首页 > 最新文献

Journal of memory and language最新文献

英文 中文
Word length and frequency effects on text reading are highly similar in 12 alphabetic languages 在 12 种字母语言中,词长和词频对文本阅读的影响高度相似
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104497
Victor Kuperman , Sascha Schroeder , Daniil Gnetov

Reading research robustly finds that shorter and more frequent words are recognized faster and skipped more often than longer and less frequent words. An empirical question that has not been tested yet is whether languages within the same writing system would produce similarly strong length and frequency effects or whether typological differences between written languages would cause those effects to vary systematically in their magnitude. We analyzed text reading eye-movement data in 12 alphabetic languages from the Multilingual Eye-Movement Corpus (MECO). The languages varied substantially in their word length and frequency distributions as a function of their orthographic depth and morpho-syntactic type. Yet, the effects of word length and frequency on fixation durations and skipping rate were highly similar in size between the languages. This finding suggests a high degree of cross-linguistic universality in the readers’ behavioral response to linguistic complexity (indexed by word length) and the amount of experience with the word (indexed by word frequency). These findings run counter to influential theories of single word recognition, which predict orthographic depth of a language to modulate the size of these benchmark effects. They also facilitate development of cross-linguistically generalizable computational models of eye-movement control in reading.

阅读研究有力地证明,较短和频率较高的单词比较长和频率较低的单词识别速度更快,跳读次数更多。一个尚未检验过的实证问题是,同一种书写系统中的语言是否会产生类似强烈的长度和频率效应,或者不同书写语言之间的类型差异是否会导致这些效应在大小上的系统性差异。我们分析了多语言眼动语料库(MECO)中 12 种字母语言的文本阅读眼动数据。作为正字法深度和形态句法类型的函数,这些语言的词长和词频分布差异很大。然而,词长和词频对固定时长和跳读率的影响在大小上却非常相似。这一发现表明,读者对语言复杂性(以词的长度为指标)和词的经验量(以词的频率为指标)的行为反应具有高度的跨语言普遍性。这些发现与有影响力的单词识别理论背道而驰,后者预测语言的正字法深度会调节这些基准效应的大小。这些发现还促进了阅读中眼动控制的跨语言通用计算模型的发展。
{"title":"Word length and frequency effects on text reading are highly similar in 12 alphabetic languages","authors":"Victor Kuperman ,&nbsp;Sascha Schroeder ,&nbsp;Daniil Gnetov","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104497","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104497","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Reading research robustly finds that shorter and more frequent words are recognized faster and skipped more often than longer and less frequent words. An empirical question that has not been tested yet is whether languages within the same writing system would produce similarly strong length and frequency effects or whether typological differences between written languages would cause those effects to vary systematically in their magnitude. We analyzed text reading eye-movement data in 12 alphabetic languages from the Multilingual Eye-Movement Corpus (MECO). The languages varied substantially in their word length and frequency distributions as a function of their orthographic depth and morpho-syntactic type. Yet, the effects of word length and frequency on fixation durations and skipping rate were highly similar in size between the languages. This finding suggests a high degree of cross-linguistic universality in the readers’ behavioral response to linguistic complexity (indexed by word length) and the amount of experience with the word (indexed by word frequency). These findings run counter to influential theories of single word recognition, which predict orthographic depth of a language to modulate the size of these benchmark effects. They also facilitate development of cross-linguistically generalizable computational models of eye-movement control in reading.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138819758","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
SEAM: An integrated activation-coupled model of sentence processing and eye movements in reading SEAM:阅读中句子加工和眼球运动的激活耦合综合模型
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104496
Maximilian M. Rabe , Dario Paape , Daniela Mertzen , Shravan Vasishth , Ralf Engbert

Models of eye-movement control during reading, developed largely within psychology, usually focus on visual, attentional, lexical, and motor processes but neglect post-lexical language processing; by contrast, models of sentence comprehension processes, developed largely within psycholinguistics, generally focus only on post-lexical language processes. We present a model that combines these two research threads, by integrating eye-movement control and sentence processing. Developing such an integrated model is extremely challenging and computationally demanding, but such an integration is an important step toward complete mathematical models of natural language comprehension in reading. We combine the SWIFT model of eye-movement control (Seelig et al., 2023) with key components of the Lewis and Vasishth sentence processing model (Lewis and Vasishth, 2005). This integration becomes possible, for the first time, due in part to recent advances in successful parameter identification in dynamical models, which allows us to investigate profile log-likelihoods for individual model parameters. We present a fully implemented proof-of-concept model demonstrating how such an integrated model can be achieved; our approach includes Bayesian model inference with Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling as a key computational tool. The integrated Sentence-Processing and Eye-Movement Activation-Coupled Model (SEAM) can successfully reproduce eye movement patterns that arise due to similarity-based interference in reading. To our knowledge, this is the first-ever integration of a complete process model of eye-movement control with linguistic dependency completion processes in sentence comprehension. In future work, this proof of concept model will need to be evaluated using a comprehensive set of benchmark data.

