首页 > 最新文献

Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Discrimination of highly similar stimuli as members of different equivalence classes. 将高度相似的刺激物识别为不同等价类的成员。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4230
Vanessa Ayres-Pereira, Deisy de Souza, Erik Arntzen

Learning to discriminate between physically similar stimuli as members of different classes can be relevant in certain situations. This study investigated effective methods of displaying two pairs of quasi-identical stimuli, as samples and/or comparisons, during the training of baseline conditional discriminations. The goal was to enable participants to form three 3-member equivalence classes and discriminate similar stimuli as members of distinct equivalence classes. Eighteen adults underwent arbitrary relations (AB/AC) training. A multiple-probe design assessed maintenance and emergence of stimulus relations. Participants were randomly assigned to one of six training conditions across three experiments. Conditions 1, 2, and 5 presented quasi-identical stimuli successively as samples during training. Condition 3 presented quasi-identical stimuli successively as comparisons, whereas Condition 4 presented quasi-identical stimuli simultaneously as comparisons. Condition 6 presented each pair of quasi-identical stimuli simultaneously as a sample and a comparison. Condition 4 uniquely resulted in successful equivalence class formation for all participants. Conditions 3 and 6 failed to form equivalence classes, whereas Conditions 1, 2, and 5 did not yield baseline learning. These findings highlight the relevance of presenting quasi-identical stimuli as simultaneous comparisons (Condition 4) to foster equivalence class formation. Understanding optimal training conditions has implications for discussions on the acquisition of simple discriminations required in training simultaneous conditional discriminations.

在某些情况下,学习将物理上相似的刺激物作为不同类别的成员进行判别是有意义的。本研究调查了在基线条件辨别训练中显示两对准相同刺激物作为样本和/或比较的有效方法。目的是让参与者能够形成三个三元等价类,并将相似的刺激物作为不同等价类的成员进行辨别。18 名成人接受了任意关系(AB/AC)训练。多重探究设计评估了刺激关系的维持和出现。参与者被随机分配到三个实验中的六个训练条件之一。条件 1、条件 2 和条件 5 在训练过程中连续呈现准相同的刺激作为样本。条件 3 将相似的刺激物作为比较物连续呈现,而条件 4 则将相似的刺激物作为比较物同时呈现。条件 6 将每对准相同刺激同时作为样本和对比样本呈现。条件 4 使所有参与者都成功地形成了等价类。条件 3 和条件 6 未能形成等价类,而条件 1、条件 2 和条件 5 则没有产生基线学习。这些发现凸显了以同时比较的方式呈现准相同刺激(条件 4)对促进等价类形成的意义。了解最佳训练条件对于讨论训练同时条件辨别时所需的简单辨别的习得具有重要意义。
{"title":"Discrimination of highly similar stimuli as members of different equivalence classes.","authors":"Vanessa Ayres-Pereira, Deisy de Souza, Erik Arntzen","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4230","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.4230","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning to discriminate between physically similar stimuli as members of different classes can be relevant in certain situations. This study investigated effective methods of displaying two pairs of quasi-identical stimuli, as samples and/or comparisons, during the training of baseline conditional discriminations. The goal was to enable participants to form three 3-member equivalence classes and discriminate similar stimuli as members of distinct equivalence classes. Eighteen adults underwent arbitrary relations (AB/AC) training. A multiple-probe design assessed maintenance and emergence of stimulus relations. Participants were randomly assigned to one of six training conditions across three experiments. Conditions 1, 2, and 5 presented quasi-identical stimuli successively as samples during training. Condition 3 presented quasi-identical stimuli successively as comparisons, whereas Condition 4 presented quasi-identical stimuli simultaneously as comparisons. Condition 6 presented each pair of quasi-identical stimuli simultaneously as a sample and a comparison. Condition 4 uniquely resulted in successful equivalence class formation for all participants. Conditions 3 and 6 failed to form equivalence classes, whereas Conditions 1, 2, and 5 did not yield baseline learning. These findings highlight the relevance of presenting quasi-identical stimuli as simultaneous comparisons (Condition 4) to foster equivalence class formation. Understanding optimal training conditions has implications for discussions on the acquisition of simple discriminations required in training simultaneous conditional discriminations.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142668347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of simultaneous point gains and losses on human persistence. 同时获得和失去积分对人类持久性的影响。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4228
André Connor de Méo Luiz, Myenne Mieko Ayres Tsutsumi, Luis Humbert Andrade de Lemos, José Martins da Silva Neto, Vinícius Kojicowski da Silva, Juliana Suemi Gomes Shirakwa, Julia Rocker Dos Santos, Guilherme Alcantara Ramos

