首页 > 最新文献

Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Assessing negative reinforcement through simultaneous observing and committed concurrent progressive-ratio procedures: Preliminary investigations 通过同步观察和承诺同步累进比率程序评估负强化:初步调查
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.913
Benjamin N. Witts, Jennifer L. Bruzek

Efficient methods for assessing the relative aversiveness of stimuli are sparse and underresearched. Having access to efficient procedures that can identify aversive stimuli would benefit researchers and practitioners alike. Across three experiments, 13 participants helped to pilot, refine, and test two approaches to identifying negative reinforcers. The first experiment presented two conditions, one in which computerized button pressing started or stopped one of two recorded infant cries (or silence, when the control button was selected). Choices were presented either in a modified observing-response procedure (i.e., simultaneous observing) or in a modified progressive-ratio procedure (i.e., committed concurrent progressive ratio; CCPR). Results were favorable though not conclusive on their own. A second experiment, using more distinct stimuli (i.e., one likely aversive, one likely not aversive), replicated the first, and clearer results emerged. Finally, the third experiment tested the stimuli from the second experiment in a CCPR arrangement where sound was terminated contingent on responding and idiosyncratic negative reinforcement hierarchies emerged. The utility of these two procedures is discussed, and future work that addresses the limitations is outlined.

评估刺激物相对厌恶性的有效方法很少,而且研究不足。如果能获得高效的程序来识别厌恶性刺激,研究人员和从业人员都将受益匪浅。在三个实验中,13 名参与者帮助试验、改进和测试了两种识别负强化物的方法。第一个实验提供了两个条件,其中一个条件是电脑按下按钮开始或停止录制的两种婴儿哭声中的一种(或选择控制按钮时保持沉默)。选择是在修改后的观察-反应程序(即同步观察)或修改后的累进比率程序(即承诺并发累进比率;CCPR)中呈现的。结果是好的,但本身并不具有决定性。第二个实验使用了更多不同的刺激物(即一个可能是厌恶性刺激,一个可能不是厌恶性刺激),重复了第一个实验,得出了更明确的结果。最后,第三个实验在 CCPR 安排中测试了第二个实验中的刺激物,在该安排中,声音的终止取决于反应,并出现了特异的负强化等级。本文讨论了这两项实验的实用性,并概述了解决这些局限性的未来工作。
{"title":"Assessing negative reinforcement through simultaneous observing and committed concurrent progressive-ratio procedures: Preliminary investigations","authors":"Benjamin N. Witts,&nbsp;Jennifer L. Bruzek","doi":"10.1002/jeab.913","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.913","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Efficient methods for assessing the relative aversiveness of stimuli are sparse and underresearched. Having access to efficient procedures that can identify aversive stimuli would benefit researchers and practitioners alike. Across three experiments, 13 participants helped to pilot, refine, and test two approaches to identifying negative reinforcers. The first experiment presented two conditions, one in which computerized button pressing started or stopped one of two recorded infant cries (or silence, when the control button was selected). Choices were presented either in a modified observing-response procedure (i.e., simultaneous observing) or in a modified progressive-ratio procedure (i.e., committed concurrent progressive ratio; CCPR). Results were favorable though not conclusive on their own. A second experiment, using more distinct stimuli (i.e., one likely aversive, one likely not aversive), replicated the first, and clearer results emerged. Finally, the third experiment tested the stimuli from the second experiment in a CCPR arrangement where sound was terminated contingent on responding and idiosyncratic negative reinforcement hierarchies emerged. The utility of these two procedures is discussed, and future work that addresses the limitations is outlined.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"346-357"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140583032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Research and mentorship in behavior analysis from a lens of cultural responsiveness and antiracism 从文化响应和反种族主义的角度开展行为分析研究和导师工作
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.911
Corina Jimenez-Gomez

Although scientific endeavors strive to be objective, they are the work of individuals whose unique perspectives and experiences influence their research and interpretations of the world and data. Much has been said and written lately about the need to embed cultural responsiveness in behavior analysis and the need to enhance diversity in the field. In fact, similar conversations are taking place in many areas of science. Despite the current buzz, many behavioral researchers may be left wondering what they can do or whether it is incumbent on them to act. For the field of behavior analysis to move toward adopting the values of diversity, equity, inclusion, and access, members of the scientific community must actively engage in behaviors that foster inclusive and safe learning environments for students, engage in collaborative work, and incorporate culturally responsive research and mentorship practices. This article will describe some current practices, showcase exemplars of culturally responsive research and mentorship, and provide resources for researchers and mentors.