阅读过程中的眼动控制模型主要由心理学发展而来,通常侧重于视觉、注意、词汇和运动过程,但忽略了词汇后的语言处理过程;相比之下,句子理解过程模型主要由心理语言学发展而来,通常只侧重于词汇后的语言处理过程。通过整合眼动控制和句子处理过程,我们提出了一个将这两个研究方向结合起来的模型。开发这样一个综合模型极具挑战性,对计算要求很高,但这种整合是迈向阅读中自然语言理解完整数学模型的重要一步。我们将 SWIFT 眼动控制模型(Seelig 等人,2023 年)与 Lewis 和 Vasishth 句子处理模型(Lewis 和 Vasishth,2005 年)的关键部分结合起来。这种整合首次成为可能,部分原因是最近在动力学模型的成功参数识别方面取得了进展,使我们能够研究单个模型参数的剖面对数似然。我们提出了一个全面实施的概念验证模型,展示了如何实现这种集成模型;我们的方法包括贝叶斯模型推理,并将马尔可夫链蒙特卡罗(MCMC)采样作为一种关键的计算工具。集成的句子处理和眼动激活耦合模型(SEAM)可以成功地再现阅读中由于相似性干扰而产生的眼动模式。据我们所知,这是首次将眼动控制的完整过程模型与句子理解中的语言依赖完成过程整合在一起。在未来的工作中,这一概念验证模型将需要使用一套全面的基准数据进行评估。
{"title":"SEAM: An integrated activation-coupled model of sentence processing and eye movements in reading","authors":"Maximilian M. Rabe ,&nbsp;Dario Paape ,&nbsp;Daniela Mertzen ,&nbsp;Shravan Vasishth ,&nbsp;Ralf Engbert","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104496","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104496","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Models of eye-movement control during reading, developed largely within psychology, usually focus on visual, attentional, lexical, and motor processes but neglect post-lexical language processing; by contrast, models of sentence comprehension processes, developed largely within psycholinguistics, generally focus only on post-lexical language processes. We present a model that combines these two research threads, by integrating eye-movement control and sentence processing. Developing such an integrated model is extremely challenging and computationally demanding, but such an integration is an important step toward complete mathematical models of natural language comprehension in reading. We combine the SWIFT model of eye-movement control (Seelig et al., 2023) with key components of the Lewis and Vasishth sentence processing model (Lewis and Vasishth, 2005). This integration becomes possible, for the first time, due in part to recent advances in successful parameter identification in dynamical models, which allows us to investigate profile log-likelihoods for individual model parameters. We present a fully implemented proof-of-concept model demonstrating how such an integrated model can be achieved; our approach includes Bayesian model inference with Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling as a key computational tool. The integrated Sentence-Processing and Eye-Movement Activation-Coupled Model (SEAM) can successfully reproduce eye movement patterns that arise due to similarity-based interference in reading. To our knowledge, this is the first-ever integration of a complete process model of eye-movement control with linguistic dependency completion processes in sentence comprehension. In future work, this proof of concept model will need to be evaluated using a comprehensive set of benchmark data.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X23000955/pdfft?md5=e957b46c6324edb315808fc2e1ef2d43&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X23000955-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138820006","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do readers here what they sea?: Effects of lexicality, predictability, and individual differences on the phonological preview benefit 读者在这里看到的是什么?词性、可预测性和个体差异对语音预览益处的影响
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104480
Sara Milligan, Elizabeth R. Schotter

For decades, researchers have debated whether readers benefit from translating visual word forms into phonological codes. A focus of this debate has been on the earliest moments of processing when a word is perceived in parafoveal vision (i.e., phonological preview benefit). A recent meta-analysis (Vasilev et al., 2019) concluded that the phonological preview benefit may be small and unreliable but they did not take into account potentially important stimulus-level or participant-level factors that varied across the included studies. Therefore, we conducted two well-powered experiments that systematically investigated the effects of sentence constraint, preview lexicality, and participant language skills on the phonological preview benefit effect. We found phonological preview benefits that were larger in high versus low constraint sentences, larger for words than pseudowords, and larger for better spellers. We conclude that phonological codes do facilitate early word recognition during reading, but that the phonological preview benefit magnitude depends on subject- and stimulus-level factors.

