clustering.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The total number of correct designs increased linearly across the three time points. Across all time points, children made very few clusters, and most clusters consisted of only 3 designs. Clustering only increased at the third assessment compared to the two previous assessments. Switching increased up to the second assessment, but not after that. The number of switches was highly correlated with the total number of correct designs at all time points (<i>r</i> = 0.78 to <i>r</i> = 0.85). These developmental trajectories were similar for all children regardless of their baseline age. Normative data are given for the total number of correct designs and switching.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Children as of age 4 onwards can perform a DF task. For children as young as 4-8 years old, computing clustering, and switching measures is of limited value to study cognitive processes underlying DF performance, next to the total number of correct designs. There were no sex differences on any of the DF outcomes. Level of parental education (LPE) was positively associated with the total number of correct designs and switching.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141247756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prospective memory is associated with aspects of disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy. 前瞻性记忆与癫痫患者的残疾和生活质量有关。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213
Yosefa A Modiano, Steven Paul Woods

Introduction: Episodic memory disruptions in epilepsy stem from shared neurocircuitry. While prior research has focused on retrospective memory (RM), prospective memory (PM; i.e. remembering to remember) also deserves consideration given its critical role in the management of daily activities. The current investigation assessed whether PM is associated with disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.

Methods: This cross-sectional, correlational study included a consecutive series of 50 people with epilepsy presenting for neuropsychological evaluation who completed the Royal Prince Alfred Prospective Memory Test (RPA) and Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) and 63 demographically comparable healthy adults. The participants with epilepsy also completed clinical measures of neuropsychological ability and questionnaires assessing disability and quality of life.

Results: People with epilepsy had significantly more frequent memory symptoms as compared to healthy adults at a very large effect size. Worse mood was associated with lower PM ability at a medium effect size and more frequent PM symptoms at a large effect size. A hierarchical linear regression indicated that PM explained 52% of the variance in disability and 43% of the variance in quality of life after accounting for RM ability.

Conclusions: PM is associated with poorer everyday functioning among people with epilepsy and shows evidence of incremental value beyond RM ability in that regard. Future studies are needed to understand the complex pathways from PM to functional limitations to inform clinical intervention.

简介癫痫患者的外显记忆障碍源于共同的神经回路。以前的研究主要集中在回顾性记忆(RM)上,而前瞻性记忆(PM,即记住要记住的事)在日常活动管理中起着至关重要的作用,因此也值得考虑。目前的调查评估了前瞻性记忆是否与癫痫患者的残疾和生活质量有关:这项横断面相关性研究连续收集了 50 名接受神经心理学评估的癫痫患者,他们完成了皇家阿尔弗雷德王子前瞻性记忆测试(RPA)和前瞻性与回顾性记忆问卷(PRMQ),以及 63 名在人口统计学上具有可比性的健康成年人。癫痫患者还完成了神经心理能力临床测量以及残疾和生活质量评估问卷:结果:与健康成年人相比,癫痫患者的记忆症状明显更频繁,影响程度非常大。在中等效应水平上,情绪不佳与较低的记忆能力有关,在较大效应水平上,与更频繁的记忆症状有关。分层线性回归表明,在考虑RM能力后,PM可解释52%的残疾差异和43%的生活质量差异:PM 与癫痫患者较差的日常功能相关,并有证据表明,PM 在这方面具有超越 RM 能力的增量价值。未来的研究需要了解从 PM 到功能限制的复杂路径,以便为临床干预提供依据。
{"title":"Prospective memory is associated with aspects of disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.","authors":"Yosefa A Modiano, Steven Paul Woods","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Episodic memory disruptions in epilepsy stem from shared neurocircuitry. While prior research has focused on retrospective memory (RM), prospective memory (PM; i.e. remembering to remember) also deserves consideration given its critical role in the management of daily activities. The current investigation assessed whether PM is associated with disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>This cross-sectional, correlational study included a consecutive series of 50 people with epilepsy presenting for neuropsychological evaluation who completed the Royal Prince Alfred Prospective Memory Test (RPA) and Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) and 63 demographically comparable healthy adults. The participants with epilepsy also completed clinical measures of neuropsychological ability and questionnaires assessing disability and quality of life.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>People with epilepsy had significantly more frequent memory symptoms as compared to healthy adults at a very large effect size. Worse mood was associated with lower PM ability at a medium effect size and more frequent PM symptoms at a large effect size. A hierarchical linear regression indicated that PM explained 52% of the variance in disability and 43% of the variance in quality of life after accounting for RM ability.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PM is associated with poorer everyday functioning among people with epilepsy and shows evidence of incremental value beyond RM ability in that regard. Future studies are needed to understand the complex pathways from PM to functional limitations to inform clinical intervention.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140849174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social cognition in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. 1 型神经纤维瘤病儿童的社会认知能力。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-28 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214
Julie Remaud, Jérémy Besnard, Sébastien Barbarot, Arnaud Roy

Introduction: Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic pathology that can lead to impaired social functioning that has a negative impact on patients' quality of life. To date, although the hypothesis of impaired social cognition has been proposed as a potential explanation for these difficulties, very few studies have focused on theory of mind in children with NF1. Furthermore, other complex sociocognitive abilities have never been investigated. The aim of the present study was to assess theory of mind, moral reasoning, and social information processing in children with NF1 compared with a control group.