尽管科学工作力求客观,但它们也是个人的工作,个人的独特视角和经历影响着他们的研究以及对世界和数据的解释。最近,关于在行为分析中植入文化响应的必要性以及加强该领域多样性的必要性,已经有很多论述和文章。事实上,许多科学领域都在进行类似的讨论。尽管目前热闹非凡,但许多行为研究人员可能仍然不知道他们能做些什么,或者他们是否有责任采取行动。行为分析领域要想实现多元化、公平、包容和可及性的价值观,科学界成员就必须积极行动起来,为学生营造包容和安全的学习环境,参与合作工作,并融入文化上相适应的研究和指导实践。本文将介绍当前的一些做法,展示具有文化敏感性的研究和导师制的典范,并为研究人员和导师提供资源。
{"title":"Research and mentorship in behavior analysis from a lens of cultural responsiveness and antiracism","authors":"Corina Jimenez-Gomez","doi":"10.1002/jeab.911","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.911","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although scientific endeavors strive to be objective, they are the work of individuals whose unique perspectives and experiences influence their research and interpretations of the world and data. Much has been said and written lately about the need to embed cultural responsiveness in behavior analysis and the need to enhance diversity in the field. In fact, similar conversations are taking place in many areas of science. Despite the current buzz, many behavioral researchers may be left wondering what they can do or whether it is incumbent on them to act. For the field of behavior analysis to move toward adopting the values of diversity, equity, inclusion, and access, members of the scientific community must actively engage in behaviors that foster inclusive and safe learning environments for students, engage in collaborative work, and incorporate culturally responsive research and mentorship practices. This article will describe some current practices, showcase exemplars of culturally responsive research and mentorship, and provide resources for researchers and mentors.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"373-388"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140583150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An apparatus and procedure for studying discounting of real outcomes of money and aversive sound 研究货币和厌恶声音实际结果贴现的仪器和程序。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.912
Elizabeth Meshes, Jonathan Tarbox, Jonathan A. Meshes, Amy L. Odum

We developed and examined a laboratory preparation with adult humans that pits shorter term avoidance over longer term positive reinforcement and may serve as a useful laboratory functional analogue of problematic behavior. Participants were exposed to choices between (1) avoiding an aversive sound and acquiring no money or (2) listening to an aversive sound for a set duration and then receiving money. The first choice, avoiding an aversive sound and acquiring no money, was conceptualized as immediate negative reinforcement and no positive reinforcement, whereas the latter choice, listening to an aversive sound for a set duration and then receiving money, was conceptualized as a potential positive punisher paired with a larger later positive reinforcer. We manipulated the duration of the sound and the magnitude of money to identify the point at which individual participants' choices changed from avoiding the sound to choosing the sound plus money. As the sound duration increased, the choice of listening to the sound and receiving money decreased. Similar functions were observed with two different monetary magnitudes. The model has potential applicability to real-world problems such as smoking, addiction, gambling, anxiety disorders, and other impulse control disorders.