Four experiments assessed the effects of simultaneous point gains and losses on human responding on a moving response button. Experiments 1 and 2 examined the effects of point loss arranged in variable-time (VT) and variable-interval (VI) schedules on persistence. For that purpose, a multiple schedule was in force. One component had point gains only, and the other had point gains and losses. The net reinforcement rate was equated across components by arranging greater point gains in the gains-plus-losses component. Increases in the speed of the moving response button disrupted responding during test sessions. No differential persistence between point-gains and point-gains-and-loss conditions was observed during Experiments 1 and 2. To ensure that point losses could function as punishers, Experiments 3 and 4 compared the effects of point loss arranged in fixed-ratio (FR) or VI schedules on response rate and persistence. The FR and VI point loss decreased the response rate during Experiment 3 but did not produce differential persistence in Experiment 4. These results suggest that point loss decreases response rate but does not weaken persistence more than gains strengthen persistence.

四项实验评估了同时增加和减少点数对人类在移动反应按钮上做出反应的影响。实验 1 和 2 考察了在可变时间(VT)和可变间隔(VI)计划中安排的点数损失对持续性的影响。为此,我们采用了多重时间表。其中一个部分只有点数收益,另一个部分则有点数收益和损失。各部分的净强化率是相等的,即在收益加损失部分安排较大的点数收益。在测试过程中,移动反应按钮速度的增加会扰乱反应。在实验 1 和 2 中,没有观察到点数收益和点数收益加损失条件下的持续性差异。为了确保丢分能够起到惩罚的作用,实验 3 和 4 比较了固定比率(FR)或 VI 计划中的丢分对反应率和持续性的影响。在实验 3 中,固定比率和 VI 失分降低了反应率,但在实验 4 中却没有产生不同的持续性。这些结果表明,失分会降低反应率,但不会比增分更能增强持续性。
{"title":"The effects of simultaneous point gains and losses on human persistence.","authors":"André Connor de Méo Luiz, Myenne Mieko Ayres Tsutsumi, Luis Humbert Andrade de Lemos, José Martins da Silva Neto, Vinícius Kojicowski da Silva, Juliana Suemi Gomes Shirakwa, Julia Rocker Dos Santos, Guilherme Alcantara Ramos","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4228","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.4228","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Four experiments assessed the effects of simultaneous point gains and losses on human responding on a moving response button. Experiments 1 and 2 examined the effects of point loss arranged in variable-time (VT) and variable-interval (VI) schedules on persistence. For that purpose, a multiple schedule was in force. One component had point gains only, and the other had point gains and losses. The net reinforcement rate was equated across components by arranging greater point gains in the gains-plus-losses component. Increases in the speed of the moving response button disrupted responding during test sessions. No differential persistence between point-gains and point-gains-and-loss conditions was observed during Experiments 1 and 2. To ensure that point losses could function as punishers, Experiments 3 and 4 compared the effects of point loss arranged in fixed-ratio (FR) or VI schedules on response rate and persistence. The FR and VI point loss decreased the response rate during Experiment 3 but did not produce differential persistence in Experiment 4. These results suggest that point loss decreases response rate but does not weaken persistence more than gains strengthen persistence.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142668330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reward deprivation is associated with elevated alcohol demand in emerging adults. 奖励剥夺与新兴成人对酒精的需求增加有关。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4229
James G Murphy, Samuel F Acuff, Avery C Buck, Kevin W Campbell, James MacKillop

