首页 > 最新文献

Perceptual and Motor Skills最新文献

英文 中文
The Use of a Just Noticeable Difference Approach to Improve Perceptual Acuity Ability in Male Runners. 使用 "明显差异法 "提高男性跑步者的感知敏锐度。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241252852
Maressa Priscila Krause, Luke Haile, Dayanne Sampaio Antonio, Andre L Peres, Robert J Robertson

We were interested in micro-variations in an athlete's psychophysical state that separate peak exertion from physiological collapse. Thus, we measured perceptual acuity in runners using a classic psychophysical approach, the just noticeable difference (JND) on two standard stimuli runs at treadmill speed corresponding to 70%VO2max and 80%VO2max. Thirty-four male runners (M age = 35.26, SD = 7.33 years) first performed a maximal treadmill test to determine the speed of a standard exercise bout for the JND trials. The JND trials consisted of four 5-minute running bouts on a treadmill with 5-minute rests between bouts. For bouts 1 and 3, participants ran at the standard stimuli pace, but for bouts 2 and 4, they adjusted their speeds to achieve a level of exertion at a JND above/below the SS. They achieved differences in the final 30 seconds of the VO2 between each JND bout and the previous standard stimuli at just above (JND-A) and just below (JND-B) the JND perceived exertions. We used a Generalized Linear Model analysis to compare the JND-A and JND-B within and between ventilatory threshold groups (lower/higher) in absolute and relative VO2 and in terms of the total JND magnitude. The magnitude of JND-A was greater than that of JND-B at 70%VO2max and 80%VO2max in absolute units (70%VO2 Δ = 2.62; SE = 0.37; p < .001; 80%VO2 Δ = 1.67; SE = 0.44; p = .002) and in relative units (70%VO2max Δ = 4.70; SE = 0.66; p < .001; 80%VO2max Δ = 2.96; SE = 0.80; p = .002). The total magnitude was greater in the 70%VO2max trial than 80%VO2max in absolute units (70%VO2 M = 3.78, SE = 0.31 mL·kg-1·min-1; 80%VO2 M = 2.62, SE = 0.37 mL·kg-1·min-1; p = .020) and in relative units (70%VO2max M = 6.57, SE = 0.53%VO2max; 80%VO2max M = 4.71, SE = 0.64%VO2max; p = .030). The JND range narrowed when physiologic demand increased, for both physical (speed) and psychological (RPE) variables.