我们开发并研究了一种以成年人类为实验对象的实验准备方法,该方法将短期回避与长期正强化相比较,可作为问题行为的一种有用的实验室功能模拟。受试者会在以下两种情况中做出选择:(1)回避厌恶的声音,不获得金钱;(2)在一定时间内聆听厌恶的声音,然后获得金钱。第一种选择,即避开厌恶的声音和不拿钱,被认为是立即的负强化和无正强化,而后一种选择,即听厌恶的声音一定时间后再拿钱,被认为是潜在的正惩罚和较大的正强化。我们对声音的持续时间和金钱的数额进行了调整,以确定每个参与者的选择从回避声音转变为选择声音加金钱的时间点。随着声音持续时间的增加,听声音和收钱的选择减少了。在两种不同的货币幅度下也观察到了类似的功能。该模型可能适用于现实世界中的问题,如吸烟、成瘾、赌博、焦虑症和其他冲动控制障碍。
{"title":"An apparatus and procedure for studying discounting of real outcomes of money and aversive sound","authors":"Elizabeth Meshes,&nbsp;Jonathan Tarbox,&nbsp;Jonathan A. Meshes,&nbsp;Amy L. Odum","doi":"10.1002/jeab.912","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.912","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We developed and examined a laboratory preparation with adult humans that pits shorter term avoidance over longer term positive reinforcement and may serve as a useful laboratory functional analogue of problematic behavior. Participants were exposed to choices between (1) avoiding an aversive sound and acquiring no money or (2) listening to an aversive sound for a set duration and then receiving money. The first choice, avoiding an aversive sound and acquiring no money, was conceptualized as immediate negative reinforcement and no positive reinforcement, whereas the latter choice, listening to an aversive sound for a set duration and then receiving money, was conceptualized as a potential positive punisher paired with a larger later positive reinforcer. We manipulated the duration of the sound and the magnitude of money to identify the point at which individual participants' choices changed from avoiding the sound to choosing the sound plus money. As the sound duration increased, the choice of listening to the sound and receiving money decreased. Similar functions were observed with two different monetary magnitudes. The model has potential applicability to real-world problems such as smoking, addiction, gambling, anxiety disorders, and other impulse control disorders.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"389-398"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140336142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resurgence of ethanol seeking following voluntary abstinence produced by nondrug differential reinforcement of other behavior 通过对其他行为进行非药物差异强化,在自愿戒酒后重新寻求乙醇。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.909
Andrew R. Craig, Charlene N. Agnew, Kate E. Derrenbacker, Beatriz Arroyo Antúnez, William E. Sullivan, Sean W. Smith, Jacqueline DeBartelo, Henry S. Roane

Resurgence refers to the relapse of a target behavior following the worsening of a source of alternative reinforcement that was made available during response elimination. Most laboratory analyses of resurgence have used a combination of extinction and alternative reinforcement to reduce target behavior. In contingency-management treatments for alcohol use disorder, however, alcohol use is not placed on extinction. Instead, participants voluntarily abstain from alcohol use to access nondrug alternative reinforcers. Inasmuch, additional laboratory research on resurgence following voluntary abstinence is warranted. The present experiment evaluated resurgence of rats' ethanol seeking following voluntary abstinence produced by differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO). Lever pressing produced ethanol reinforcers during baseline phases. During DRO phases, lever pressing continued to produce ethanol and food reinforcers were delivered according to resetting DRO schedules. Ethanol and food reinforcers were suspended during resurgence test phases to evaluate resurgence following voluntary abstinence. Lever pressing was elevated during baseline phases and occurred at near-zero rates during DRO phases. During the resurgence test phases, lever pressing increased, despite that it no longer produced ethanol. The procedure introduced here may help researchers better understand the variables that affect voluntary abstinence from ethanol seeking and resurgence following voluntary abstinence.

卷土重来指的是在消除反应时提供的替代性强化源恶化后,目标行为再次出现。大多数关于复吸的实验室分析都采用消退和替代性强化相结合的方法来减少目标行为。然而,在针对饮酒障碍的应急管理疗法中,饮酒行为并没有被消灭。相反,参与者会自愿戒酒,以获得非药物替代强化物。因此,有必要对自愿戒酒后的恢复情况进行更多的实验室研究。本实验评估了大鼠在自愿戒酒后通过其他行为的差异强化(DRO)而重新寻求乙醇的情况。在基线阶段,按压杠杆会产生乙醇强化物。在 DRO 阶段,按压杠杆继续产生乙醇,并根据重置 DRO 计划提供食物强化物。在复吸测试阶段,乙醇和食物强化物被暂停,以评估自愿戒断后的复吸情况。在基线阶段,按压杠杆的频率较高,而在DRO阶段,按压杠杆的频率几乎为零。在复吸测试阶段,尽管按压杠杆不再产生乙醇,但按压杠杆的次数仍有所增加。本文介绍的程序可帮助研究人员更好地了解影响自愿戒断乙醇寻求和自愿戒断后复吸的变量。
{"title":"Resurgence of ethanol seeking following voluntary abstinence produced by nondrug differential reinforcement of other behavior","authors":"Andrew R. Craig,&nbsp;Charlene N. Agnew,&nbsp;Kate E. Derrenbacker,&nbsp;Beatriz Arroyo Antúnez,&nbsp;William E. Sullivan,&nbsp;Sean W. Smith,&nbsp;Jacqueline DeBartelo,&nbsp;Henry S. Roane","doi":"10.1002/jeab.909","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.909","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Resurgence refers to the relapse of a target behavior following the worsening of a source of alternative reinforcement that was made available during response elimination. Most laboratory analyses of resurgence have used a combination of extinction and alternative reinforcement to reduce target behavior. In contingency-management treatments for alcohol use disorder, however, alcohol use is not placed on extinction. Instead, participants voluntarily abstain from alcohol use to access nondrug alternative reinforcers. Inasmuch, additional laboratory research on resurgence following voluntary abstinence is warranted. The present experiment evaluated resurgence of rats' ethanol seeking following voluntary abstinence produced by differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO). Lever pressing produced ethanol reinforcers during baseline phases. During DRO phases, lever pressing continued to produce ethanol and food reinforcers were delivered according to resetting DRO schedules. Ethanol and food reinforcers were suspended during resurgence test phases to evaluate resurgence following voluntary abstinence. Lever pressing was elevated during baseline phases and occurred at near-zero rates during DRO phases. During the resurgence test phases, lever pressing increased, despite that it no longer produced ethanol. The procedure introduced here may help researchers better understand the variables that affect voluntary abstinence from ethanol seeking and resurgence following voluntary abstinence.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"314-326"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140158384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A systematic review and meta-analysis of test–retest reliability and stability of delay and probability discounting 对延迟和概率折扣的测试重复可靠性和稳定性进行系统回顾和荟萃分析。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.910
Brett W. Gelino, Rebekah D. Schlitzer, Derek D. Reed, Justin C. Strickland