Method: We administered the Paediatric Evaluation of Emotions, Relationships and Socialization® to 38 children with NF1 aged between 8 years and 16 years 11 months (mean = 11.4, SD = 2.3) and 43 control children with comparable sociodemographic characteristics.

Results: Patients performed significantly worse than controls on moral reasoning and social information processing tests, but there was no significant difference on theory of mind.

Conclusions: These results seem to confirm the presence of social cognition difficulties in NF1 that could explain, at least in part, their social difficulties, although not all dimensions are concerned. The differences between the processes we assessed are discussed in relation to the methodologies used to measure them, and raises questions about the complementarity of traditional tools and more ecological assessments.

简介神经纤维瘤病 1 型(NF1)是一种遗传性疾病,可导致社交功能受损,对患者的生活质量产生负面影响。迄今为止,尽管人们提出了社会认知能力受损的假说,作为这些困难的潜在解释,但很少有研究关注 NF1 儿童的心智理论。此外,对其他复杂的社会认知能力也从未进行过调查。本研究旨在评估 NF1 儿童与对照组相比在心智理论、道德推理和社会信息处理方面的能力:我们对 38 名年龄在 8 岁至 16 岁 11 个月之间的 NF1 儿童(平均值 = 11.4,标准差 = 2.3)和 43 名社会人口学特征相当的对照组儿童进行了儿科情感、关系和社会化评估®:结果:患者在道德推理和社会信息处理测试中的表现明显差于对照组,但在思维理论方面没有明显差异:这些结果似乎证实了 NF1 患儿存在社会认知障碍,至少可以部分解释他们的社交障碍,尽管并非涉及所有方面。我们所评估的过程之间的差异与测量这些过程所使用的方法有关,并提出了传统工具与更生态的评估是否互补的问题。
{"title":"Social cognition in children with neurofibromatosis type 1.","authors":"Julie Remaud, Jérémy Besnard, Sébastien Barbarot, Arnaud Roy","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic pathology that can lead to impaired social functioning that has a negative impact on patients' quality of life. To date, although the hypothesis of impaired social cognition has been proposed as a potential explanation for these difficulties, very few studies have focused on theory of mind in children with NF1. Furthermore, other complex sociocognitive abilities have never been investigated. The aim of the present study was to assess theory of mind, moral reasoning, and social information processing in children with NF1 compared with a control group.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We administered the Paediatric Evaluation of Emotions, Relationships and Socialization® to 38 children with NF1 aged between 8 years and 16 years 11 months (mean = 11.4, <i>SD</i> = 2.3) and 43 control children with comparable sociodemographic characteristics.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Patients performed significantly worse than controls on moral reasoning and social information processing tests, but there was no significant difference on theory of mind.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These results seem to confirm the presence of social cognition difficulties in NF1 that could explain, at least in part, their social difficulties, although not all dimensions are concerned. The differences between the processes we assessed are discussed in relation to the methodologies used to measure them, and raises questions about the complementarity of traditional tools and more ecological assessments.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An evaluation of the moderating effects of routine and busyness on the relationship between prospective memory and everyday functioning in older persons with HIV disease. 评估例行公事和繁忙程度对患有艾滋病的老年人的前瞻性记忆和日常功能之间关系的调节作用。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577
Dhruvi M Mehta, Steven Paul Woods, Nneka Akpotaire

Introduction: People living with HIV (PLWH) often experience difficulties in everyday functioning, which can arise in part from deficits in the strategic/executive aspects of prospective memory (PM). Using Suchy's Contextually Valid Executive Assessment (ConVExA) framework, this study sought to determine whether the contextual factors of busyness and routine moderate the relationship between the strategic/executive aspects of PM and everyday functioning in older PLWH.

Methods: Participants in this cross-sectional analysis were 145 PLWH aged 50 years and older who had completed the Martin and Park Environmental Demands (MPED) questionnaire of routine and busyness, the performance-based Cambridge Test of Prospective Memory, and self-report measures of activities of daily living (ADLs) and cognitive symptoms in daily life.

Results: Multiple regression analyses covarying for relevant comorbidities showed that higher levels of busyness - but not routine - were associated with more frequent cognitive symptoms in daily life. Neither busyness nor routine interacted with PM in association with cognitive symptoms. However, routine and a strategic/executive measure of PM interacted in predicting ADLs; specifically, the association between time-based PM and ADLs was stronger in persons with higher levels of routine in their daily lives. Parallel analyses with less executively-demanding event-based PM were null and small.

Conclusions: Overall, findings provided mixed - and unexpected - evidence for the associations between contextual factors (i.e. routine and busyness), everyday functioning, and PM in this sample of older adults with HIV disease. Results and clinical implications are interpreted and discussed in the framework of the ConVExA model.