Policies vary substantially in terms of providing sources of psychosocial enrichment. Behavioral economic models of substance use and addiction emphasize that deficits in access to substance-free sources of reward increase substance reinforcing value and risk for addiction. The current study used an alcohol demand curve approach to test the hypothesis that various indices of reward deprivation would be associated with elevated alcohol reinforcing efficacy. We examined associations between alcohol demand indices and several facets of reward deprivation in a sample of young adults (N = 1,331; ages 19-25 years) recruited from the United States and Canada who reported recent binge drinking. Additionally, we created an index of cumulative reward deprivation that integrated the various reward facets and examined its association with alcohol demand intensity and maximum expenditure on alcohol. Our findings indicate that reward deprivation is associated with elevated alcohol demand and provide support for alcohol prevention and intervention approaches that emphasize environmental enrichment.

在提供丰富的社会心理来源方面,政策大相径庭。药物使用和成瘾的行为经济学模型强调,在获得无药物奖励来源方面的缺陷会增加药物的强化价值和成瘾风险。本研究采用酒精需求曲线的方法来验证一个假设,即各种奖励剥夺指数与酒精强化效能的升高有关。我们对从美国和加拿大招募的、报告最近曾酗酒的年轻成年人样本(样本数=1,331;年龄19-25岁)中的酒精需求指数和奖赏剥夺的几个方面进行了研究。此外,我们还创建了一个综合了各种奖励因素的累积奖励剥夺指数,并研究了该指数与酒精需求强度和最大酒精支出之间的关系。我们的研究结果表明,奖赏剥夺与酒精需求增加有关,并为强调丰富环境的酒精预防和干预方法提供了支持。
{"title":"Reward deprivation is associated with elevated alcohol demand in emerging adults.","authors":"James G Murphy, Samuel F Acuff, Avery C Buck, Kevin W Campbell, James MacKillop","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4229","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4229","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Policies vary substantially in terms of providing sources of psychosocial enrichment. Behavioral economic models of substance use and addiction emphasize that deficits in access to substance-free sources of reward increase substance reinforcing value and risk for addiction. The current study used an alcohol demand curve approach to test the hypothesis that various indices of reward deprivation would be associated with elevated alcohol reinforcing efficacy. We examined associations between alcohol demand indices and several facets of reward deprivation in a sample of young adults (N = 1,331; ages 19-25 years) recruited from the United States and Canada who reported recent binge drinking. Additionally, we created an index of cumulative reward deprivation that integrated the various reward facets and examined its association with alcohol demand intensity and maximum expenditure on alcohol. Our findings indicate that reward deprivation is associated with elevated alcohol demand and provide support for alcohol prevention and intervention approaches that emphasize environmental enrichment.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142622976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Zoographics in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior: Increasing inclusion of female animals 行为实验分析期刊》上的动物图谱:增加雌性动物。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4220
Amy L. Odum, Kiernan T. Callister, Mariah E. Willis-Moore, Daniel S. Da Silva, David N. Legaspi, Lucy N. Scribner, Josephine N. Hannah

We examined the zoographics, or the characteristics of nonhuman animal subjects, reported for the entirety of the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (JEAB) through the most recent complete year (1958–2023). Animal sex in particular was evaluated to determine whether the lack of inclusion of female subjects in other disciplines extends to JEAB. Through systematic coding of all nonhuman empirical articles, we found consistent underreporting of most zoographics and a disproportionate use of male subjects relative to female subjects. Additionally, animal sex was commonly unreported and the inclusion of both male and female subjects was sparse. Recent years show some improvement, but greater inclusion is required. Lack of female subjects in research as well as underreporting of zoographics can generate unrepresentative results and hamper replication, generalization, and translation. We provide resources to guide future research and reporting suggestions such as equal inclusion and disaggregation of data by sex. We also clarify misunderstandings about the use of both sexes in research such as beliefs that it necessarily increases the cost of research.