In this meta-analysis, we describe a benchmark value of delay and probability discounting reliability and stability that might be used to (a) evaluate the meaningfulness of clinically achieved changes in discounting and (b) support the role of discounting as a valid and enduring measure of intertemporal choice. We examined test–retest reliability, stability effect sizes (dz; Cohen, 1992), and relevant moderators across 30 publications comprising 39 independent samples and 262 measures of discounting, identified via a systematic review of PsychInfo, PubMed, and Google Scholar databases. We calculated omnibus effect-size estimates and evaluated the role of proposed moderators using a robust variance estimation meta-regression method. The meta-regression output reflected modest test–retest reliability, r = .670, p < .001, 95% CI [.618, .716]. Discounting was most reliable when measured in the context of temporal constraints, in adult respondents, when using money as a medium, and when reassessed within 1 month. Testing also suggested acceptable stability via nonsignificant and small changes in effect magnitude over time, dz = 0.048, p = .31, 95% CI [−0.051, 0.146]. Clinicians and researchers seeking to measure discounting can consider the contexts when reliability is maximized for specific cases.

在这项荟萃分析中,我们描述了延迟和概率折扣可靠性和稳定性的基准值,该基准值可用于:(a) 评估临床上实现的折扣变化的意义;(b) 支持折扣作为时际选择的有效和持久测量方法的作用。我们通过对 PsychInfo、PubMed 和 Google Scholar 数据库的系统性审查,对包含 39 个独立样本和 262 种折现测量方法的 30 篇出版物中的测试重复可靠性、稳定性效应大小(dz;Cohen,1992 年)和相关调节因素进行了研究。我们使用稳健方差估计元回归方法计算了总效应大小估计值,并评估了所提出的调节因子的作用。元回归结果反映了适度的重复测试可靠性,r = .670,p z = 0.048,p = .31,95% CI [-0.051, 0.146]。寻求测量折现的临床医生和研究人员可以考虑在特定情况下最大限度地提高可靠性。
{"title":"A systematic review and meta-analysis of test–retest reliability and stability of delay and probability discounting","authors":"Brett W. Gelino,&nbsp;Rebekah D. Schlitzer,&nbsp;Derek D. Reed,&nbsp;Justin C. Strickland","doi":"10.1002/jeab.910","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.910","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this meta-analysis, we describe a benchmark value of delay and probability discounting reliability and stability that might be used to (a) evaluate the meaningfulness of clinically achieved changes in discounting and (b) support the role of discounting as a valid and enduring measure of intertemporal choice. We examined test–retest reliability, stability effect sizes (<i>d</i><sub>z</sub>; Cohen, 1992), and relevant moderators across 30 publications comprising 39 independent samples and 262 measures of discounting, identified via a systematic review of PsychInfo, PubMed, and Google Scholar databases. We calculated omnibus effect-size estimates and evaluated the role of proposed moderators using a robust variance estimation meta-regression method. The meta-regression output reflected modest test–retest reliability, <i>r</i> = .670, <i>p</i> &lt; .001, 95% CI [.618, .716]. Discounting was most reliable when measured in the context of temporal constraints, in adult respondents, when using money as a medium, and when reassessed within 1 month. Testing also suggested acceptable stability via nonsignificant and small changes in effect magnitude over time, <i>d</i><sub>z</sub> = 0.048, <i>p</i> = .31, 95% CI [−0.051, 0.146]. Clinicians and researchers seeking to measure discounting can consider the contexts when reliability is maximized for specific cases.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"358-372"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140158383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analyses of relational coherence and rule following: Consistent liars are preferred over occasional truth tellers 对关系一致性和规则遵循的分析:一致的说谎者比偶尔说真话的人更受欢迎。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.907
Jesús Alonso-Vega, Colin Harte, Dermot Barnes-Holmes