我们对运动员心理物理状态的微观变化很感兴趣,这种微观变化是峰值消耗和生理崩溃之间的分水岭。因此,我们使用一种经典的心理物理方法测量了跑步者的知觉敏锐度,即在跑步机上以 70%VO2max 和 80%VO2max 的速度进行两次标准刺激跑时的刚注意到的差异 (JND)。34 名男性跑步者(平均年龄 35.26 岁,平均年龄 7.33 岁)首先进行了最大跑步机测试,以确定 JND 试验的标准运动速度。JND 试验包括在跑步机上进行四次 5 分钟的跑步,每次跑步之间休息 5 分钟。在第 1 次和第 3 次试验中,参与者以标准刺激速度跑步,但在第 2 次和第 4 次试验中,他们调整了速度,以达到高于/低于 SS 的 JND 运动强度。他们在最后 30 秒的 VO2 值上实现了每场 JND 与前一场标准刺激之间的差异,即略高于(JND-A)和略低于(JND-B)JND 感知消耗量。我们使用广义线性模型分析法比较了通气阈值组(较低/较高)内和组间的 JND-A 和 JND-B 的绝对和相对 VO2 以及总 JND 幅值。在绝对单位(70%VO2 Δ = 2.62;SE = 0.37;p < .001;80%VO2 Δ = 1.67;SE = 0.44;p = .002)和相对单位(70%VO2max Δ = 4.70;SE = 0.66;p < .001;80%VO2max Δ = 2.96;SE = 0.80;p = .002)上,70%VO2max 和 80%VO2max 时的 JND-A 幅值大于 JND-B 幅值。就绝对单位(70%VO2 M = 3.78,SE = 0.31 mL-kg-1-min-1;80%VO2 M = 2.62,SE = 0.37 mL-kg-1-min-1;p = .020)和相对单位(70%VO2max M = 6.57,SE = 0.53%VO2max;80%VO2max M = 4.71,SE = 0.64%VO2max;p = .030)而言,70%VO2max 试验的总幅度大于 80%VO2max。当生理需求增加时,JND 范围缩小,生理(速度)和心理(RPE)变量均是如此。
{"title":"The Use of a <i>Just Noticeable Difference</i> Approach to Improve Perceptual Acuity Ability in Male Runners.","authors":"Maressa Priscila Krause, Luke Haile, Dayanne Sampaio Antonio, Andre L Peres, Robert J Robertson","doi":"10.1177/00315125241252852","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241252852","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We were interested in micro-variations in an athlete's psychophysical state that separate peak exertion from physiological collapse. Thus, we measured perceptual acuity in runners using a classic psychophysical approach, the <i>just noticeable difference</i> (JND) on two standard stimuli runs at treadmill speed corresponding to 70%VO<sub>2max</sub> and 80%VO<sub>2max</sub>. Thirty-four male runners (<i>M</i> age = 35.26, <i>SD</i> = 7.33 years) first performed a maximal treadmill test to determine the speed of a standard exercise bout for the JND trials. The JND trials consisted of four 5-minute running bouts on a treadmill with 5-minute rests between bouts. For bouts 1 and 3, participants ran at the standard stimuli pace, but for bouts 2 and 4, they adjusted their speeds to achieve a level of exertion at a JND above/below the SS. They achieved differences in the final 30 seconds of the VO<sub>2</sub> between each JND bout and the previous standard stimuli at just above (JND-A) and just below (JND-B) the JND perceived exertions. We used a Generalized Linear Model analysis to compare the JND-A and JND-B within and between ventilatory threshold groups (lower/higher) in absolute and relative VO<sub>2</sub> and in terms of the total JND magnitude. The magnitude of JND-A was greater than that of JND-B at 70%VO<sub>2max</sub> and 80%VO<sub>2max</sub> in absolute units (70%VO<sub>2</sub> Δ = 2.62; <i>SE</i> = 0.37; <i>p</i> < .001; 80%VO<sub>2</sub> Δ = 1.67; <i>SE</i> = 0.44; <i>p</i> = .002) and in relative units (70%VO<sub>2max</sub> Δ = 4.70; <i>SE</i> = 0.66; <i>p</i> < .001; 80%VO<sub>2max</sub> Δ = 2.96; <i>SE</i> = 0.80; <i>p</i> = .002). The total magnitude was greater in the 70%VO<sub>2max</sub> trial than 80%VO<sub>2max</sub> in absolute units (70%VO<sub>2</sub> <i>M</i> = 3.78, <i>SE</i> = 0.31 mL·kg<sup>-1</sup>·min<sup>-1</sup>; 80%VO<sub>2</sub> <i>M =</i> 2.62, <i>SE</i> = 0.37 mL·kg<sup>-1</sup>·min<sup>-1</sup>; <i>p =</i> .020) and in relative units (70%VO<sub>2max</sub> <i>M =</i> 6.57, <i>SE</i> = 0.53%VO<sub>2max</sub>; 80%VO<sub>2max</sub> <i>M =</i> 4.71, <i>SE</i> = 0.64%VO<sub>2max</sub>; <i>p =</i> .030). The JND range narrowed when physiologic demand increased, for both physical (speed) and psychological (RPE) variables.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1341-1359"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141420330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effects of Motionless Interventions Based on Visual-Auditory Instructions With Sonification on Learning a Rhythmic Motor Skill. 基于视觉-听觉指令和声音化的无动作干预对学习有节奏的运动技能的影响
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241252855
Shiva Ghambari, Saeed Arsham, Hesam Ramezanzade