几十年来,研究人员一直在争论读者是否能从将视觉词形转化为语音编码中获益。这一争论的焦点一直集中在视网膜旁视觉感知到单词时的最初处理时刻(即语音预览益处)。最近的一项荟萃分析(Vasilev et al., 2019)认为,语音预览益处可能很小,而且不可靠,但他们并没有考虑到潜在的重要刺激水平或参与者水平因素,而这些因素在所纳入的研究中各不相同。因此,我们进行了两项有充分证据支持的实验,系统地研究了句子限制、预览词性和参与者语言技能对语音预览效益效应的影响。我们发现,在高限制句子和低限制句子中,语音预览的益处更大,单词的益处大于伪词,拼写能力更强的人的益处更大。我们的结论是,语音编码确实有助于阅读过程中的早期单词识别,但语音预览效益的大小取决于主体和刺激水平因素。
{"title":"Do readers here what they sea?: Effects of lexicality, predictability, and individual differences on the phonological preview benefit","authors":"Sara Milligan,&nbsp;Elizabeth R. Schotter","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104480","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104480","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>For decades, researchers have debated whether readers benefit from translating visual word forms into phonological codes. A focus of this debate has been on the earliest moments of processing when a word is perceived in parafoveal vision (i.e., phonological preview benefit). A recent meta-analysis (<span>Vasilev et al., 2019</span>) concluded that the phonological preview benefit may be small and unreliable but they did not take into account potentially important stimulus-level or participant-level factors that varied across the included studies. Therefore, we conducted two well-powered experiments that systematically investigated the effects of sentence constraint, preview lexicality, and participant language skills on the phonological preview benefit effect. We found phonological preview benefits that were larger in high versus low constraint sentences, larger for words than pseudowords, and larger for better spellers. We conclude that phonological codes do facilitate early word recognition during reading, but that the phonological preview benefit magnitude depends on subject- and stimulus-level factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138549960","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexically-specific syntactic restrictions in second-language speakers 第二语言使用者的词汇特定句法限制
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104470
Mariana Vega-Mendoza , Iva Ivanova , Janet F. McLean , Martin J. Pickering , Holly P. Branigan

In two structural priming experiments, we investigated the representations of lexically-specific syntactic restrictions of English verbs for highly proficient and immersed second language (L2) speakers of English. We considered the interplay of two possible mechanisms: generalization from the first language (L1) and statistical learning within the L2 (both of abstract structure and of lexically-specific information). In both experiments, L2 speakers with either Germanic or Romance languages as L1 were primed to produce dispreferred double-object structures involving non-alternating dative verbs. Priming occurred from ungrammatical double-object primes involving different non-alternating verbs (Experiment 1) and from grammatical primes involving alternating verbs (Experiment 2), supporting abstract statistical learning within the L2. However, we found no differences between L1-Germanic speakers (who have the double-object structure in their L1) and L1-Romance speakers (who do not), inconsistent with the prediction for between-group differences of the L1-generalization account. Additionally, L2 speakers in Experiment 2 showed a lexical boost: There was stronger priming after (dispreferred) non-alternating same-verb double-object primes than after (grammatical) alternating different-verb primes. Such lexically-driven persistence was also shown by L1 English speakers (Ivanova, Pickering, McLean, Costa, & Branigan, 2012) and may underlie statistical learning of lexically-dependent structural regularities. We conclude that lexically-specific syntactic restrictions in highly proficient and immersed L2 speakers are shaped by statistical learning (both abstract and lexically-specific) within the L2, but not by generalization from the L1.