导言:艾滋病病毒感染者(PLWH)在日常生活中经常会遇到困难,这可能部分源于前瞻性记忆(PM)的策略/执行方面的缺陷。本研究采用 Suchy 的情境有效执行评估(ConVExA)框架,试图确定忙碌和日常事务等情境因素是否会缓和老年艾滋病病毒感染者前瞻性记忆的策略/执行方面与日常功能之间的关系:这项横断面分析的参与者是 145 名 50 岁及以上的 PLWH,他们完成了马丁和帕克环境需求(MPED)常规和忙碌程度问卷、基于表现的剑桥前瞻性记忆测试,以及日常生活活动(ADLs)和日常生活认知症状的自我报告测量:与相关合并症共同作用的多元回归分析表明,较高程度的忙碌与日常生活中更频繁出现的认知症状有关,但与常规症状无关。在认知症状与 PM 的关系方面,忙碌程度和日常工作都没有相互作用。然而,例行工作和策略性/执行性工作的衡量标准在预测日常生活自理能力方面是相互影响的;特别是,日常生活例行工作水平较高的人,其基于时间的工作与日常生活自理能力之间的关联性更强。对执行要求较低的基于事件的个人生活方式进行的平行分析结果为零,且结果很小:总体而言,研究结果提供了不同的--也是出乎意料的--证据,证明了在感染艾滋病病毒的老年人样本中,环境因素(即常规和繁忙程度)、日常功能和PM之间的关联。在 ConVExA 模型的框架内对结果和临床意义进行了解释和讨论。
{"title":"An evaluation of the moderating effects of routine and busyness on the relationship between prospective memory and everyday functioning in older persons with HIV disease.","authors":"Dhruvi M Mehta, Steven Paul Woods, Nneka Akpotaire","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>People living with HIV (PLWH) often experience difficulties in everyday functioning, which can arise in part from deficits in the strategic/executive aspects of prospective memory (PM). Using Suchy's Contextually Valid Executive Assessment (ConVExA) framework, this study sought to determine whether the contextual factors of busyness and routine moderate the relationship between the strategic/executive aspects of PM and everyday functioning in older PLWH.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Participants in this cross-sectional analysis were 145 PLWH aged 50 years and older who had completed the Martin and Park Environmental Demands (MPED) questionnaire of routine and busyness, the performance-based Cambridge Test of Prospective Memory, and self-report measures of activities of daily living (ADLs) and cognitive symptoms in daily life.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Multiple regression analyses covarying for relevant comorbidities showed that higher levels of busyness - but not routine - were associated with more frequent cognitive symptoms in daily life. Neither busyness nor routine interacted with PM in association with cognitive symptoms. However, routine and a strategic/executive measure of PM interacted in predicting ADLs; specifically, the association between time-based PM and ADLs was stronger in persons with higher levels of routine in their daily lives. Parallel analyses with less executively-demanding event-based PM were null and small.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Overall, findings provided mixed - and unexpected - evidence for the associations between contextual factors (i.e. routine and busyness), everyday functioning, and PM in this sample of older adults with HIV disease. Results and clinical implications are interpreted and discussed in the framework of the ConVExA model.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11309909/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward a cross-cultural understanding of intraindividual variability metrics. 实现对个体内部变异度量的跨文化理解。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870
Bonnie M Scott, Donald R Royall, Jared F Benge, Robin C Hilsabeck

Objective: Compare the construct validity and predictive utility of cognitive intraindividual variability (IIV) in a sample of community-dwelling Hispanic and non-Hispanic white (NHW) older adults.

Methods: The present study included annual data from 651 older adult control participants (Hispanic = 293; NHW = 358) enrolled in the Texas Alzheimer's Research and Care Consortium for at least 5 years. Mean composite z-scores were calculated for attention, language, memory, and executive domains. IIV was calculated as was the standard deviation both within (IIV-Within) and between (IIV-Between) these domains.

Results: At baseline, NHW individuals obtained significantly higher mean scores in each domain than their Hispanic counterparts. They also showed significantly greater variability within and between domains, except for IIV-Within the language domain which was significantly larger in the Hispanic group. IIV-Between domains was driven primarily by IIV-Within the executive function domain in the NHW cohort and by IIV-Within the language domain in the Hispanic cohort. In both groups, the addition of IIV-Within and IIV-Between cognitive domains at baseline significantly improved prediction of global cognitive status after 5 years above and beyond demographic characteristics, genetic and cardiovascular risk. However, IIV-Between domains was the strongest predictor in the NHW group, while IIV-Within the attention domain was the strongest predictor in the Hispanic group.

Conclusions: Findings suggest that, while IIV-Between domains is a promising adjunctive method for predicting future cognitive decline, its construct validity and predictive utility varies based on ethnic group.