我们研究了《行为实验分析期刊》(JEAB)最近完整年份(1958-2023 年)所报道的动物图谱,即非人类动物受试者的特征。我们特别对动物的性别进行了评估,以确定在其他学科中缺少雌性受试者的情况是否也存在于 JEAB 中。通过对所有非人类经验性文章进行系统编码,我们发现大多数动物图谱都存在报告不足的情况,而且相对于女性受试者而言,男性受试者的使用比例过高。此外,未报告动物性别的情况也很普遍,同时纳入男性和女性受试者的情况也很少。近几年的情况有所改善,但还需要更多的纳入。研究中缺少女性受试者以及动物图谱报告不足会导致研究结果不具代表性,并阻碍研究结果的复制、推广和转化。我们提供了指导未来研究的资源和报告建议,如平等纳入和按性别分列数据。我们还澄清了在研究中使用两性的误解,如认为这必然会增加研究成本。
{"title":"Zoographics in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior: Increasing inclusion of female animals","authors":"Amy L. Odum,&nbsp;Kiernan T. Callister,&nbsp;Mariah E. Willis-Moore,&nbsp;Daniel S. Da Silva,&nbsp;David N. Legaspi,&nbsp;Lucy N. Scribner,&nbsp;Josephine N. Hannah","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4220","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4220","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We examined the zoographics, or the characteristics of nonhuman animal subjects, reported for the entirety of the <i>Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</i> (<i>JEAB</i>) through the most recent complete year (1958–2023). Animal sex in particular was evaluated to determine whether the lack of inclusion of female subjects in other disciplines extends to <i>JEAB</i>. Through systematic coding of all nonhuman empirical articles, we found consistent underreporting of most zoographics and a disproportionate use of male subjects relative to female subjects. Additionally, animal sex was commonly unreported and the inclusion of both male and female subjects was sparse. Recent years show some improvement, but greater inclusion is required. Lack of female subjects in research as well as underreporting of zoographics can generate unrepresentative results and hamper replication, generalization, and translation. We provide resources to guide future research and reporting suggestions such as equal inclusion and disaggregation of data by sex. We also clarify misunderstandings about the use of both sexes in research such as beliefs that it necessarily increases the cost of research.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"392-407"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546162","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluation of resurgence following differential reinforcement of alternative behavior with and without extinction in a human operant model 在人类操作性模型中,评估对替代行为进行有消退和无消退的不同强化后的复发情况。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4222
Skylar DeWitt, Adam M. Briggs

One of the most common treatments for severe challenging behavior involves placing the challenging behavior on extinction and differentially reinforcing an alternative response (DRA). However, extinction is not always feasible and may be unsafe or impractical to implement in some circumstances. Thus, implementing a DRA without extinction intervention may be necessary for some cases. Currently, the extent to which DRA without extinction produces durable treatment outcomes, particularly as it relates to the resurgence of challenging behavior, is unclear. The present study investigated resurgence following DRA with and without extinction using a three-phase resurgence evaluation in a translational human operant model with college students as participants. All participants demonstrated resurgence across both experimental groups. Although there were no statistically significant differences in the prevalence, magnitude, or persistence of resurgence between groups, levels of resurgence magnitude were relatively higher in the DRA-without-extinction group than in the DRA-with-extinction group. Clinical implications of these findings and directions for future human operant investigations of resurgence are discussed.