在两个结构启动实验中,我们研究了英语高度熟练和沉浸式第二语言(L2)使用者对英语动词词汇特定句法限制的表征。我们考虑了两种可能的机制的相互作用:第一语言(L1)的泛化和第二语言(抽象结构和词汇特定信息)中的统计学习。在这两个实验中,以日耳曼语或罗曼语为母语的第二语言使用者被启动产生不受欢迎的双宾语结构,包括非交替动词。启动来自涉及不同非交替动词的非语法双宾语启动(实验1)和涉及交替动词的语法启动(实验2),支持第二语言的抽象统计学习。然而,我们发现L1-日耳曼语使用者(他们的L1语言中有双宾语结构)和L1-罗曼语使用者(他们的L1语言中没有双宾语结构)之间没有差异,这与L1-概括解释的组间差异预测不一致。此外,实验2中的二语说话者表现出词汇量的提升:在(不喜欢的)非交替的相同动词双宾语启动后,他们的启动比(语法的)交替的不同动词启动后的启动更强。这种词汇驱动的坚持在母语为英语的人身上也有体现(Ivanova, Pickering, McLean, Costa, &Branigan, 2012),并且可能是词汇依赖结构规律的统计学习的基础。我们的结论是,高度精通和沉浸式二语使用者的词汇特异性句法限制是由二语中的统计学习(抽象和词汇特异性)形成的,而不是由母语的概括形成的。
{"title":"Lexically-specific syntactic restrictions in second-language speakers","authors":"Mariana Vega-Mendoza ,&nbsp;Iva Ivanova ,&nbsp;Janet F. McLean ,&nbsp;Martin J. Pickering ,&nbsp;Holly P. Branigan","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104470","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104470","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In two structural priming experiments, we investigated the representations of lexically-specific syntactic restrictions of English verbs for highly proficient and immersed second language (L2) speakers of English. We considered the interplay of two possible mechanisms: generalization from the first language (L1) and statistical learning within the L2 (both of abstract structure and of lexically-specific information). In both experiments, L2 speakers with either Germanic or Romance languages as L1 were primed to produce dispreferred double-object structures involving non-alternating dative verbs. Priming occurred from ungrammatical double-object primes involving different non-alternating verbs (Experiment 1) and from grammatical primes involving alternating verbs (Experiment 2), supporting abstract statistical learning within the L2. However, we found no differences between L1-Germanic speakers (who have the double-object structure in their L1) and L1-Romance speakers (who do not), inconsistent with the prediction for between-group differences of the L1-generalization account. Additionally, L2 speakers in Experiment 2 showed a lexical boost: There was stronger priming after (dispreferred) non-alternating same-verb double-object primes than after (grammatical) alternating different-verb primes. Such lexically-driven persistence was also shown by L1 English speakers (Ivanova, Pickering, McLean, Costa, &amp; Branigan, 2012) and may underlie statistical learning of lexically-dependent structural regularities. We conclude that lexically-specific syntactic restrictions in highly proficient and immersed L2 speakers are shaped by statistical learning (both abstract and lexically-specific) within the L2, but not by generalization from the L1.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X23000694/pdfft?md5=68cbe1c90d4cd5aa4b5afbc2cb46cbfb&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X23000694-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138448500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language comprehenders are sensitive to multiple states of semantically similar objects 语言理解者对语义相似对象的多种状态很敏感
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104478
Oleksandr V. Horchak, Margarida V. Garrido

The present research shows that language comprehenders are sensitive to multiple states of target and semantically related objects. In Experiments 1 to 2B, participants (total N = 273) read sentences that either implied a minimal change of an object’s state (e.g., “Jane chose a mango”) or a substantial change (e.g., “Jane stepped on a mango”) and then verified whether a subsequently pictured object was mentioned in the sentence. Crucially, the picture either showed the original/modified state of an object that was mentioned in the sentence (e.g., “mango” in Experiment 1) or not (e.g., “banana” in Experiments 2A and 2B). The results of Experiment 1 demonstrated that the objects in a modified state were verified faster when a sentence implied a substantial state-change rather than a minimal state-change. In contrast, the reverse was true for the objects in the original state. Importantly, verification latencies of pictures depicting original and modified states of an object in the substantial state-change condition were approximately similar, thus suggesting that language comprehenders maintain multiple representations of an object in different states. The results of Experiments 2A and 2B revealed that when participants had to indicate that a pictured object (e.g., banana) was not mentioned in the sentence, their verification latencies were slowed down when the sentence contained a semantically related item (e.g., mango) and described this item as being changed substantially by the action. However, these verification latencies varied continuously with the degree of change: the more dissimilar the states of a semantically related item, the less time participants needed to verify a pictured object. The results are discussed through the prism of theories emphasizing dynamic views of event cognition.