目的比较在社区居住的西班牙裔和非西班牙裔白人(NHW)老年人样本中认知个体内变异性(IIV)的构建有效性和预测效用:本研究纳入了 651 名老年人对照组参与者(西班牙裔 = 293 人;非西班牙裔白人 = 358 人)的年度数据,这些参与者已加入德克萨斯州阿尔茨海默氏症研究与护理联合会至少 5 年。计算了注意力、语言、记忆和执行领域的平均综合 Z 值。计算了这些领域内(IIV-Within)和领域间(IIV-Between)的标准偏差:结果:基线时,非华裔女性在每个领域的平均得分都明显高于西班牙裔女性。他们在各领域内和领域之间的变异性也明显更大,只有语言领域内的 IIV 变异性在西班牙裔群体中明显更大。在非华裔组群中,执行功能领域内的 IIV 主要驱动了领域间的 IIV,而在西班牙裔组群中,语言领域内的 IIV 主要驱动了领域间的 IIV。在这两组人群中,在基线时增加 IIV-Within 和 IIV-Between 认知功能域可显著提高对 5 年后总体认知状况的预测能力,而不局限于人口统计学特征、遗传和心血管风险。然而,IIV-Between 领域对非高危人群的预测作用最强,而 IIV-Within 的注意力领域对西班牙裔人群的预测作用最强:研究结果表明,虽然 IIV-Between domains 是预测未来认知能力下降的一种很有前景的辅助方法,但其构建有效性和预测效用因种族群体而异。
{"title":"Toward a cross-cultural understanding of intraindividual variability metrics.","authors":"Bonnie M Scott, Donald R Royall, Jared F Benge, Robin C Hilsabeck","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Compare the construct validity and predictive utility of cognitive intraindividual variability (IIV) in a sample of community-dwelling Hispanic and non-Hispanic white (NHW) older adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The present study included annual data from 651 older adult control participants (Hispanic = 293; NHW = 358) enrolled in the Texas Alzheimer's Research and Care Consortium for at least 5 years. Mean composite z-scores were calculated for attention, language, memory, and executive domains. IIV was calculated as was the standard deviation both within (IIV-Within) and between (IIV-Between) these domains.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>At baseline, NHW individuals obtained significantly higher mean scores in each domain than their Hispanic counterparts. They also showed significantly greater variability within and between domains, except for IIV-Within the language domain which was significantly larger in the Hispanic group. IIV-Between domains was driven primarily by IIV-Within the executive function domain in the NHW cohort and by IIV-Within the language domain in the Hispanic cohort. In both groups, the addition of IIV-Within and IIV-Between cognitive domains at baseline significantly improved prediction of global cognitive status after 5 years above and beyond demographic characteristics, genetic and cardiovascular risk. However, IIV-Between domains was the strongest predictor in the NHW group, while IIV-Within the attention domain was the strongest predictor in the Hispanic group.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings suggest that, while IIV-Between domains is a promising adjunctive method for predicting future cognitive decline, its construct validity and predictive utility varies based on ethnic group.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140119620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Patterns of prospective memory errors differ in persons with multiple sclerosis. 多发性硬化症患者的前瞻性记忆错误模式有所不同。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775
Caitlyn A Nguyen, Sarah A Raskin, Aaron P Turner, Zaenab Dhari, Lindsay O Neto, Elizabeth S Gromisch

Introduction: Prospective memory (PM) deficits have been documented in multiple sclerosis (MS). This study aimed to explore the specific types of errors made by persons with MS (PwMS), including differences between PwMS and healthy controls (HC) and PwMS who do and do not have impairments in processing speed and/or verbal learning and memory.

Method: PwMS (n = 111) and HC (n = 75) completed the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), an objective measure of PM that has five types of errors that can be coded (PM failure, task substitution, loss of content, loss of time, and random errors). The number and types of PM errors were calculated for the overall MIST and six subscales, which break down performance by types of delay (2-Minute and 15-Minute), cue (Time and Event), and response (Verbal and Action). Impairment was defined as performing < 1.5 SD on either the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) or Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Bivariate analyses were used to examine group differences, with post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections.

Results: Nearly 93% of PwMS made at least one PM error, compared to 76% of HC (V = .24, p = .001). The most commonly made PM error by PwMS was loss of content errors (45.0%). PwMS made significantly more task substitution errors (26.4% vs. 7.6%, p < .001) and fewer loss of time errors (9.5% vs. 21.2%, p < .001) than HC. Impaired PwMS made more errors than non-impaired PwMS, specifically PM failures on time-based tasks.

Conclusions: PM errors are common in PwMS, particularly when there are longer delays and time-based cues. Not only do PwMS make more errors than demographically similar HC, but they exhibit different cognitive process failures.