The current study explored the influence of different levels of speaker coherence on rule following and speaker preference. In Experiment 1, rules provided by three different speakers were either 100% accurate, 0% accurate, or 50% accurate/inaccurate. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except that the speaker's coherence was adjusted to 80% accurate, 20% accurate, and 50% accurate/inaccurate, respectively. Overall, participants tended to follow coherent speaker rules and avoid following incoherent speaker rules during training and testing phases. The results also indicated that following and not following rules provided by speakers may be generalizable to novel stimuli and maintained in the absence of differential reinforcement (i.e., in experimental test phases). Additionally, in a preference test, participants tended to prefer coherent over incoherent and partially coherent speakers. Furthermore, participants tended to prefer the relatively more incoherent speaker (i.e., 0% or 20% accurate) over the 50% accurate coherent speaker in both experiments. Finally, a comparison of the results of both experiments indicated that different levels of relational coherence affected the variability of rule-following and speaker preference behaviors. These findings are discussed in the context of the complexities that appear to be involved in rule-following behaviors and speaker preference.

本研究探讨了不同程度的说话者连贯性对规则遵循和说话者偏好的影响。在实验 1 中,由三个不同说话者提供的规则要么是 100% 准确,要么是 0% 准确,要么是 50% 准确/不准确。实验 2 与实验 1 相似,只是说话者的连贯度分别调整为 80%准确、20% 准确和 50%准确/不准确。总体而言,在训练和测试阶段,参与者倾向于遵循连贯的说话者规则,而避免遵循不连贯的说话者规则。研究结果还表明,遵循和不遵循说话者提供的规则可能会对新刺激产生普遍影响,并且在没有差异强化的情况下(即在实验测试阶段)仍能保持。此外,在偏好测试中,与不连贯和部分连贯的说话者相比,参与者倾向于偏好连贯的说话者。此外,在两次实验中,相对于准确率为 50%的连贯说话者,参与者倾向于更喜欢不连贯说话者(即准确率为 0% 或 20%)。最后,对两个实验结果的比较表明,不同程度的关系一致性会影响规则遵循和说话者偏好行为的变化。我们将结合规则遵循行为和说话者偏好所涉及的复杂性来讨论这些发现。
{"title":"Analyses of relational coherence and rule following: Consistent liars are preferred over occasional truth tellers","authors":"Jesús Alonso-Vega,&nbsp;Colin Harte,&nbsp;Dermot Barnes-Holmes","doi":"10.1002/jeab.907","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.907","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study explored the influence of different levels of speaker coherence on rule following and speaker preference. In Experiment 1, rules provided by three different speakers were either 100% accurate, 0% accurate, or 50% accurate/inaccurate. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except that the speaker's coherence was adjusted to 80% accurate, 20% accurate, and 50% accurate/inaccurate, respectively. Overall, participants tended to follow coherent speaker rules and avoid following incoherent speaker rules during training and testing phases. The results also indicated that following and not following rules provided by speakers may be generalizable to novel stimuli and maintained in the absence of differential reinforcement (i.e., in experimental test phases). Additionally, in a preference test, participants tended to prefer coherent over incoherent and partially coherent speakers. Furthermore, participants tended to prefer the relatively more incoherent speaker (i.e., 0% or 20% accurate) over the 50% accurate coherent speaker in both experiments. Finally, a comparison of the results of both experiments indicated that different levels of relational coherence affected the variability of rule-following and speaker preference behaviors. These findings are discussed in the context of the complexities that appear to be involved in rule-following behaviors and speaker preference.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"281-293"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jeab.907","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139996606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of reinforcement and its omission on trial-by-trial changes of response bias in perceptual decision making 强化和省略对知觉决策中逐次试验反应偏差变化的影响
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.908
Maik C. Stüttgen, Andrea Dietl, Vanya V. Stoilova Eckert, Luis de la Cuesta-Ferrer, Jan-Hendrik Blanke, Christina Koß, Frank Jäkel

