首页 > 最新文献

Learning & memory最新文献

英文 中文
The influence of encoding strategy on associative memory consolidation across wake and sleep. 编码策略对清醒和睡眠时联想记忆巩固的影响。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-19 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053765.123
Dan Denis, Ryan Bottary, Tony J Cunningham, Mario-Cyriac Tcheukado, Jessica D Payne

Sleep benefits memory consolidation. However, factors present at initial encoding may moderate this effect. Here, we examined the role that encoding strategy plays in subsequent memory consolidation during sleep. Eighty-nine participants encoded pairs of words using two different strategies. Each participant encoded half of the word pairs using an integrative visualization technique, where the two items were imagined in an integrated scene. The other half were encoded nonintegratively, with each word pair item visualized separately. Memory was tested before and after a period of nocturnal sleep (N = 47) or daytime wake (N = 42) via cued recall tests. Immediate memory performance was significantly better for word pairs encoded using the integrative strategy compared with the nonintegrative strategy (P < 0.001). When looking at the change in recall across the delay, there was significantly less forgetting of integrated word pairs across a night of sleep compared with a day spent awake (P < 0.001), with no significant difference in the nonintegrated pairs (P = 0.19). This finding was driven by more forgetting of integrated compared with not-integrated pairs across the wake delay (P < 0.001), whereas forgetting was equivalent across the sleep delay (P = 0.26). Together, these results show that the strategy engaged in during encoding impacts both the immediate retention of memories and their subsequent consolidation across sleep and wake intervals.

睡眠有利于巩固记忆。然而,在最初编码时存在的因素可能会缓和这种影响。在这里,我们研究了编码策略在睡眠期间后续记忆巩固中所起的作用。89名参与者使用两种不同的策略对成对的单词进行编码。每个参与者使用综合可视化技术编码了一半的单词对,在综合场景中想象这两个项目。另一半是非集成编码的,每个单词对项目都是单独可视化的。在夜间睡眠(N=47)或日间觉醒(N=42)前后,通过提示回忆测试测试记忆力。与非整合策略相比,使用整合策略编码的单词对的即时记忆表现明显更好(P<0.001)。当观察整个延迟期间回忆的变化时,与清醒的一天相比,睡眠一晚对整合单词对的遗忘明显更少(P<001),在非积分对中没有显著差异(P=0.19)。这一发现是由于在整个觉醒延迟期间,与未积分对相比,积分对的遗忘更多(P<0.001),而在整个睡眠延迟期间,遗忘是等效的(P=0.26),这些结果表明,在编码过程中所采用的策略会影响记忆的即时保留以及随后在睡眠和清醒时间段内的巩固。
{"title":"The influence of encoding strategy on associative memory consolidation across wake and sleep.","authors":"Dan Denis,&nbsp;Ryan Bottary,&nbsp;Tony J Cunningham,&nbsp;Mario-Cyriac Tcheukado,&nbsp;Jessica D Payne","doi":"10.1101/lm.053765.123","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053765.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sleep benefits memory consolidation. However, factors present at initial encoding may moderate this effect. Here, we examined the role that encoding strategy plays in subsequent memory consolidation during sleep. Eighty-nine participants encoded pairs of words using two different strategies. Each participant encoded half of the word pairs using an integrative visualization technique, where the two items were imagined in an integrated scene. The other half were encoded nonintegratively, with each word pair item visualized separately. Memory was tested before and after a period of nocturnal sleep (<i>N</i> = 47) or daytime wake (<i>N</i> = 42) via cued recall tests. Immediate memory performance was significantly better for word pairs encoded using the integrative strategy compared with the nonintegrative strategy (<i>P</i> < 0.001). When looking at the change in recall across the delay, there was significantly less forgetting of integrated word pairs across a night of sleep compared with a day spent awake (<i>P</i> < 0.001), with no significant difference in the nonintegrated pairs (<i>P</i> = 0.19). This finding was driven by more forgetting of integrated compared with not-integrated pairs across the wake delay (<i>P</i> < 0.001), whereas forgetting was equivalent across the sleep delay (<i>P</i> = 0.26). Together, these results show that the strategy engaged in during encoding impacts both the immediate retention of memories and their subsequent consolidation across sleep and wake intervals.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 9","pages":"185-191"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547373/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41121819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental changes in retention and generalization of nonadjacent dependencies over a period containing sleep in 18-mo-old infants. 18个月大婴儿在睡眠期内非相邻依赖性的保留和概括的发育变化。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-19 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053772.123
Lucia M Sweeney, Hatty Lara, Rebecca L Gómez