导言:多发性硬化症(MS)患者存在前瞻性记忆(PM)缺陷。本研究旨在探讨多发性硬化症患者(PwMS)所犯错误的具体类型,包括多发性硬化症患者与健康对照组(HC)之间的差异,以及多发性硬化症患者在处理速度和/或言语学习与记忆方面有无障碍:PwMS(n=111)和HC(n=75)完成了 "意图记忆测试"(MIST),这是一种客观的PM测量方法,有五种可编码的错误类型(PM失败、任务替代、内容丢失、时间丢失和随机错误)。PM 错误的数量和类型是根据总体 MIST 和六个子量表计算得出的,这六个子量表按延迟类型(2 分钟和 15 分钟)、提示(时间和事件)和反应(语言和动作)进行了细分。障碍被定义为表现结果:近 93% 的 PwMS 至少出现过一次 PM 错误,而 HC 的这一比例为 76% (V = .24, p = .001)。残疾人最常犯的 PM 错误是内容丢失错误(45.0%)。PwMS 犯的任务替代错误要多得多(26.4% 对 7.6%,p p 结论:PM 错误在 PwMS 中很常见,尤其是在有较长的延迟和基于时间的提示时。与人口统计学上相似的 HC 相比,PwMS 不仅会犯更多的错误,而且会表现出不同的认知过程失误。
{"title":"Patterns of prospective memory errors differ in persons with multiple sclerosis.","authors":"Caitlyn A Nguyen, Sarah A Raskin, Aaron P Turner, Zaenab Dhari, Lindsay O Neto, Elizabeth S Gromisch","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Prospective memory (PM) deficits have been documented in multiple sclerosis (MS). This study aimed to explore the specific types of errors made by persons with MS (PwMS), including differences between PwMS and healthy controls (HC) and PwMS who do and do not have impairments in processing speed and/or verbal learning and memory.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>PwMS (<i>n</i> = 111) and HC (<i>n</i> = 75) completed the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), an objective measure of PM that has five types of errors that can be coded (PM failure, task substitution, loss of content, loss of time, and random errors). The number and types of PM errors were calculated for the overall MIST and six subscales, which break down performance by types of delay (2-Minute and 15-Minute), cue (Time and Event), and response (Verbal and Action). Impairment was defined as performing < 1.5 SD on either the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) or Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Bivariate analyses were used to examine group differences, with post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Nearly 93% of PwMS made at least one PM error, compared to 76% of HC (<i>V</i> = .24, <i>p</i> = .001). The most commonly made PM error by PwMS was loss of content errors (45.0%). PwMS made significantly more task substitution errors (26.4% vs. 7.6%, <i>p</i> < .001) and fewer loss of time errors (9.5% vs. 21.2%, <i>p</i> < .001) than HC. Impaired PwMS made more errors than non-impaired PwMS, specifically PM failures on time-based tasks.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PM errors are common in PwMS, particularly when there are longer delays and time-based cues. Not only do PwMS make more errors than demographically similar HC, but they exhibit different cognitive process failures.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring cultural contributions to the neuropsychology of social cognition: the advanced clinical solutions. 探索社会认知神经心理学的文化贡献:高级临床解决方案。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212
Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull

Introduction: Culture and social cognition are deeply intertwined, yet how this rich intersectionality is expressed neuropsychologically remains an important question.

Method: In a convenience sample of 128 young adults (mean age = 24.9 years) recruited from a majority-minority urban university, we examined performance-based neuropsychological measures of social cognition, the Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), in relation to both cultural orientation, as assessed by the Individualism-Collectivism Scale (ICS) and spoken English language, as assessed by the oral word pronunciation measure of the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT4).

Results: Results indicated higher WRAT4 scores correlated with better performance across all ACS-SP measures of social cognition. Controlling for these associations in spoken English, partial correlations linked lower scores across both prosody interpretation and affect naming ACS-SP tasks with a propensity to view social relationships vertically, irrespective of individualistic or collectivistic orientations. Hierarchical regression results showed that cultural orientation and English-language familiarity each specifically and uniquely contributed to ACS-SP performance for matching prosody with facial expressions.

Conclusions: These findings underscore the importance of incorporating and prioritizing both language and cultural factors in neuropsychological studies of social cognition. They may be viewed as offering strong support for expanding the boundaries of the construct of social cognition beyond its current theoretical framework of one that privileges Western, educated, industralized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) values, customs, and epistemologies.