目前的研究表明,语言理解者对目标对象和语义相关对象的多种状态都很敏感。在实验1到2B中,参与者(总人数为273人)阅读了一些句子,这些句子要么暗示了物体状态的微小变化(例如,“简选择了一个芒果”),要么暗示了物体状态的重大变化(例如,“简踩到了芒果”),然后验证句子中是否提到了随后被描绘成图片的物体。至关重要的是,图片要么显示了句子中提到的物体的原始/修改状态(例如,实验1中的“芒果”),要么没有(例如,实验2A和2B中的“香蕉”)。实验1的结果表明,当句子暗示了一个实质性的状态变化时,修正状态下的对象被验证的速度比暗示了一个最小的状态变化时要快。相反,对于处于原始状态的对象,情况正好相反。重要的是,描述物体在实质性状态变化条件下的原始状态和修改状态的图片的验证延迟大致相似,这表明语言理解者在不同状态下保持对物体的多重表征。实验2A和2B的结果显示,当参与者必须指出句子中没有提到一个图片物体(如香蕉)时,当句子中包含一个语义相关的项目(如芒果)并将该项目描述为被动作实质上改变时,他们的验证延迟会减慢。然而,这些验证延迟随着变化的程度而不断变化:语义相关项目的状态越不相似,参与者验证图片对象所需的时间越少。结果通过强调事件认知的动态观点的理论棱镜进行讨论。
{"title":"Language comprehenders are sensitive to multiple states of semantically similar objects","authors":"Oleksandr V. Horchak,&nbsp;Margarida V. Garrido","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104478","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104478","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present research shows that language comprehenders are sensitive to multiple states of target and semantically related objects. In Experiments 1 to 2B, participants (total <em>N</em> = 273) read sentences that either implied a minimal change of an object’s state (e.g., “Jane <em>chose</em> a mango”) or a substantial change (e.g., “Jane <em>stepped</em> on a mango”) and then verified whether a subsequently pictured object was mentioned in the sentence. Crucially, the picture either showed the original/modified state of an object that was mentioned in the sentence (e.g., “mango” in Experiment 1) or not (e.g., “banana” in Experiments 2A and 2B). The results of Experiment 1 demonstrated that the objects in a modified state were verified faster when a sentence implied a substantial state-change rather than a minimal state-change. In contrast, the reverse was true for the objects in the original state. Importantly, verification latencies of pictures depicting original and modified states of an object in the substantial state-change condition were approximately similar, thus suggesting that language comprehenders maintain multiple representations of an object in different states. The results of Experiments 2A and 2B revealed that when participants had to indicate that a pictured object (e.g., banana) was not mentioned in the sentence, their verification latencies were slowed down when the sentence contained a semantically related item (e.g., mango) and described this item as being changed substantially by the action. However, these verification latencies varied continuously with the degree of change: the more dissimilar the states of a semantically related item, the less time participants needed to verify a pictured object. The results are discussed through the prism of theories emphasizing dynamic views of event cognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X23000773/pdfft?md5=f159957dfd86618c4d6a6f0b6fa7bd43&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X23000773-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138412312","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moving experimental psychology online: How to obtain high quality data when we can’t see our participants 把实验心理学搬到网上:当我们看不到我们的参与者时,如何获得高质量的数据
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104472
Jennifer M. Rodd

The past 10 years have seen rapid growth of online (web-based) data collection across the behavioural sciences. Despite the many important contributions of such studies, some researchers have concerns about the reduction in experimental control when research moves outside of laboratory conditions. This paper provides an accessible overview of the issues that can adversely affect data quality in online experiments, with particular focus on cognitive studies of memory and language. I provide checklists for researchers setting up such experiments to help improve data quality. These recommendations focus on three key aspects of experimental design: the technology choices made by researchers and participants, participant recruitment methods, and the performance of participants during experiments. I argue that ensuring high data quality for online experiments requires significant effort prior to data collection to maintain the credibility of our rapidly expanding evidence base. With such safeguards in place, online experiments will continue to provide important, paradigm-changing opportunities across the behavioural sciences.

过去10年,行为科学领域的在线(基于网络的)数据收集迅速增长。尽管这些研究做出了许多重要贡献,但一些研究人员担心,当研究转移到实验室条件之外时,实验控制会减少。本文提供了对在线实验中可能对数据质量产生不利影响的问题的可访问概述,特别关注记忆和语言的认知研究。我为建立此类实验的研究人员提供了清单,以帮助提高数据质量。这些建议集中在实验设计的三个关键方面:研究人员和参与者的技术选择、参与者招募方法以及参与者在实验期间的表现。我认为,确保在线实验的高数据质量需要在数据收集之前做出重大努力,以保持我们迅速扩大的证据基础的可信度。有了这些保障措施,在线实验将继续为整个行为科学领域提供重要的、改变范式的机会。
{"title":"Moving experimental psychology online: How to obtain high quality data when we can’t see our participants","authors":"Jennifer M. Rodd","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104472","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104472","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The past 10 years have seen rapid growth of online (web-based) data collection across the behavioural sciences. Despite the many important contributions of such studies, some researchers have concerns about the reduction in experimental control when research moves outside of laboratory conditions. This paper provides an accessible overview of the issues that can adversely affect data quality in online experiments, with particular focus on cognitive studies of memory and language. I provide checklists for researchers setting up such experiments to help improve data quality. These recommendations focus on three key aspects of experimental design: the technology choices made by researchers and participants, participant recruitment methods, and the performance of participants during experiments. I argue that ensuring high data quality for online experiments requires significant effort prior to data collection to maintain the credibility of our rapidly expanding evidence base. With such safeguards in place, online experiments will continue to provide important, paradigm-changing opportunities across the behavioural sciences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X23000712/pdfft?md5=d6e85cf3736e86817a12d398321cfa8f&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X23000712-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138412466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical choice and word formation in a taboo game paradigm 禁忌游戏范式中的词汇选择与构词
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104477
Vasilisa Pugacheva , Fritz Günther