Sleep promotes the stabilization of memories in adulthood, with a growing literature on the benefits of sleep for memory in infants and children. In two studies, we examined the role of sleep in the retention and generalization of nonadjacent dependencies (NADs; e.g., a-X-b/c-X-d phrases) in an artificial language. Previously, a study demonstrated that over a delay of 4 h, 15 mo olds who nap after training retain a general memory of the NAD rule instead of memory for specific NADs heard during training. In experiment 1, we designed a replication of the nap condition used in the earlier study but tested 18-mo-old infants. Infants of this age retained veridical memory for specific NADs over a delay containing sleep, providing preliminary evidence of the development of memory processes (experiment 1). In experiment 2, we tested 18 mo olds' ability to generalize the NAD to new vocabulary, finding only infants who napped after training generalized their knowledge of the pattern to completely novel phrases. Overall, by 18 mo of age, children retain specific memories over a period containing sleep, and sleep promotes abstract memories to a greater extent than wakefulness.

睡眠促进成年后记忆的稳定,越来越多的文献报道睡眠对婴儿和儿童记忆的好处。在两项研究中,我们研究了睡眠在人工语言中非相邻依赖性(NADs;例如a-X-b/c-X-d短语)的保留和泛化中的作用。此前,一项研究表明,经过4小时的延迟,训练后打盹的15个月大的孩子会保留对NAD规则的一般记忆,而不是对训练中听到的特定NAD的记忆。在实验1中,我们设计了一个复制早期研究中使用的小睡条件,但对18个月大的婴儿进行了测试。这个年龄段的婴儿在含有睡眠的延迟中保留了对特定NAD的真实记忆,为记忆过程的发展提供了初步证据(实验1)。在实验2中,我们测试了18个月大的婴儿将NAD概括为新词汇的能力,发现只有在训练后打盹的婴儿将他们对模式的知识概括为完全新颖的短语。总的来说,到18个月大时,孩子们会在一段包含睡眠的时间内保留特定的记忆,而睡眠比清醒更能促进抽象记忆。
{"title":"Developmental changes in retention and generalization of nonadjacent dependencies over a period containing sleep in 18-mo-old infants.","authors":"Lucia M Sweeney, Hatty Lara, Rebecca L Gómez","doi":"10.1101/lm.053772.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053772.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sleep promotes the stabilization of memories in adulthood, with a growing literature on the benefits of sleep for memory in infants and children. In two studies, we examined the role of sleep in the retention and generalization of nonadjacent dependencies (NADs; e.g., a-X-b/c-X-d phrases) in an artificial language. Previously, a study demonstrated that over a delay of 4 h, 15 mo olds who nap after training retain a general memory of the NAD rule instead of memory for specific NADs heard during training. In experiment 1, we designed a replication of the nap condition used in the earlier study but tested 18-mo-old infants. Infants of this age retained veridical memory for specific NADs over a delay containing sleep, providing preliminary evidence of the development of memory processes (experiment 1). In experiment 2, we tested 18 mo olds' ability to generalize the NAD to new vocabulary, finding only infants who napped after training generalized their knowledge of the pattern to completely novel phrases. Overall, by 18 mo of age, children retain specific memories over a period containing sleep, and sleep promotes abstract memories to a greater extent than wakefulness.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 9","pages":"212-220"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547371/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41140071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Special issue on sleep and memory. 睡眠与记忆特刊。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-19 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053886.123
{"title":"Special issue on sleep and memory.","authors":"","doi":"10.1101/lm.053886.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053886.123","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 9","pages":"v"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547372/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41124751","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sleep consolidates stimulus-response learning. 睡眠巩固了刺激反应学习。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-19 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053753.123
Xiu Miao, Carolin Müller, Nicolas D Lutz, Qing Yang, Florian Waszak, Jan Born, Karsten Rauss