引言文化与社会认知深深地交织在一起,然而这种丰富的交叉性如何在神经心理学上表现出来仍然是一个重要的问题:方法:我们从一所少数族裔占多数的城市大学中方便抽取了 128 名年轻人(平均年龄为 24.9 岁)作为样本,研究了基于神经心理学表现的社会认知测量--高级临床解决方案-社会认知(ACS-SP)--与文化取向(由个人主义-集体主义量表(ICS)评估)和英语口语(由广域成就测验-4(WRAT4)的口语单词发音测量评估)之间的关系:结果表明,WRAT4 分数越高,在 ACS-SP 的所有社会认知测量中的表现就越好。在控制了英语口语中的这些相关性后,部分相关性将ACS-SP任务中较低的拟声词解释和情感命名得分与纵向看待社会关系的倾向联系起来,而与个人主义或集体主义取向无关。分层回归结果表明,文化取向和英语语言熟悉程度分别对ACS-SP中面部表情与拟声词匹配的成绩有特殊和独特的影响:这些发现强调了在社会认知的神经心理学研究中纳入语言和文化因素并将其置于优先地位的重要性。这些研究结果可以被看作是对扩大社会认知结构边界的有力支持,而不是局限于其当前的理论框架,即西方的、受过教育的、工业化的、富裕的和民主的(WEIRD)价值观、习俗和认识论。
{"title":"Exploring cultural contributions to the neuropsychology of social cognition: the advanced clinical solutions.","authors":"Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Culture and social cognition are deeply intertwined, yet how this rich intersectionality is expressed neuropsychologically remains an important question.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a convenience sample of 128 young adults (mean age = 24.9 years) recruited from a majority-minority urban university, we examined performance-based neuropsychological measures of social cognition, the Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), in relation to both cultural orientation, as assessed by the Individualism-Collectivism Scale (ICS) and spoken English language, as assessed by the oral word pronunciation measure of the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT4).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results indicated higher WRAT4 scores correlated with better performance across all ACS-SP measures of social cognition. Controlling for these associations in spoken English, partial correlations linked lower scores across both prosody interpretation and affect naming ACS-SP tasks with a propensity to view social relationships vertically, irrespective of individualistic or collectivistic orientations. Hierarchical regression results showed that cultural orientation and English-language familiarity each specifically and uniquely contributed to ACS-SP performance for matching prosody with facial expressions.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings underscore the importance of incorporating and prioritizing both language and cultural factors in neuropsychological studies of social cognition. They may be viewed as offering strong support for expanding the boundaries of the construct of social cognition beyond its current theoretical framework of one that privileges Western, educated, industralized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) values, customs, and epistemologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140876566","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prospective memory performance in veterans with and without histories of mild traumatic brain injury: effect of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 genotype. 有轻度脑外伤史和无轻度脑外伤史退伍军人的前瞻性记忆表现:载脂蛋白 E (APOE) ε4基因型的影响。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205
Jennifer S Adler, Erin D Ozturk, Victoria C Merritt, Lisa Delano-Wood, Dawn M Schiehser, Mark W Bondi, Monica T Ly, Adan Ton-Loy, Scott F Sorg

Objective: Identifying factors that moderate cognitive outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is crucial. Prospective memory (PM) is a cognitive domain of interest in mTBI recovery as it may be especially sensitive to TBI-related changes. Since studies show that genetic status - particularly possession of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele - can modify PM performance, we investigated associations between mTBI status and APOE-ε4 genotype on PM performance in a well-characterized sample of Veterans with neurotrauma histories.

Methods: 59 Veterans (mTBI = 33, Military Controls [MCs] = 26; age range: 24-50; average years post-injury = 10.41) underwent a structured clinical interview, neuropsychological assessment, and genotyping. The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) measured PM across multiple subscales. ANCOVAs, adjusting for age and posttraumatic stress symptoms, tested the effects of mTBI status (mTBI vs. MC) and ε4 status (ε4+ vs. ε4-) on MIST scores.

Results: Veterans with mTBI history performed more poorly compared to MCs on the MIST 15-min delay (p=.002, ηp2 =.160), Time Cue (p = .003, ηp2 =.157), and PM Total (p = .016, ηp2 =.102). Those with at least one copy of the ε4 allele performed more poorly compared to ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay (p = .011, ηp2 =.113) and PM Total (p = .048, ηp2 = .071). No significant interactions were observed between mTBI and APOE-ε4 status on MIST outcomes (ps>.25). Within the mTBI group, APOE-ε4+ Veterans performed worse than APOE-ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay subscale (p = .031, ηp2 = .150).

Conclusions: mTBI history and APOE-ε4 genotype status were independently associated with worse PM performance compared to those without head injury histories or possession of the APOE-e4 genotype. Performance on the MIST 15-min delay was worse in Veterans with both risk factors (mTBI history and APOE-ε4 positivity). Findings suggest that genetic status may modify outcomes even in relatively young Veterans with mTBI histories. Future research examining longitudinal associations and links to neuroimaging and biomarker data are needed.