We investigate the onomasiological question of which words speakers actually use and produce when trying to convey an intended meaning. This is not limited to selecting the best-fitting available existing word, but also includes word formation, the coinage of novel words. In the first two experiments, we introduce the taboo game paradigm in which participants were instructed to produce a single-word substitution for different words so that others can later identify them. Using distributional semantic models with the capability to produce quantitative representations for existing and novel word responses, we find that (a) responses tend to be semantically close to the targets and (b) existing words were represented closer than novel words, but (c) even novel compounds were often closer than the targets’ free associates. In a final third experiment, we find that other participants are more likely to guess the correct original word (a) for responses closer to the original targets, and (b) for novel compound responses as compared to existing word responses. This shows that the production of both existing and novel words can be accurately captured in a unified computational framework of the semantic mechanisms driving word choice.

我们调查的象声学问题,哪些词说话者实际使用和产生时,试图传达一个预期的意义。这不仅限于选择最合适的现有单词,还包括构词法,新单词的创造。在前两个实验中,我们引入了禁忌游戏范式,在该范式中,参与者被指示为不同的单词产生一个单词替代,以便其他人稍后能够识别它们。使用分布式语义模型,我们发现(a)响应在语义上倾向于接近目标,(b)现有单词的表示比新单词更接近,但(c)即使是新的复合词也经常比目标的自由关联词更接近。在最后的第三个实验中,我们发现其他参与者更有可能猜出正确的原始单词(a)对于更接近原始目标的反应,以及(b)与现有的单词反应相比,对于新的复合反应。这表明,在驱动词选择的语义机制的统一计算框架中,可以准确地捕获现有词和新词的产生。
{"title":"Lexical choice and word formation in a taboo game paradigm","authors":"Vasilisa Pugacheva ,&nbsp;Fritz Günther","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104477","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104477","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We investigate the onomasiological question of which words speakers actually use and produce when trying to convey an intended meaning. This is not limited to selecting the best-fitting available existing word, but also includes word formation, the coinage of novel words. In the first two experiments, we introduce the taboo game paradigm in which participants were instructed to produce a single-word substitution for different words so that others can later identify them. Using distributional semantic models with the capability to produce quantitative representations for existing and novel word responses, we find that (a) responses tend to be semantically close to the targets and (b) existing words were represented closer than novel words, but (c) even novel compounds were often closer than the targets’ free associates. In a final third experiment, we find that other participants are more likely to guess the correct original word (a) for responses closer to the original targets, and (b) for novel compound responses as compared to existing word responses. This shows that the production of both existing and novel words can be accurately captured in a unified computational framework of the semantic mechanisms driving word choice.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138390009","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interaction between the testing and forward testing effects in the case of Cued-Recall: Implications for Theory, individual difference Studies, and application 提示回忆中测试和正向测试效应的相互作用:理论、个体差异研究和应用的启示
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104476
Mohan W. Gupta , Steven C. Pan , Timothy C. Rickard

Recall from episodic memory has been shown to enhance both memory for the retrieved information (e.g., relative to a restudy control condition; the testing effect, or TE) and memory for different, subsequently studied materials (the forward testing effect, or FTE). Hence, the TE may be subject to an FTE confound when training in a TE experiment involves either testing prior to restudy or when restudied and tested items are randomly mixed. Across two cued-recall TE experiments, we show that (1) a potent FTE confound exists in the test-first but not the mixed training design, (2) there are no other evident learning related interactions between restudied and tested items across three frequently used training phase task orderings, and (3) the predictions of the dual-memory model of test-enhanced learning – which posits that a test trial creates a memory that is separate from the initially encoded study memory, yielding two routes to retrieval for tested items – are held both when there is and is not a confounding FTE. Further, our results yielded no evidence for two accounts of the FTE (the proactive interference and reset of encoding hypotheses) as applied to cued recall but are consistent with two alternative accounts (the strategy change and increasing effort hypotheses). Through distribution analyses we identify a novel and potent FTE individual differences effect that can be accommodated by the latter accounts. Finally, we show that at least three large-n studies exploring individual differences in the TE are confounded by the FTE, compromising conclusions in those papers about the efficacy of the TE across individuals.