Performing a motor response to a sensory stimulus creates a memory trace whose behavioral correlates are classically investigated in terms of repetition priming effects. Such stimulus-response learning entails two types of associations that are partly independent: (1) an association between the stimulus and the motor response and (2) an association between the stimulus and the classification task in which it is encountered. Here, we tested whether sleep supports long-lasting stimulus-response learning on a task requiring participants (1) for establishing stimulus-classification associations to classify presented objects along two different dimensions ("size" and "mechanical") and (2) as motor response (action) to respond with either the left or right index finger. Moreover, we examined whether strengthening of stimulus-classification associations is preferentially linked to nonrapid eye movement (non-REM) sleep and strengthening of stimulus-action associations to REM sleep. We tested 48 healthy volunteers in a between-subjects design comparing postlearning retention periods of nighttime sleep versus daytime wakefulness. At postretention testing, we found that sleep supports consolidation of both stimulus-action and stimulus-classification associations, as indicated by increased reaction times in "switch conditions"; that is, when, at test, the acutely instructed classification task and/or correct motor response for a given stimulus differed from that during original learning. Polysomnographic recordings revealed that both kinds of associations were correlated with non-REM spindle activity. Our results do not support the view of differential roles for non-REM and REM sleep in the consolidation of stimulus-classification and stimulus-action associations, respectively.

对感觉刺激进行运动反应会产生记忆痕迹,其行为相关性在重复启动效应方面得到了经典的研究。这种刺激-反应学习需要两种部分独立的关联:(1)刺激和运动反应之间的关联;(2)刺激和遇到的分类任务之间的关联。在这里,我们测试了睡眠是否支持对一项任务的长期刺激反应学习,该任务要求参与者(1)建立刺激分类关联,以沿着两个不同的维度(“大小”和“机械”)对呈现的物体进行分类,以及(2)将其作为运动反应(动作),用左手或右手食指做出反应。此外,我们还研究了刺激分类关联的加强是否优先与非快速眼动(non-REM)睡眠有关,以及刺激动作关联的加强与REM睡眠有关。我们在受试者之间的设计中测试了48名健康志愿者,比较了学习后夜间睡眠和白天清醒的保持期。在保留后测试中,我们发现睡眠支持刺激动作和刺激分类关联的巩固,如“切换条件”下反应时间的增加所示;也就是说,在测试中,当对给定刺激的敏锐指示的分类任务和/或正确的运动反应与最初学习时不同时。多导睡眠图记录显示,这两种关联都与非快速眼动纺锤体活动相关。我们的研究结果不支持非快速眼动睡眠和快速眼动睡眠分别在刺激分类和刺激-动作关联的整合中发挥不同作用的观点。
{"title":"Sleep consolidates stimulus-response learning.","authors":"Xiu Miao, Carolin Müller, Nicolas D Lutz, Qing Yang, Florian Waszak, Jan Born, Karsten Rauss","doi":"10.1101/lm.053753.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053753.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Performing a motor response to a sensory stimulus creates a memory trace whose behavioral correlates are classically investigated in terms of repetition priming effects. Such stimulus-response learning entails two types of associations that are partly independent: (1) an association between the stimulus and the motor response and (2) an association between the stimulus and the classification task in which it is encountered. Here, we tested whether sleep supports long-lasting stimulus-response learning on a task requiring participants (1) for establishing stimulus-classification associations to classify presented objects along two different dimensions (\"size\" and \"mechanical\") and (2) as motor response (action) to respond with either the left or right index finger. Moreover, we examined whether strengthening of stimulus-classification associations is preferentially linked to nonrapid eye movement (non-REM) sleep and strengthening of stimulus-action associations to REM sleep. We tested 48 healthy volunteers in a between-subjects design comparing postlearning retention periods of nighttime sleep versus daytime wakefulness. At postretention testing, we found that sleep supports consolidation of both stimulus-action and stimulus-classification associations, as indicated by increased reaction times in \"switch conditions\"; that is, when, at test, the acutely instructed classification task and/or correct motor response for a given stimulus differed from that during original learning. Polysomnographic recordings revealed that both kinds of associations were correlated with non-REM spindle activity. Our results do not support the view of differential roles for non-REM and REM sleep in the consolidation of stimulus-classification and stimulus-action associations, respectively.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 9","pages":"175-184"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547380/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41104162","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A reward effect on memory retention, consolidation, and generalization? 对记忆保持、巩固和概括的奖励效应?
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-07 Print Date: 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053842.123
Heidrun Schultz, Hanna Stoffregen, Roland G Benoit

Reward improves memory through both encoding and consolidation processes. In this preregistered study, we tested whether reward effects on memory generalize from high-rewarded items to low-rewarded but episodically related items. Fifty-nine human volunteers incidentally encoded associations between unique objects and repeated scenes. Some scenes typically yielded high reward, whereas others typically yielded low reward. Memory was tested immediately after encoding (n = 29) or the next day (n = 30). Overall, reward had only a limited influence on memory. It did not enhance consolidation and its effect did not generalize to episodically related stimuli. We thus contribute to understanding the boundary conditions of reward effects on memory.