目的:确定影响轻度创伤性脑损伤(mTBI)后认知结果的因素至关重要。前瞻性记忆(PM)是轻微创伤性脑损伤恢复中一个值得关注的认知领域,因为它可能对创伤性脑损伤相关的变化特别敏感。由于研究表明遗传状态--尤其是拥有脂蛋白E(APOE)ε4等位基因--会改变前瞻性记忆的表现,因此我们在具有神经创伤史的退伍军人样本中调查了mTBI状态和APOE-ε4基因型对前瞻性记忆表现的影响。方法:59名退伍军人(mTBI=33人,军事对照组[MCs]=26人;年龄范围:24-50岁;伤后平均年数=10.41年)接受了结构化临床访谈、神经心理学评估和基因分型。意向记忆测试(MIST)通过多个分量表测量 PM。在对年龄和创伤后应激症状进行调整后,方差分析检验了mTBI状态(mTBI vs. MC)和ε4状态(ε4+ vs. ε4-)对MIST得分的影响:与 MCs 相比,有 mTBI 史的退伍军人在 MIST 15 分钟延迟(p=.002,ηp2 =.160)、时间线索(p=.003,ηp2 =.157)和 PM 总分(p=.016,ηp2 =.102)上的表现更差。与ε4-退伍军人相比,至少有一个ε4等位基因拷贝的人在MIST 15分钟延迟(p = .011,ηp2 = .113)和PM总分(p = .048,ηp2 = .071)上的表现更差。mTBI 和 APOE-ε4 状态之间在 MIST 结果上没有观察到明显的交互作用(ps>.25)。在mTBI组中,APOE-ε4+退伍军人在MIST 15分钟延迟分量表上的表现比APOE-ε4-退伍军人差(p = .031,ηp2 = .150)。结论:与没有头部损伤史或拥有APOE-e4基因型的退伍军人相比,mTBI史和APOE-ε4基因型状态与较差的PM表现独立相关。同时具有这两种风险因素(mTBI史和APOE-ε4阳性)的退伍军人在MIST 15分钟延迟中的表现更差。研究结果表明,即使是有 mTBI 史的相对年轻的退伍军人,其遗传状况也可能会改变其结果。未来需要对纵向关联以及与神经影像学和生物标记物数据的联系进行研究。
{"title":"Prospective memory performance in veterans with and without histories of mild traumatic brain injury: effect of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 genotype.","authors":"Jennifer S Adler, Erin D Ozturk, Victoria C Merritt, Lisa Delano-Wood, Dawn M Schiehser, Mark W Bondi, Monica T Ly, Adan Ton-Loy, Scott F Sorg","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Identifying factors that moderate cognitive outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is crucial. Prospective memory (PM) is a cognitive domain of interest in mTBI recovery as it may be especially sensitive to TBI-related changes. Since studies show that genetic status - particularly possession of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele - can modify PM performance, we investigated associations between mTBI status and APOE-ε4 genotype on PM performance in a well-characterized sample of Veterans with neurotrauma histories.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>59 Veterans (mTBI = 33, Military Controls [MCs] = 26; age range: 24-50; average years post-injury = 10.41) underwent a structured clinical interview, neuropsychological assessment, and genotyping. The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) measured PM across multiple subscales. ANCOVAs, adjusting for age and posttraumatic stress symptoms, tested the effects of mTBI status (mTBI vs. MC) and ε4 status (ε4+ vs. ε4-) on MIST scores.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Veterans with mTBI history performed more poorly compared to MCs on the MIST 15-min delay (<i>p</i>=.002, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.160), Time Cue (<i>p</i> = .003, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.157), and PM Total (<i>p</i> = .016, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.102). Those with at least one copy of the ε4 allele performed more poorly compared to ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay (<i>p</i> = .011, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.113) and PM Total (<i>p</i> = .048, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .071). No significant interactions were observed between mTBI and APOE-ε4 status on MIST outcomes (<i>p</i>s>.25). Within the mTBI group, APOE-ε4+ Veterans performed worse than APOE-ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay subscale (<i>p</i> = .031, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .150).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>mTBI history and APOE-ε4 genotype status were independently associated with worse PM performance compared to those without head injury histories or possession of the APOE-e4 genotype. Performance on the MIST 15-min delay was worse in Veterans with both risk factors (mTBI history and APOE-ε4 positivity). Findings suggest that genetic status may modify outcomes even in relatively young Veterans with mTBI histories. Future research examining longitudinal associations and links to neuroimaging and biomarker data are needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140876567","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development and validation of an episodic memory measure in the Mobile Toolbox (MTB): Arranging Pictures. 开发并验证移动工具箱(MTB)中的外显记忆测量方法:排列图片。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945
Stephanie Ruth Young, Elizabeth M Dworak, Miriam A Novack, Aaron J Kaat, Hubert Adam, Cindy J Nowinski, Zahra Hosseinian, Jerry Slotkin, Jordan Stoeger, Saki Amagai, Maria Varela Diaz, Anyelo Almonte Correa, Keith Alperin, Larsson Omberg, Michael Kellen, Monica R Camacho, Bernard Landavazo, Rachel L Nosheny, Michael W Weiner, Richard Gershon

Introduction: Arranging Pictures is a new episodic memory test based on the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Picture Sequence Memory measure and optimized for self-administration on a personal smartphone within the Mobile Toolbox (MTB). We describe evidence from three distinct validation studies.

Method: In Study 1, 92 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures on study-provided smartphones in the lab and were administered external measures of similar and dissimilar constructs by trained examiners to assess validity under controlled circumstances. In Study 2, 1,021 participants completed the external measures in the lab and self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely on their personal smartphones to assess validity in real-world contexts. In Study 3, 141 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely twice with a two-week delay on personal iOS smartphones to assess test-retest reliability and practice effects.