从情景记忆中回忆已被证明可以增强对检索信息的记忆(例如,相对于再学习控制条件;对于不同的,随后研究的材料(正向测试效应,或FTE)和记忆。因此,当TE实验中的训练包括重新学习之前的测试或重新学习和测试的项目随机混合时,TE可能会受到FTE混淆的影响。在两个线索回忆TE实验中,我们发现:(1)在测试优先而不是混合训练设计中存在有效的FTE混淆,(2)在三个经常使用的训练阶段任务顺序中,重新学习和被测试项目之间没有其他明显的学习相关交互作用,以及(3)测试增强学习的双记忆模型的预测-假设测试试验创造了与最初编码的学习记忆分开的记忆。在存在和不存在混淆FTE的情况下,产生两种检索测试项目的途径。此外,我们的研究结果没有为FTE的两种解释(主动干扰和编码重置假设)在线索回忆中的应用提供证据,但与两种替代解释(策略变化和增加努力假设)一致。通过分布分析,我们发现了一种新的、有效的FTE个体差异效应,这种效应可以被后一种解释所适应。最后,我们表明,至少有三个探究TE个体差异的大n研究与FTE混淆,损害了这些论文中关于TE在个体间有效性的结论。
{"title":"Interaction between the testing and forward testing effects in the case of Cued-Recall: Implications for Theory, individual difference Studies, and application","authors":"Mohan W. Gupta ,&nbsp;Steven C. Pan ,&nbsp;Timothy C. Rickard","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104476","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104476","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recall from episodic memory has been shown to enhance both memory for the retrieved information (e.g., relative to a restudy control condition; the <em>testing effect, or</em> TE) and memory for different, subsequently studied materials (the <em>forward testing effect, or</em> FTE). Hence, the TE may be subject to an FTE confound when training in a TE experiment involves either testing prior to restudy or when restudied and tested items are randomly mixed. Across two cued-recall TE experiments, we show that (1) a potent FTE confound exists in the test-first but not the mixed training design, (2) there are no other evident learning related interactions between restudied and tested items across three frequently used training phase task orderings, and (3) the predictions of the dual-memory model of test-enhanced learning – which posits that a test trial creates a memory that is separate from the initially encoded study memory, yielding two routes to retrieval for tested items – are held both when there is and is not a confounding FTE. Further, our results yielded no evidence for two accounts of the FTE (the proactive interference and reset of encoding hypotheses) as applied to cued recall but are consistent with two alternative accounts (the strategy change and increasing effort hypotheses). Through distribution analyses we identify a novel and potent FTE individual differences effect that can be accommodated by the latter accounts. Finally, we show that at least three large-<em>n</em> studies exploring individual differences in the TE are confounded by the FTE, compromising conclusions in those papers about the efficacy of the TE across individuals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X2300075X/pdfft?md5=fb987b63056b3e615d24c1ad82319ffa&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X2300075X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134656284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Romanian (subject-like) DPs attract more than bare nouns: Evidence from speeded continuations 罗马尼亚(类主题)dp吸引的不仅仅是单纯的名词:来自快速延续的证据
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104445
Adina Camelia Bleotu , Brian Dillon

This paper investigates whether agreement attraction is modulated by distributional properties determining subject-likelihood by examining the degree to which bare nouns and full determiner phrases (DPs) cause agreement attraction effects in Romanian. Romanian represents an ideal testing ground for this, given two distributional constraints making bare nouns less subject-like: Locative Determiner Omission, preventing locative prepositions from taking nouns with definite articles (unless modified by adjectives), and the Naked Noun Constraint, disallowing bare nouns as preverbal subjects. We predicted that bare nouns should be less likely to trigger agreement attraction than overt DPs. We conducted four speeded forced-choice sentence continuation tasks on Romanian native speakers to test this prediction. We observe that overt DPs cause significantly more attraction than bare nouns. We suggest that the results are consistent with a cue-based retrieval mechanism for forming agreement dependencies, where cues that determine subjecthood are used to reactivate elements in working memory upon processing a verb. These cues can be language specific, and in Romanian, this means that agreement attraction is sensitive to the morphophonological overtness of the determiner.