奖励通过编码和整合过程提高记忆力。在这项预先注册的研究中,我们测试了奖励对记忆的影响是否从高奖励项目推广到低奖励但偶发相关的项目。59名人类志愿者偶然编码了独特物体和重复场景之间的关联。有些场景通常获得高回报,而另一些场景通常获得低回报。在编码后立即(n=29)或第二天(n=30)测试记忆力。总的来说,奖励对记忆力的影响有限。它并没有增强巩固作用,其作用也没有推广到偶发性相关刺激。因此,我们有助于理解奖励效应对记忆的边界条件。
{"title":"A reward effect on memory retention, consolidation, and generalization?","authors":"Heidrun Schultz, Hanna Stoffregen, Roland G Benoit","doi":"10.1101/lm.053842.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053842.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reward improves memory through both encoding and consolidation processes. In this preregistered study, we tested whether reward effects on memory generalize from high-rewarded items to low-rewarded but episodically related items. Fifty-nine human volunteers incidentally encoded associations between unique objects and repeated scenes. Some scenes typically yielded high reward, whereas others typically yielded low reward. Memory was tested immediately after encoding (<i>n</i> = 29) or the next day (<i>n</i> = 30). Overall, reward had only a limited influence on memory. It did not enhance consolidation and its effect did not generalize to episodically related stimuli. We thus contribute to understanding the boundary conditions of reward effects on memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 8","pages":"169-174"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519399/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10252435","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chemogenetic activation of the ventral subiculum-BNST pathway reduces context fear expression. 腹侧下托BNST通路的化学遗传学激活减少了上下文恐惧的表达。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-24 Print Date: 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053797.123
Leeza Kopaeva, Alexandrina Yakimov, Louise Urien, Elizabeth P Bauer

An inability to reduce fear in nonthreatening environments characterizes many anxiety disorders. The pathway from the ventral subiculum (vSUB) to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) is more active in safe contexts than in aversive ones, as indexed by FOS expression. Here, we used chemogenetic techniques to specifically activate the vSUB-BNST pathway during both context and cued fear expression by expressing a Cre-dependent hM3D(Gq) receptor in BNST-projecting vSUB neurons. Activation of the vSUB-BNST pathway reduced context but not cued fear expression. These data suggest that the vSUB-BNST pathway contributes to behavioral responses to nonaversive contexts.

在没有威胁的环境中无法减少恐惧是许多焦虑症的特征。根据FOS的表达,从腹侧下托(vSUB)到终纹床核(BNST)的通路在安全环境中比在厌恶环境中更活跃。在这里,我们使用化学遗传学技术,通过在BNST投射的vSUB神经元中表达Cre依赖性hM3D(Gq)受体,在上下文和提示的恐惧表达过程中特异性激活vSUB BNST通路。vSUB-BNST通路的激活减少了上下文,但没有提示恐惧的表达。这些数据表明,vSUB-BNST通路有助于对非可逆环境的行为反应。
{"title":"Chemogenetic activation of the ventral subiculum-BNST pathway reduces context fear expression.","authors":"Leeza Kopaeva, Alexandrina Yakimov, Louise Urien, Elizabeth P Bauer","doi":"10.1101/lm.053797.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053797.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An inability to reduce fear in nonthreatening environments characterizes many anxiety disorders. The pathway from the ventral subiculum (vSUB) to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) is more active in safe contexts than in aversive ones, as indexed by FOS expression. Here, we used chemogenetic techniques to specifically activate the vSUB-BNST pathway during both context and cued fear expression by expressing a Cre-dependent hM3D(Gq) receptor in BNST-projecting vSUB neurons. Activation of the vSUB-BNST pathway reduced context but not cued fear expression. These data suggest that the vSUB-BNST pathway contributes to behavioral responses to nonaversive contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 8","pages":"164-168"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-08-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519403/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10435030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interpolated retrieval retroactively increases recall and promotes cross-episode memory interdependence. 内插检索可追溯地增加回忆,并促进跨事件记忆的相互依赖性。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-15 Print Date: 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053782.123
Christopher N Wahlheim, Sydney T Smith, Sydney M Garlitch, Robert W Wiley