针对严重挑战行为最常见的治疗方法之一是对挑战行为进行消 灭,并对替代反应(DRA)进行不同程度的强化。然而,消减并不总是可行的,在某些情况下实施消减可能不安全或不切实际。因此,在某些情况下,可能有必要实施不使用消减干预的 DRA。目前,不使用绝迹的 DRA 能在多大程度上产生持久的治疗效果,尤其是与挑战性行为复发有关的效果,尚不清楚。本研究以大学生为研究对象,在转化人类操作模型中采用三阶段复归评估方法,调查了使用或不使用消退法进行 DRA 后的复归情况。在两个实验组中,所有参与者都表现出了恢复行为。虽然各组之间在复燃的发生率、程度或持续时间上没有统计学意义上的显著差异,但不使用绝迹的 DRA 组的复燃程度相对高于使用绝迹的 DRA 组。本文讨论了这些研究结果的临床意义以及未来人类操作性复归研究的方向。
{"title":"Evaluation of resurgence following differential reinforcement of alternative behavior with and without extinction in a human operant model","authors":"Skylar DeWitt,&nbsp;Adam M. Briggs","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4222","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4222","url":null,"abstract":"<p>One of the most common treatments for severe challenging behavior involves placing the challenging behavior on extinction and differentially reinforcing an alternative response (DRA). However, extinction is not always feasible and may be unsafe or impractical to implement in some circumstances. Thus, implementing a DRA without extinction intervention may be necessary for some cases. Currently, the extent to which DRA without extinction produces durable treatment outcomes, particularly as it relates to the resurgence of challenging behavior, is unclear. The present study investigated resurgence following DRA with and without extinction using a three-phase resurgence evaluation in a translational human operant model with college students as participants. All participants demonstrated resurgence across both experimental groups. Although there were no statistically significant differences in the prevalence, magnitude, or persistence of resurgence between groups, levels of resurgence magnitude were relatively higher in the DRA-without-extinction group than in the DRA-with-extinction group. Clinical implications of these findings and directions for future human operant investigations of resurgence are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"351-361"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142502907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ecological momentary assessment of delay discounting, reward valuation, and craving in very light cigarette users 对极少吸烟者的延迟贴现、奖励估价和渴求进行生态瞬间评估。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4221
Melinda L. Furer, Siyuan Huang, Joshua M. Smyth, Stephen J. Wilson

Heightened delay discounting has been linked to adverse smoking cessation outcomes, including among light cigarette users. Few studies have evaluated delay discounting's proposed mechanism, preference reversal (concurrent increases in valuation of/craving for desired objects), and none have done so in naturalistic settings. We examined how person-level delay discounting moderated the within-person association between cigarette valuation and craving among very light daily cigarette users who were financially incentivized to abstain. Forty participants completed a baseline delay-discounting task and intermittent ratings of cigarette valuation and craving during the incentivized abstinence attempt. Subjects earned monetary rewards for abstinence on a descending schedule (e.g., $20 on Days 1 and 2 and $2.50 on Days 9 and 10). Consistent with preference reversals, there was a positive association between cigarette valuation and craving. This relation was moderated by delay discounting (stronger among those with low discounting rates) and by monetary reinforcement amount (stronger on days with low reinforcement). Additionally, subjects were more likely to report stronger cravings on days with high monetary reinforcement, with this effect moderated by delay discounting (stronger among those with low discounting rates). The results suggest that heightened delay discounting may not confer risk for preference reversal among very light daily cigarette users who are attempting abstinence.

延迟折现的增加与不良戒烟结果有关,包括轻烟使用者。很少有研究对延迟折现的拟议机制--偏好逆转(对所需对象的估价/渴望同时增加)进行评估,也没有研究在自然环境下进行评估。我们研究了个人层面的延迟折现是如何调节受经济激励戒烟的极轻度日常吸烟者的香烟估值与渴求之间的人际关联的。40 名受试者完成了基线延迟贴现任务,并在受激励的戒烟尝试中对香烟价值和渴求进行了间歇性评分。受试者按递减计划获得戒烟金钱奖励(例如,第 1 天和第 2 天 20 美元,第 9 天和第 10 天 2.50 美元)。与偏好逆转一致,香烟估值与渴求之间存在正相关。这种关系受延迟折现(低折现率者更强)和货币强化金额(低强化日更强)的调节。此外,在高货币强化日,受试者更有可能报告出更强烈的渴求感,这种效应受延迟折现的调节(在低折现率受试者中更强烈)。这些结果表明,在试图戒烟的每日吸烟量极少的人中,延迟折现的增加可能不会带来偏好逆转的风险。
{"title":"Ecological momentary assessment of delay discounting, reward valuation, and craving in very light cigarette users","authors":"Melinda L. Furer,&nbsp;Siyuan Huang,&nbsp;Joshua M. Smyth,&nbsp;Stephen J. Wilson","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4221","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4221","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Heightened delay discounting has been linked to adverse smoking cessation outcomes, including among light cigarette users. Few studies have evaluated delay discounting's proposed mechanism, preference reversal (concurrent increases in valuation of/craving for desired objects), and none have done so in naturalistic settings. We examined how person-level delay discounting moderated the within-person association between cigarette valuation and craving among very light daily cigarette users who were financially incentivized to abstain. Forty participants completed a baseline delay-discounting task and intermittent ratings of cigarette valuation and craving during the incentivized abstinence attempt. Subjects earned monetary rewards for abstinence on a descending schedule (e.g., $20 on Days 1 and 2 and $2.50 on Days 9 and 10). Consistent with preference reversals, there was a positive association between cigarette valuation and craving. This relation was moderated by delay discounting (stronger among those with low discounting rates) and by monetary reinforcement amount (stronger on days with low reinforcement). Additionally, subjects were more likely to report stronger cravings on days with high monetary reinforcement, with this effect moderated by delay discounting (stronger among those with low discounting rates). The results suggest that heightened delay discounting may not confer risk for preference reversal among very light daily cigarette users who are attempting abstinence.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"335-350"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jeab.4221","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142502906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Avoidance of hot air blast in Rattus norvegicus 鼠类躲避热气冲击的能力
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-23 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4225
Elisama Almeida Condurú Melo, Amilcar Rodrigues Fonseca-Júnior, Marcus Bentes de Carvalho-Neto