Discrimination performance in perceptual choice tasks is known to reflect both sensory discriminability and nonsensory response bias. In the framework of signal detection theory, these aspects of discrimination performance are quantified through separate measures, sensitivity (d′) for sensory discriminability and decision criterion (c) for response bias. However, it is unknown how response bias (i.e., criterion) changes at the single-trial level as a consequence of reinforcement history. We subjected rats to a two-stimulus two-response conditional discrimination task with auditory stimuli and induced response bias through unequal reinforcement probabilities for the two responses. We compared three signal-detection-theory-based criterion learning models with respect to their ability to fit experimentally observed fluctuations of response bias on a trial-by-trial level. These models shift the criterion by a fixed step (1) after each reinforced response or (2) after each nonreinforced response or (3) after both. We find that all three models fail to capture essential aspects of the data. Prompted by the observation that steady-state criterion values conformed well to a behavioral model of signal detection based on the generalized matching law, we constructed a trial-based version of this model and find that it provides a superior account of response bias fluctuations under changing reinforcement contingencies.

众所周知,知觉选择任务中的辨别能力既反映了感官辨别能力,也反映了非感官反应偏差。在信号检测理论的框架下,分辨成绩的这两个方面是通过单独的测量来量化的,即感官可分辨性的灵敏度(d')和反应偏差的决策标准(c)。然而,在单次试验水平上,反应偏差(即标准)是如何随强化历史而变化的,目前还不得而知。我们用听觉刺激对大鼠进行双刺激双反应条件辨别任务,并通过两种反应的不等强化概率诱导反应偏差。我们比较了三种基于信号检测理论的标准学习模型,看它们是否能够适应实验观察到的逐次试验水平的反应偏差波动。这些模型通过一个固定的步骤(1)在每次强化反应之后或(2)在每次非强化反应之后或(3)在两者之后移动标准。我们发现这三种模型都无法捕捉到数据的基本方面。在观察到稳态标准值与基于广义匹配定律的信号检测行为模型十分吻合后,我们构建了该模型的试验版本,并发现它能更好地解释强化条件变化下的反应偏差波动。
{"title":"Influence of reinforcement and its omission on trial-by-trial changes of response bias in perceptual decision making","authors":"Maik C. Stüttgen,&nbsp;Andrea Dietl,&nbsp;Vanya V. Stoilova Eckert,&nbsp;Luis de la Cuesta-Ferrer,&nbsp;Jan-Hendrik Blanke,&nbsp;Christina Koß,&nbsp;Frank Jäkel","doi":"10.1002/jeab.908","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.908","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Discrimination performance in perceptual choice tasks is known to reflect both sensory discriminability and nonsensory response bias. In the framework of signal detection theory, these aspects of discrimination performance are quantified through separate measures, sensitivity (<i>d′</i>) for sensory discriminability and decision criterion (<i>c</i>) for response bias. However, it is unknown how response bias (i.e., criterion) changes at the single-trial level as a consequence of reinforcement history. We subjected rats to a two-stimulus two-response conditional discrimination task with auditory stimuli and induced response bias through unequal reinforcement probabilities for the two responses. We compared three signal-detection-theory-based criterion learning models with respect to their ability to fit experimentally observed fluctuations of response bias on a trial-by-trial level. These models shift the criterion by a fixed step (1) after each reinforced response or (2) after each nonreinforced response or (3) after both. We find that all three models fail to capture essential aspects of the data. Prompted by the observation that steady-state criterion values conformed well to a behavioral model of signal detection based on the generalized matching law, we constructed a trial-based version of this model and find that it provides a superior account of response bias fluctuations under changing reinforcement contingencies.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 3","pages":"294-313"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jeab.908","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139996607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial: Process and progress 社论:过程与进步。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.906
Suzanne H. Mitchell
{"title":"Editorial: Process and progress","authors":"Suzanne H. Mitchell","doi":"10.1002/jeab.906","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.906","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 2","pages":"147-150"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139931755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use of preexposure prophylaxis and condom purchasing decisions 暴露前预防措施的使用和购买安全套的决定。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.905
Nioud Mulugeta Gebru, Justin C. Strickland, Derek D. Reed, Christopher W. Kahler, Robert F. Leeman