Retrieving existing memories before new learning can lead to retroactive facilitation. Three experiments examined whether interpolated retrieval is associated with retroactive facilitation and memory interdependence that reflects integrative encoding. Participants studied two lists of cue-response word pairs that repeated across lists (A-B, A-B), appeared in list 1 (A-B, -), or included the same cues with changed responses in each list (A-B, A-C). For A-B, A-C pairs, the tasks interpolated between lists required recall of list 1 (B) responses (with or without feedback) or restudy of complete list 1 (A-B) pairs. In list 2, participants only studied pairs (experiment 1) or studied pairs, attempted to detect changed (C) responses, and attempted to recall list 1 responses for detected changes (experiments 2 and 3). On a final cued recall test, participants attempted to recall list 1 responses, indicated whether responses changed between lists, and if so, attempted to recall list 2 responses. Interpolated retrieval was associated with subsequent retroactive facilitation and greater memory interdependence for B and C responses. These correlational findings are compatible with the view that retrieval retroactively facilitates memories, promotes coactivation of existing memories and new learning, and enables integrative encoding that veridically binds information across episodes.

在新的学习之前检索现有的记忆可以促进追溯。三个实验检验了插值检索是否与反映综合编码的追溯促进和记忆相互依赖有关。参与者研究了两个线索-反应词对列表,它们在列表中重复(A-B,A-B),出现在列表1中(A-B、-),或在每个列表中包含相同的线索,但反应发生了变化(A-B和A-C)。对于A-B、A-C对,在列表之间插入的任务需要回忆列表1(B)响应(有或没有反馈)或重新研究完整的列表1(A-B)对。在列表2中,参与者只研究配对(实验1)或研究配对,试图检测变化的(C)反应,并试图回忆列表1中检测到的变化的反应(实验2和3)。在最后的提示回忆测试中,参与者试图回忆列表1的回答,指出回答是否在列表之间发生了变化,如果是,则尝试回忆列表2的回答。内插检索与B和C反应的后续追溯促进和更大的记忆相互依赖性有关。这些相关发现与这样一种观点相一致,即检索可以追溯性地促进记忆,促进现有记忆和新学习的共同激活,并实现整合编码,从而真实地将信息绑定到不同的事件中。
{"title":"Interpolated retrieval retroactively increases recall and promotes cross-episode memory interdependence.","authors":"Christopher N Wahlheim, Sydney T Smith, Sydney M Garlitch, Robert W Wiley","doi":"10.1101/lm.053782.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053782.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Retrieving existing memories before new learning can lead to retroactive facilitation. Three experiments examined whether interpolated retrieval is associated with retroactive facilitation and memory interdependence that reflects integrative encoding. Participants studied two lists of cue-response word pairs that repeated across lists (A-B, A-B), appeared in list 1 (A-B, -), or included the same cues with changed responses in each list (A-B, A-C). For A-B, A-C pairs, the tasks interpolated between lists required recall of list 1 (B) responses (with or without feedback) or restudy of complete list 1 (A-B) pairs. In list 2, participants only studied pairs (experiment 1) or studied pairs, attempted to detect changed (C) responses, and attempted to recall list 1 responses for detected changes (experiments 2 and 3). On a final cued recall test, participants attempted to recall list 1 responses, indicated whether responses changed between lists, and if so, attempted to recall list 2 responses. Interpolated retrieval was associated with subsequent retroactive facilitation and greater memory interdependence for B and C responses. These correlational findings are compatible with the view that retrieval retroactively facilitates memories, promotes coactivation of existing memories and new learning, and enables integrative encoding that veridically binds information across episodes.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 8","pages":"151-163"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519378/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10003475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring human context fear conditioning and retention after consolidation. 测量人类环境对巩固后的恐惧条件和保留。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-08 Print Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053781.123
Yanfang Xia, Jelena Wehrli, Samuel Gerster, Marijn Kroes, Maxime Houtekamer, Dominik R Bach