The hegemonic use of electric shock as an aversive stimulus limits what is known about the generality of avoidance behavior and related phenomena. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of using the hot air blast (HAB) instead of electric shock as an aversive stimulus in avoidance conditioning. Four male Wistar rats were exposed to a discrete-trial procedure. In the first phase, the emission of a lever-press response during a trial was positively reinforced. In the second phase, the same contingency was employed, but an escapable HAB was presented at the end of a trial when no response was emitted. In the third phase, positive reinforcement was suspended and a discrete-trial avoidance procedure was employed. In the fourth phase, HAB presentation was completely suspended. As a result, all subjects learned the avoidance response and showed a marked reduction in its frequency during HAB suspension. These results offer evidence indicating that avoidance behavior is controlled by the HAB, thereby advancing the understanding of the generality of this stimulus as being aversive.

使用电击作为厌恶刺激的霸权限制了人们对回避行为及相关现象普遍性的了解。本研究的目的是调查在回避条件反射中使用热空气冲击(HAB)而不是电击作为厌恶刺激的效果。研究人员对四只雄性 Wistar 大鼠进行了离散试验。在第一阶段,试验中按下杠杆的反应被正强化。在第二阶段,采用相同的条件反射,但在试验结束时,如果大鼠没有做出任何反应,则会出现一个可逃脱的 HAB。第三阶段,暂停正强化,采用离散试验回避程序。在第四阶段,HAB 的呈现完全暂停。结果,所有受试者都学会了回避反应,并且在 HAB 暂停期间,回避反应的频率明显降低。这些结果提供了证据,表明回避行为是由 HAB 控制的,从而加深了人们对这种刺激具有厌恶性的普遍性的理解。
{"title":"Avoidance of hot air blast in Rattus norvegicus","authors":"Elisama Almeida Condurú Melo,&nbsp;Amilcar Rodrigues Fonseca-Júnior,&nbsp;Marcus Bentes de Carvalho-Neto","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4225","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4225","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The hegemonic use of electric shock as an aversive stimulus limits what is known about the generality of avoidance behavior and related phenomena. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of using the hot air blast (HAB) instead of electric shock as an aversive stimulus in avoidance conditioning. Four male Wistar rats were exposed to a discrete-trial procedure. In the first phase, the emission of a lever-press response during a trial was positively reinforced. In the second phase, the same contingency was employed, but an escapable HAB was presented at the end of a trial when no response was emitted. In the third phase, positive reinforcement was suspended and a discrete-trial avoidance procedure was employed. In the fourth phase, HAB presentation was completely suspended. As a result, all subjects learned the avoidance response and showed a marked reduction in its frequency during HAB suspension. These results offer evidence indicating that avoidance behavior is controlled by the HAB, thereby advancing the understanding of the generality of this stimulus as being aversive.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"413-422"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142502905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social discounting in a symmetric giving and taking frame: An artifactual field experiment with young South African adults 对称施与受框架下的社会折扣:以南非年轻成年人为对象的人工现场实验。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4218
Frederik Booysen, Sevias Guvuriro, Herkulaas Morkel van Eyssen Combrink