Preexposure prophylaxis (PrEP) prevents human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) but not other sexually transmitted infections (STIs). Men who have sex with men (MSM) who take PrEP tend to report reduced condom use, but little is known about the underlying mechanisms. For this study, MSM who take PrEP (i.e., PrEP experienced; n = 88) and MSM who do not (i.e., PrEP naïve; n = 113) completed an online study, including the condom purchase task (CoPT). The CoPT assesses decisions to purchase condoms across escalating prices (range: free–$55) for sex with different types of hypothetical partners: those least likely to have an STD (least STD) and those that participants most want to have sex with (most want sex with). When condoms were free, PrEP-experienced MSM had a lower rate of condom purchasing than did PrEP-naïve MSM. For both partner types, PrEP-experienced MSM reached a price break point (i.e., would not buy condoms) at a lower price than did PrEP-naïve pariticipants. For the most-want-sex-with partner at the price at which participants elected not to buy condoms, only 23% of PrEP-experienced MSM chose to abstain from sex when not purchasing condoms versus 53% among PrEP-naïve MSM. Similar patterns were observed for the least-STD partner. The results support the potential utility of the CoPT in identifying behavioral mechanisms related to condom use and PrEP.

预防性暴露疗法(PrEP)可预防人类免疫缺陷病毒(HIV),但不能预防其他性传播感染(STI)。服用 PrEP 的男男性行为者(MSM)往往会报告安全套使用率下降,但人们对其背后的机制知之甚少。在这项研究中,服用 PrEP 的 MSM(即有 PrEP 经验者;n = 88)和没有服用 PrEP 的 MSM(即没有 PrEP 经验者;n = 113)完成了一项在线研究,包括安全套购买任务(CoPT)。CoPT 评估的是在价格不断攀升(范围:免费-55 美元)的情况下购买安全套的决定,以便与不同类型的假想伴侣发生性关系:最不可能感染性传播疾病的伴侣(最不可能感染性传播疾病)和参与者最想发生性关系的伴侣(最想发生性关系)。当安全套免费提供时,有 PrEP 经验的 MSM 购买安全套的比例低于无 PrEP 经验的 MSM。对于这两种伴侣类型,有 PrEP 经验的 MSM 在达到价格断点(即不购买安全套)时的价格低于 PrEP 未尝试者。对于最想与之发生性关系的性伴侣,在参与者选择不购买安全套的价格上,只有 23% 的有 PrEP 经验的 MSM 在不购买安全套的情况下选择放弃性生活,而在没有 PrEP 经验的 MSM 中,这一比例为 53%。在性传播疾病最少的性伴侣中也观察到了类似的模式。这些结果支持了 CoPT 在确定与安全套使用和 PrEP 相关的行为机制方面的潜在作用。
{"title":"Use of preexposure prophylaxis and condom purchasing decisions","authors":"Nioud Mulugeta Gebru,&nbsp;Justin C. Strickland,&nbsp;Derek D. Reed,&nbsp;Christopher W. Kahler,&nbsp;Robert F. Leeman","doi":"10.1002/jeab.905","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.905","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Preexposure prophylaxis (PrEP) prevents human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) but not other sexually transmitted infections (STIs). Men who have sex with men (MSM) who take PrEP tend to report reduced condom use, but little is known about the underlying mechanisms. For this study, MSM who take PrEP (i.e., PrEP experienced; <i>n</i> = 88) and MSM who do not (i.e., PrEP naïve; <i>n</i> = 113) completed an online study, including the condom purchase task (CoPT). The CoPT assesses decisions to purchase condoms across escalating prices (range: free–$55) for sex with different types of hypothetical partners: those least likely to have an STD (least STD) and those that participants most want to have sex with (most want sex with). When condoms were free, PrEP-experienced MSM had a lower rate of condom purchasing than did PrEP-naïve MSM. For both partner types, PrEP-experienced MSM reached a price break point (i.e., would not buy condoms) at a lower price than did PrEP-naïve pariticipants. For the most-want-sex-with partner at the price at which participants elected not to buy condoms, only 23% of PrEP-experienced MSM chose to abstain from sex when not purchasing condoms versus 53% among PrEP-naïve MSM. Similar patterns were observed for the least-STD partner. The results support the potential utility of the CoPT in identifying behavioral mechanisms related to condom use and PrEP.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 2","pages":"233-245"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139735551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of a training package to teach note taking on the formation of equivalence classes 笔记教学培训教材对等价类形成的影响。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.903
Sarah E. Frampton, Emily Linehan