Fear conditioning is a laboratory paradigm commonly used to investigate aversive learning and memory. In context fear conditioning, a configuration of elemental cues (conditioned stimulus [CTX]) predicts an aversive event (unconditioned stimulus [US]). To quantify context fear acquisition in humans, previous work has used startle eyeblink responses (SEBRs), skin conductance responses (SCRs), and verbal reports, but different quantification methods have rarely been compared. Moreover, preclinical intervention studies mandate recall tests several days after acquisition, and it is unclear how to induce and measure context fear memory retention over such a time interval. First, we used a semi-immersive virtual reality paradigm. In two experiments (N = 23 and N = 28), we found successful declarative learning and memory retention over 7 d but no evidence of other conditioned responses. Next, we used a configural fear conditioning paradigm with five static room images as CTXs in two experiments (N = 29 and N = 24). Besides successful declarative learning and memory retention after 7 d, SCR and pupil dilation in response to CTX onset differentiated CTX+/CTX- during acquisition training, and SEBR and pupil dilation differentiated CTX+/CTX- during the recall test, with medium to large effect sizes for the most sensitive indices (SEBR: Hedge's g = 0.56 and g = 0.69; pupil dilation: Hedge's g = 0.99 and g = 0.88). Our results demonstrate that with a configural learning paradigm, context fear memory retention can be demonstrated over 7 d, and we provide robust and replicable measurement methods to this end.

恐惧条件反射是一种实验室范式,通常用于研究厌恶性学习和记忆。在上下文恐惧条件反射中,基本线索的配置(条件刺激[CTX])预测厌恶事件(非条件刺激[US])。为了量化人类的情境恐惧获取,先前的工作使用了惊跳眨眼反应(SEBR)、皮肤电导反应(SCR)和言语报告,但很少比较不同的量化方法。此外,临床前干预研究要求在获得后几天进行回忆测试,目前尚不清楚如何在这样的时间间隔内诱导和测量情境恐惧记忆保持。首先,我们使用了一种半沉浸式虚拟现实范式。在两个实验中(N=23和N=28),我们发现在7天内成功的陈述性学习和记忆保持,但没有其他条件反应的证据。接下来,我们在两个实验(N=29和N=24)中使用了五个静态房间图像作为CTX的结构恐惧条件反射范式。除了7天后成功的陈述性学习和记忆保持外,在获取训练中,SCR和瞳孔扩张对CTX发作的反应区分了CTX+/CTX-,在回忆测试中,SEBR和瞳孔扩张区分了CTX+/CTX-,最敏感指数的效应大小为中到大(SEBR:Hedge的g=0.56和g=0.69;瞳孔扩张:Hedge的g=0.99和g=0.88)。我们的结果表明,使用配置学习范式,情境恐惧记忆保持可以在7天内得到证明,为此,我们提供了稳健且可复制的测量方法。
{"title":"Measuring human context fear conditioning and retention after consolidation.","authors":"Yanfang Xia, Jelena Wehrli, Samuel Gerster, Marijn Kroes, Maxime Houtekamer, Dominik R Bach","doi":"10.1101/lm.053781.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053781.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fear conditioning is a laboratory paradigm commonly used to investigate aversive learning and memory. In context fear conditioning, a configuration of elemental cues (conditioned stimulus [CTX]) predicts an aversive event (unconditioned stimulus [US]). To quantify context fear acquisition in humans, previous work has used startle eyeblink responses (SEBRs), skin conductance responses (SCRs), and verbal reports, but different quantification methods have rarely been compared. Moreover, preclinical intervention studies mandate recall tests several days after acquisition, and it is unclear how to induce and measure context fear memory retention over such a time interval. First, we used a semi-immersive virtual reality paradigm. In two experiments (<i>N</i> = 23 and <i>N</i> = 28), we found successful declarative learning and memory retention over 7 d but no evidence of other conditioned responses. Next, we used a configural fear conditioning paradigm with five static room images as CTXs in two experiments (<i>N</i> = 29 and <i>N</i> = 24). Besides successful declarative learning and memory retention after 7 d, SCR and pupil dilation in response to CTX onset differentiated CTX<sup>+</sup>/CTX<sup>-</sup> during acquisition training, and SEBR and pupil dilation differentiated CTX<sup>+</sup>/CTX<sup>-</sup> during the recall test, with medium to large effect sizes for the most sensitive indices (SEBR: Hedge's <i>g</i> = 0.56 and <i>g</i> = 0.69; pupil dilation: Hedge's <i>g</i> = 0.99 and <i>g</i> = 0.88). Our results demonstrate that with a configural learning paradigm, context fear memory retention can be demonstrated over 7 d, and we provide robust and replicable measurement methods to this end.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 7","pages":"139-150"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519410/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10002973","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Activation of prefrontal cortex and striatal regions in rats after shifting between rules in a T-maze. T迷宫中规则转换后大鼠前额叶皮层和纹状体区域的激活。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-07-24 Print Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053795.123
Virginie Oberto, Hongying Gao, Ana Biondi, Susan J Sara, Sidney I Wiener