In the dictator game, the evidence that giving is equivalent to taking is mixed. The purpose of this study was to investigate framing effects (giving/taking) on social-discounting rates among young African adults from an informal settlement in South Africa. Employing a within-participant design, these young adults completed a series of incentivized dictator games with an isomorphically equivalent giving and taking frame at each of eight social distances. Altruism was measured by the social-discounting rate, and framing effects were assessed using generalized linear regression. The study provides empirical evidence that prosocial behavior among young South African adults is subject to framing because exponential, hyperbolic, and q-exponential social-discounting rates in all instances were lower in the taking than in the giving frame. This difference may be the result of greater “egalitarianism” and “selflessness” elicited by the taking frame, which likely is a product of the experiment's particular social and economic field context. More comparative research is required to establish the working of specific mechanics of morality that may operate differently in diverse socioeconomic contexts, thus contributing to elucidating the heterogeneous nature of findings in this area of study.

在独裁者游戏中,关于给予等同于索取的证据不一。本研究的目的是调查框架效应(给予/索取)对南非非正规居住区非洲年轻人社会折扣率的影响。这些年轻人采用参与者内部设计,在八个社会距离中的每一个距离完成了一系列具有同构等价给予和接受框架的激励独裁者游戏。利他主义通过社会折扣率来衡量,框架效应则通过广义线性回归来评估。该研究提供了南非年轻成年人的亲社会行为受框架影响的实证证据,因为在所有情况下,指数型、双曲线型和 q 指数型社会折扣率在接受框架下均低于给予框架。这种差异可能是由于 "索取 "框架引发了更多的 "平等主义 "和 "无私奉献精神",而这很可能是实验的特定社会和经济背景下的产物。需要进行更多的比较研究,以确定在不同的社会经济背景下可能以不同方式运作的特定道德机制,从而有助于阐明这一研究领域中发现的异质性。
{"title":"Social discounting in a symmetric giving and taking frame: An artifactual field experiment with young South African adults","authors":"Frederik Booysen,&nbsp;Sevias Guvuriro,&nbsp;Herkulaas Morkel van Eyssen Combrink","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4218","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4218","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the dictator game, the evidence that giving is equivalent to taking is mixed. The purpose of this study was to investigate framing effects (giving/taking) on social-discounting rates among young African adults from an informal settlement in South Africa. Employing a within-participant design, these young adults completed a series of incentivized dictator games with an isomorphically equivalent giving and taking frame at each of eight social distances. Altruism was measured by the social-discounting rate, and framing effects were assessed using generalized linear regression. The study provides empirical evidence that prosocial behavior among young South African adults is subject to framing because exponential, hyperbolic, and q-exponential social-discounting rates in all instances were lower in the taking than in the giving frame. This difference may be the result of greater “egalitarianism” and “selflessness” elicited by the taking frame, which likely is a product of the experiment's particular social and economic field context. More comparative research is required to establish the working of specific mechanics of morality that may operate differently in diverse socioeconomic contexts, thus contributing to elucidating the heterogeneous nature of findings in this area of study.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"322-334"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jeab.4218","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142468617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Application of synchronous music reinforcement to increase walking speed: A novel approach for training intensity 应用同步音乐强化提高步行速度:提高训练强度的新方法
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4219
Jonathan W. Pinkston, Jennifer L. Cook, Rasha R. Baruni, John T. Rapp, Shreeya Deshmukh, Raymond G. Miltenberger