本文通过考察裸名词和完全限定词短语(dp)在罗马尼亚语中引起协议吸引效应的程度,研究了协议吸引是否受到决定主体可能性的分布属性的调节。罗马尼亚语是一个理想的试验场,给出了两个使裸名词不像主语的分布约束:位置限定词省略,防止位置介词带定冠词(除非被形容词修饰),以及裸名词约束,不允许裸名词作为前语主语。我们预测,与直白的名词相比,直白的名词不太可能引发协议吸引力。我们对以罗马尼亚语为母语的人进行了四个快速强迫选择句子接续任务来测试这一预测。我们观察到明显的DPs比纯粹的名词更有吸引力。我们认为,结果与形成协议依赖的基于线索的检索机制是一致的,在这种机制中,决定主体性的线索被用来在处理动词时重新激活工作记忆中的元素。这些线索可能是特定于语言的,在罗马尼亚语中,这意味着一致吸引对限定词的词音明显性很敏感。
{"title":"Romanian (subject-like) DPs attract more than bare nouns: Evidence from speeded continuations","authors":"Adina Camelia Bleotu ,&nbsp;Brian Dillon","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104445","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104445","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper investigates whether agreement attraction is modulated by distributional properties determining subject-likelihood by examining the degree to which bare nouns and full determiner phrases (DPs) cause agreement attraction effects in Romanian. Romanian represents an ideal testing ground for this, given two distributional constraints making bare nouns less subject-like: <em>Locative Determiner Omission</em>, preventing locative prepositions from taking nouns with definite articles (unless modified by adjectives), and the <em>Naked Noun Constraint</em><span>, disallowing bare nouns as preverbal subjects. We predicted that bare nouns should be less likely to trigger agreement attraction than overt DPs. We conducted four speeded forced-choice sentence continuation tasks on Romanian native speakers to test this prediction. We observe that overt DPs cause significantly more attraction than bare nouns. We suggest that the results are consistent with a cue-based retrieval mechanism for forming agreement dependencies, where cues that determine subjecthood are used to reactivate elements in working memory upon processing a verb. These cues can be language specific, and in Romanian, this means that agreement attraction is sensitive to the morphophonological overtness of the determiner.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"109146069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reading compound words in Finnish and Chinese: An eye-tracking study 芬兰语和汉语复合词阅读:一项眼球追踪研究
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-10-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104474
Jukka Hyönä , Lei Cui , Timo T. Heikkilä , Birgitta Paranko , Yun Gao , Xingzhi Su

Two eye-tracking experiments in alphabetic Finnish and two in logographic Chinese examined the recognition of two-constituent compound words in reading. In Finnish, two-constituent compound words vary greatly in length, whereas in Chinese they are identical in length. According to the visual acuity principle (Bertram & Hyönä, 2003), short Finnish compound words and all two-character Chinese compound words that fit in foveal vision are recognized holistically, whereas long Finnish compound words are recognized via components. Experiment 1 in Finnish provided evidence consistent with the account, whereas the results for long compound words presented in condensed font in Experiment 2 were inconsistent with it. In Chinese, the first-character frequency effect was non-significant even when the compound words were presented in large font. The Finnish results suggest that componential processing is necessary when the compound word entails more than 10 letters. The Chinese results are compatible with the Chinese Reading Model (Li & Pollatsek, 2020) that assumes whole-word representations to overrule the activation of components during compound word recognition.

字母芬兰语和汉语的两个眼动实验考察了阅读中双成分复合词的识别。芬兰语双成分复合词的长度差异很大,而汉语双成分复合词的长度相同。根据视觉敏锐度原理(Bertram &Hyönä, 2003),芬兰语短复合词和所有符合中央凹视觉的双字复合词都是整体识别,而芬兰语长复合词是通过成分识别。芬兰语实验1提供的证据与该说法一致,而实验2中以缩略语呈现的长复合词的结果与该说法不一致。在汉语中,即使以大字体呈现复合词,首字频次效应也不显著。芬兰的研究结果表明,当复合词包含超过10个字母时,成分处理是必要的。中文结果与中文阅读模型(Li &Pollatsek, 2020),假设在复合词识别过程中,全词表示会推翻组件的激活。
{"title":"Reading compound words in Finnish and Chinese: An eye-tracking study","authors":"Jukka Hyönä ,&nbsp;Lei Cui ,&nbsp;Timo T. Heikkilä ,&nbsp;Birgitta Paranko ,&nbsp;Yun Gao ,&nbsp;Xingzhi Su","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104474","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104474","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Two eye-tracking experiments in alphabetic Finnish and two in logographic Chinese examined the recognition of two-constituent compound words in reading. In Finnish, two-constituent compound words vary greatly in length, whereas in Chinese they are identical in length. According to the visual acuity principle (<span>Bertram &amp; Hyönä, 2003</span>), short Finnish compound words and all two-character Chinese compound words that fit in foveal vision are recognized holistically, whereas long Finnish compound words are recognized via components. Experiment 1 in Finnish provided evidence consistent with the account, whereas the results for long compound words presented in condensed font in Experiment 2 were inconsistent with it. In Chinese, the first-character frequency effect was non-significant even when the compound words were presented in large font. The Finnish results suggest that componential processing is necessary when the compound word entails more than 10 letters. The Chinese results are compatible with the Chinese Reading Model (<span>Li &amp; Pollatsek, 2020</span>) that assumes whole-word representations to overrule the activation of components during compound word recognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X23000736/pdfft?md5=1bc809d1f89499693927f3f209f4e66c&pid=1-s2.0-S0749596X23000736-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92045821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of memory and language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1