Prefrontal cortical and striatal areas have been identified by inactivation or lesion studies to be required for behavioral flexibility, including selecting and processing of different types of information. In order to identify these networks activated selectively during the acquisition of new reward contingency rules, rats were trained to discriminate orientations of bars presented in pseudorandom sequence on two video monitors positioned behind the goal sites on a T-maze with return arms. A second group already trained in the visual discrimination task learned to alternate left and right goal arm visits in the same maze while ignoring the visual cues still being presented. In each experimental group, once the rats reached criterion performance, the brains were prepared after a 90-min delay for later processing for c-fos immunohistochemistry. While both groups extinguished a prior strategy and acquired a new rule, they differed by the identity of the strategies and previous learning experience. Among the 28 forebrain areas examined, there were significant increases in the relative density of c-fos immunoreactive cell bodies after learning the second rule in the prefrontal cortex cingulate, the prelimbic and infralimbic areas, the dorsomedial striatum and the core of the nucleus accumbens, the ventral subiculum, and the central nucleus of the amygdala. These largely correspond to structures previously identified in inactivation studies, and their neurons fire synchronously during learning and strategy shifts. The data suggest that this dynamic network may underlie reward-based selection for action-a type of cognitive flexibility.

额前皮质和纹状体区域已通过失活或损伤研究确定为行为灵活性所需,包括选择和处理不同类型的信息。为了识别在获取新的奖励应急规则过程中选择性激活的这些网络,训练大鼠辨别位于带返回臂的T型迷宫目标位置后面的两个视频监视器上以伪随机序列呈现的杆的方向。第二组已经接受了视觉辨别任务的训练,他们学会在同一个迷宫中交替进行左、右门臂访问,同时忽略仍在呈现的视觉线索。在每个实验组中,一旦大鼠达到标准性能,在延迟90分钟后准备大脑,以便稍后进行c-fos免疫组织化学处理。虽然两组人都消灭了先前的策略并获得了新的规则,但他们在策略的身份和先前的学习经验方面有所不同。在检查的28个前脑区域中,学习第二规则后,c-fos免疫反应细胞体的相对密度在前额叶皮层扣带、边缘前和边缘下区域、背内侧纹状体和伏隔核核心、腹侧亚托和杏仁核中央核显著增加。这些在很大程度上对应于先前在失活研究中确定的结构,并且它们的神经元在学习和策略转变过程中同步放电。数据表明,这种动态网络可能是基于奖励的行动选择的基础,这是一种认知灵活性。
{"title":"Activation of prefrontal cortex and striatal regions in rats after shifting between rules in a T-maze.","authors":"Virginie Oberto, Hongying Gao, Ana Biondi, Susan J Sara, Sidney I Wiener","doi":"10.1101/lm.053795.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053795.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prefrontal cortical and striatal areas have been identified by inactivation or lesion studies to be required for behavioral flexibility, including selecting and processing of different types of information. In order to identify these networks activated selectively during the acquisition of new reward contingency rules, rats were trained to discriminate orientations of bars presented in pseudorandom sequence on two video monitors positioned behind the goal sites on a T-maze with return arms. A second group already trained in the visual discrimination task learned to alternate left and right goal arm visits in the same maze while ignoring the visual cues still being presented. In each experimental group, once the rats reached criterion performance, the brains were prepared after a 90-min delay for later processing for c-fos immunohistochemistry. While both groups extinguished a prior strategy and acquired a new rule, they differed by the identity of the strategies and previous learning experience. Among the 28 forebrain areas examined, there were significant increases in the relative density of c-fos immunoreactive cell bodies after learning the second rule in the prefrontal cortex cingulate, the prelimbic and infralimbic areas, the dorsomedial striatum and the core of the nucleus accumbens, the ventral subiculum, and the central nucleus of the amygdala. These largely correspond to structures previously identified in inactivation studies, and their neurons fire synchronously during learning and strategy shifts. The data suggest that this dynamic network may underlie reward-based selection for action-a type of cognitive flexibility.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 7","pages":"133-138"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519402/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9940435","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Elevated corticosterone after fear learning impairs remote auditory memory retrieval and alters brain network connectivity. 恐惧学习后皮质酮升高会损害远程听觉记忆检索并改变大脑网络连接。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-07-24 Print Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053836.123
Niek Brosens, Sylvie L Lesuis, Ilse Bassie, Lara Reyes, Priya Gajadien, Paul J Lucassen, Harm J Krugers