Results: Internal consistency was good across samples (ρxx = .80 to .85, p < .001). Test-retest reliability was marginal (ICC = .49, p < .001) and there were significant practice effects after a two-week delay (ΔM = 3.21 (95% CI [2.56, 3.88]). As expected, correlations with convergent measures were significant and moderate to large in magnitude (ρ = .44 to .76, p < .001), while correlations with discriminant measures were small (ρ = .23 to .27, p < .05) or nonsignificant. Scores demonstrated significant negative correlations with age (ρ = -.32 to -.21, p < .001). Mean performance was slightly higher in the iOS compared to the Android group (MiOS = 18.80, NiOS = 635; MAndroid = 17.11, NAndroid = 386; t(757.73) = 4.17, p < .001), but device type did not significantly influence the psychometric properties of the measure. Indicators of potential cheating were mixed; average scores were significantly higher in the remote samples (F(2, 850) = 11.415, p < .001), but there were not significantly more perfect scores.

Conclusion: The MTB Arranging Pictures measure demonstrated evidence of reliability and validity when self-administered on personal device. Future research should examine the potential for cheating in remote settings and the properties of the measure in clinical samples.

简介排列图片是一种新的外显记忆测试,它以美国国立卫生研究院工具箱(NIHTB)图片序列记忆测量为基础,并经过优化,可在移动工具箱(MTB)内的个人智能手机上进行自我管理。我们介绍了三项不同验证研究的证据:在研究 1 中,92 名参与者在实验室中使用研究人员提供的智能手机进行了 "排列图片 "自我管理,并由训练有素的考官对相似和不相似的结构进行了外部测量,以评估在受控情况下的有效性。在研究 2 中,1,021 名参与者在实验室中完成了外部测量,并在个人智能手机上进行了 "排列图片 "的远程自我管理,以评估在真实环境中的有效性。在研究3中,141名参与者在个人iOS智能手机上进行了2次远程《排列图片》自测,每次延迟两周,以评估重测可靠性和练习效果:结果:不同样本的内部一致性良好(ρxx = .80 至 .85,p p p p iOS = 18.80,NiOS = 635;MAndroid = 17.11,NAndroid = 386;t(757.73) = 4.17,p p p 结论:MTB "排列图片 "测验的内部一致性良好:在个人设备上进行自我管理时,MTB 排列图片测量结果显示了可靠性和有效性。未来的研究应考察在远程环境中作弊的可能性以及该测量方法在临床样本中的特性。
{"title":"Development and validation of an episodic memory measure in the Mobile Toolbox (MTB): Arranging Pictures.","authors":"Stephanie Ruth Young, Elizabeth M Dworak, Miriam A Novack, Aaron J Kaat, Hubert Adam, Cindy J Nowinski, Zahra Hosseinian, Jerry Slotkin, Jordan Stoeger, Saki Amagai, Maria Varela Diaz, Anyelo Almonte Correa, Keith Alperin, Larsson Omberg, Michael Kellen, Monica R Camacho, Bernard Landavazo, Rachel L Nosheny, Michael W Weiner, Richard Gershon","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Arranging Pictures is a new episodic memory test based on the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Picture Sequence Memory measure and optimized for self-administration on a personal smartphone within the Mobile Toolbox (MTB). We describe evidence from three distinct validation studies.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In Study 1, 92 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures on study-provided smartphones in the lab and were administered external measures of similar and dissimilar constructs by trained examiners to assess validity under controlled circumstances. In Study 2, 1,021 participants completed the external measures in the lab and self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely on their personal smartphones to assess validity in real-world contexts. In Study 3, 141 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely twice with a two-week delay on personal iOS smartphones to assess test-retest reliability and practice effects.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Internal consistency was good across samples (ρ<sub>xx</sub> = .80 to .85, <i>p</i> < .001). Test-retest reliability was marginal (ICC = .49, <i>p</i> < .001) and there were significant practice effects after a two-week delay (ΔM = 3.21 (95% CI [2.56, 3.88]). As expected, correlations with convergent measures were significant and moderate to large in magnitude (ρ = .44 to .76, <i>p</i> < .001), while correlations with discriminant measures were small (ρ = .23 to .27, <i>p</i> < .05) or nonsignificant. Scores demonstrated significant negative correlations with age (ρ = -.32 to -.21, <i>p</i> < .001). Mean performance was slightly higher in the iOS compared to the Android group (M<sub>iOS</sub> = 18.80, N<sub>iOS</sub> = 635; M<sub>Android</sub> = 17.11, N<sub>Android</sub> = 386; t(757.73) = 4.17, <i>p</i> < .001), but device type did not significantly influence the psychometric properties of the measure. Indicators of potential cheating were mixed; average scores were significantly higher in the remote samples (F(2, 850) = 11.415, <i>p</i> < .001), but there were not significantly more perfect scores.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The MTB Arranging Pictures measure demonstrated evidence of reliability and validity when self-administered on personal device. Future research should examine the potential for cheating in remote settings and the properties of the measure in clinical samples.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11309919/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140957318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1