Effective note taking may enhance learning outcomes for students and serve as a directly observable form of mediation within a test context. Frampton et al. (2023) used stimulus fading to teach note taking in the form of a graphic organizer (GO) during matching-to-sample baseline relations training (MTS-BRT). Moderately high yields were observed with young adults despite the use of linear series training, abstract stimuli, and five-member classes. The present study taught the same note taking strategy using an intervention package including video illustration, voice-over instructions, and feedback to eight college students. Participants were taught to construct the GO during MTS-BRT with three three-member classes of familiar stimuli. Then the effects of MTS-BRT alone with three five-member classes of abstract stimuli was evaluated. Participants efficiently completed training with familiar stimuli and passed the posttest on the first attempt. With the abstract stimuli, participants engaged in GO construction during MTS-BRT and the six participants that demonstrated high levels of fidelity to the trained note taking strategy passed the posttest on the first attempt. These results replicate findings from Frampton et al. while using a more efficient intervention package. Benefits of teaching overt mediation responses are discussed as well as future directions for translation to applied contexts.

有效的笔记可以提高学生的学习效果,并在测试中作为一种可直接观察到的调解形式。Frampton 等人(2023 年)在匹配与样本基线关系训练(MTS-BRT)中使用刺激消退法,以图形组织器(GO)的形式教授记笔记。尽管使用了线性序列训练、抽象刺激和五人班,但在年轻人身上观察到了中等偏上的收益率。本研究使用包括视频插图、画外音指导和反馈在内的干预包,向八名大学生教授了相同的笔记策略。在 MTS-BRT 过程中,教导参与者用三个三元类的熟悉刺激构建 GO。然后评估了单独使用三类五人抽象刺激进行 MTS-BRT 的效果。学员高效地完成了熟悉刺激的训练,并在首次尝试时通过了后测。在使用抽象刺激物时,学员在 MTS-BRT 过程中参与了 GO 构建,其中六名学员表现出了对训练笔记策略的高度忠诚,并在首次尝试时通过了后测。这些结果重复了 Frampton 等人的研究结果,同时使用了更有效的干预包。本文讨论了公开调解反应教学的益处以及未来应用的方向。
{"title":"The effects of a training package to teach note taking on the formation of equivalence classes","authors":"Sarah E. Frampton,&nbsp;Emily Linehan","doi":"10.1002/jeab.903","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jeab.903","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Effective note taking may enhance learning outcomes for students and serve as a directly observable form of mediation within a test context. Frampton et al. (2023) used stimulus fading to teach note taking in the form of a graphic organizer (GO) during matching-to-sample baseline relations training (MTS-BRT). Moderately high yields were observed with young adults despite the use of linear series training, abstract stimuli, and five-member classes. The present study taught the same note taking strategy using an intervention package including video illustration, voice-over instructions, and feedback to eight college students. Participants were taught to construct the GO during MTS-BRT with three three-member classes of familiar stimuli. Then the effects of MTS-BRT alone with three five-member classes of abstract stimuli was evaluated. Participants efficiently completed training with familiar stimuli and passed the posttest on the first attempt. With the abstract stimuli, participants engaged in GO construction during MTS-BRT and the six participants that demonstrated high levels of fidelity to the trained note taking strategy passed the posttest on the first attempt. These results replicate findings from Frampton et al. while using a more efficient intervention package. Benefits of teaching overt mediation responses are discussed as well as future directions for translation to applied contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":17411,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior","volume":"121 2","pages":"218-232"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jeab.903","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139723170","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1