Glucocorticoids are potent memory modulators that can modify behavior in an adaptive or maladaptive manner. Elevated glucocorticoid levels after learning promote memory consolidation at recent time points, but their effects on remote time points are not well established. Here we set out to assess whether corticosterone (CORT) given after learning modifies remote fear memory. To that end, mice were exposed to a mild auditory fear conditioning paradigm followed by a single 2 mg/kg CORT injection, and after 28 d, auditory memory was assessed. Neuronal activation was investigated using immunohistochemistry for the immediate early gene c-Fos, and coactivation of brain regions was determined using a correlation matrix analysis. CORT-treated mice displayed significantly less remote auditory memory retrieval. While the net activity of studied brain regions was similar compared with the control condition, CORT-induced remote memory impairment was associated with altered correlated activity between brain regions. Specifically, connectivity of the lateral amygdala with the basal amygdala and the dorsal dentate gyrus was significantly reduced in CORT-treated mice, suggesting disrupted network connectivity that may underlie diminished remote memory retrieval. Elucidating the pathways underlying these effects could help provide mechanistic insight into the effects of stress on memory and possibly provide therapeutic targets for psychopathology.

糖皮质激素是一种强大的记忆调节剂,可以以适应或不适应的方式改变行为。学习后糖皮质激素水平的升高促进了最近时间点的记忆巩固,但它们对远程时间点的影响尚不明确。在这里,我们开始评估学习后给予皮质酮(CORT)是否会改变远程恐惧记忆。为此,将小鼠暴露于轻度听觉恐惧条件反射模式,然后单次注射2mg/kg CORT,28天后,评估听觉记忆。使用免疫组织化学方法研究即刻早期基因c-Fos的神经元激活,并使用相关矩阵分析确定大脑区域的共激活。CORT处理的小鼠表现出明显较少的远程听觉记忆检索。虽然与对照条件相比,所研究的大脑区域的净活动相似,但CORT诱导的远程记忆损伤与大脑区域之间的相关活动改变有关。具体而言,在CORT治疗的小鼠中,外侧杏仁核与基底杏仁核和齿状回的连接显著降低,这表明网络连接中断,这可能是远程记忆检索减少的原因。阐明这些影响的潜在途径有助于从机制上深入了解压力对记忆的影响,并可能为精神病理学提供治疗靶点。
{"title":"Elevated corticosterone after fear learning impairs remote auditory memory retrieval and alters brain network connectivity.","authors":"Niek Brosens, Sylvie L Lesuis, Ilse Bassie, Lara Reyes, Priya Gajadien, Paul J Lucassen, Harm J Krugers","doi":"10.1101/lm.053836.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053836.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Glucocorticoids are potent memory modulators that can modify behavior in an adaptive or maladaptive manner. Elevated glucocorticoid levels after learning promote memory consolidation at recent time points, but their effects on remote time points are not well established. Here we set out to assess whether corticosterone (CORT) given after learning modifies remote fear memory. To that end, mice were exposed to a mild auditory fear conditioning paradigm followed by a single 2 mg/kg CORT injection, and after 28 d, auditory memory was assessed. Neuronal activation was investigated using immunohistochemistry for the immediate early gene <i>c</i>-<i>Fos</i>, and coactivation of brain regions was determined using a correlation matrix analysis. CORT-treated mice displayed significantly less remote auditory memory retrieval. While the net activity of studied brain regions was similar compared with the control condition, CORT-induced remote memory impairment was associated with altered correlated activity between brain regions. Specifically, connectivity of the lateral amygdala with the basal amygdala and the dorsal dentate gyrus was significantly reduced in CORT-treated mice, suggesting disrupted network connectivity that may underlie diminished remote memory retrieval. Elucidating the pathways underlying these effects could help provide mechanistic insight into the effects of stress on memory and possibly provide therapeutic targets for psychopathology.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":"30 7","pages":"125-132"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519398/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10244687","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1