Walking is a common and preferred form of exercise. Although there are current recommendations for walking volume (e.g., steps per day), recent research has begun to distinguish volume from intensity (e.g., “brisk” walking) as an important dimension of exercise. Increasing intensity may confer health advantages beyond volume measures because it shifts cardiovascular performance to more vigorous training zones. Reinforcement-based approaches have been valuable in increasing volume measures of exercise, and the present study sought to develop a corresponding reinforcement approach to training walking intensity. For this study, we used a continuous reinforcement paradigm where music played only while walking met specified criteria; otherwise, music playback stopped. As a result, music was synchronized with walking performance. Seventeen participants walked on a nonmotorized treadmill at a self-selected pace. Across the session, different conditions arranged for music to play independent of walking speed or contingent on speed increases or decreases. An extinction component assessed performance when music was withdrawn completely. Walking speed was selectively increased and decreased by adjusting the contingencies that were arranged for music, and variability in speed increased during extinction, with both findings indicating that music was a reinforcer. Heart rate was also increased to moderate–vigorous intensities during reinforcement. The findings provide a compelling case that walking intensity can be modified by music reinforcement. We suggest that synchronous schedules may be an important foundation for future exercise technologies that are based on reinforcement.

步行是一种常见和首选的锻炼方式。虽然目前有关于步行量(如每天步行步数)的建议,但最近的研究已开始将步行量与强度(如 "快走")作为运动的一个重要维度加以区分。增加强度可能会带来超越运动量的健康优势,因为它能将心血管性能转移到更剧烈的训练区域。基于强化的方法在增加运动量方面很有价值,本研究试图开发一种相应的强化方法来训练步行强度。在本研究中,我们采用了一种连续强化范式,即只有在步行达到指定标准时才播放音乐,否则音乐停止播放。因此,音乐与行走表现是同步的。17 名参与者在非机动跑步机上以自选的速度行走。在整个过程中,不同的条件安排了与步行速度无关的音乐播放,或者根据速度的增减来播放音乐。当音乐完全停止时,会有一个消退环节来评估成绩。通过调整为音乐安排的条件,步行速度有选择性地增加或减少,在消退过程中速度的变化增加,这两个结果都表明音乐是一种强化剂。在强化过程中,心率也增加到中等强度。研究结果令人信服地证明,音乐强化可以改变步行强度。我们认为,同步时间表可能是未来基于强化的运动技术的重要基础。
{"title":"Application of synchronous music reinforcement to increase walking speed: A novel approach for training intensity","authors":"Jonathan W. Pinkston,&nbsp;Jennifer L. Cook,&nbsp;Rasha R. Baruni,&nbsp;John T. Rapp,&nbsp;Shreeya Deshmukh,&nbsp;Raymond G. Miltenberger","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4219","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4219","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Walking is a common and preferred form of exercise. Although there are current recommendations for walking volume (e.g., steps per day), recent research has begun to distinguish volume from intensity (e.g., “brisk” walking) as an important dimension of exercise. Increasing intensity may confer health advantages beyond volume measures because it shifts cardiovascular performance to more vigorous training zones. Reinforcement-based approaches have been valuable in increasing volume measures of exercise, and the present study sought to develop a corresponding reinforcement approach to training walking intensity. For this study, we used a continuous reinforcement paradigm where music played only while walking met specified criteria; otherwise, music playback stopped. As a result, music was synchronized with walking performance. Seventeen participants walked on a nonmotorized treadmill at a self-selected pace. Across the session, different conditions arranged for music to play independent of walking speed or contingent on speed increases or decreases. An extinction component assessed performance when music was withdrawn completely. Walking speed was selectively increased and decreased by adjusting the contingencies that were arranged for music, and variability in speed increased during extinction, with both findings indicating that music was a reinforcer. Heart rate was also increased to moderate–vigorous intensities during reinforcement. The findings provide a compelling case that walking intensity can be modified by music reinforcement. We suggest that synchronous schedules may be an important foundation for future exercise technologies that are based on reinforcement.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"362-374"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142468616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Common Ground 共同点。
IF 1.4 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4216
Kennon A. Lattal
{"title":"Common Ground","authors":"Kennon A. Lattal","doi":"10.1002/jeab.4216","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.4216","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"122 3","pages":"411-412"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142361719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1