Our aim in this study was to investigate the effects of motionless interventions, based on visual-auditory integration with a sonification technique, on the learning a complex rhythmic motor skill. We recruited 22 male participants with high physical fitness and provided them four acquisition sessions in which to practice hurdle running, based on a visual-auditory instructional pattern. Next, we divided participants into three groups: visual-auditory, auditory, and control. In six sessions of motionless interventions, with no physical practice, participants in the visual-auditory group received a visual-auditory pattern similar to their experience during the acquisition period. The auditory group only listened to the sound of sonified movements of an expert hurdler, and the control group received no instructional interventions. Finally, participants in all three groups underwent post-intervention and transfer tests to determine their errors in the spatial and relative timing of their leading leg's knee angular displacement. Both visual-auditory and auditory groups had significantly less spatial error than the control group. However, there were no significant group differences in relative timing in any test phase. These results indicate that the use of the sonification technique in the form of visual-auditory instruction adapted to the athletes' needs benefitted perception-sensory capacities to improve motor skill learning.

我们这项研究的目的是调查基于视觉-听觉整合和声化技术的静态干预对学习复杂节奏性运动技能的影响。我们招募了 22 名体能较好的男性参与者,并根据视觉-听觉教学模式为他们提供了四次练习跨栏跑的习得课程。接下来,我们将参与者分为三组:视觉-听觉组、听觉组和控制组。视听组的参与者在六节没有身体练习的静态干预课程中,接受与他们在习得阶段的体验类似的视听模式。听觉组只聆听跨栏高手的声波动作,而对照组则不接受任何教学干预。最后,三组学员都接受了干预后测试和转移测试,以确定他们在前腿膝关节角位移的空间和相对时间上的误差。视听组和听觉组的空间误差都明显小于对照组。然而,在任何测试阶段,相对计时方面都没有明显的组间差异。这些结果表明,根据运动员的需要,以视听教学的形式使用声化技术有利于提高感知能力,从而改善运动技能的学习。
{"title":"The Effects of Motionless Interventions Based on Visual-Auditory Instructions With Sonification on Learning a Rhythmic Motor Skill.","authors":"Shiva Ghambari, Saeed Arsham, Hesam Ramezanzade","doi":"10.1177/00315125241252855","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241252855","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our aim in this study was to investigate the effects of motionless interventions, based on visual-auditory integration with a sonification technique, on the learning a complex rhythmic motor skill. We recruited 22 male participants with high physical fitness and provided them four acquisition sessions in which to practice hurdle running, based on a visual-auditory instructional pattern. Next, we divided participants into three groups: visual-auditory, auditory, and control. In six sessions of motionless interventions, with no physical practice, participants in the visual-auditory group received a visual-auditory pattern similar to their experience during the acquisition period. The auditory group only listened to the sound of sonified movements of an expert hurdler, and the control group received no instructional interventions. Finally, participants in all three groups underwent post-intervention and transfer tests to determine their errors in the spatial and relative timing of their leading leg's knee angular displacement. Both visual-auditory and auditory groups had significantly less spatial error than the control group. However, there were no significant group differences in relative timing in any test phase. These results indicate that the use of the sonification technique in the form of visual-auditory instruction adapted to the athletes' needs benefitted perception-sensory capacities to improve motor skill learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1321-1340"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140958653","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dual-Tasks in Soccer: Effects of Players' Experience and Task Condition on Physical Performance. 足球中的双重任务:球员经验和任务条件对身体表现的影响
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241257398
Gibson Moreira Praça, Pedro Henrique de Almeida Oliveira, Vitor Hugo Santos Resende

In this study, we examined whether experience level and various dual motor and cognitive or single tasks influenced young soccer players' physical performance during small-sided games. Participants were 72 players from U-13 (n = 36) and U-17 (n = 36) groups who participated in 3-to-a-side small-sided games under four experimental conditions: control, a secondary motor task, an additional related secondary cognitive task, and an additional secondary non-specific task. We used GPS devices to measure physical performance in terms of distances covered and accelerations at different thresholds. We found no significant interaction effect between player experience and task condition (p = .540), meaning that dual tasks had comparable effects on players of different experience levels. There were significant main effects of both experience level (p < .001) and condition (p < .001) on most physically related variables. Older players outperformed younger ones, particularly in high-intensity actions. While secondary motor tasks decreased physical performance, secondary cognitive tasks, irrespective of specificity, did not impair players' performances. In conclusion, experience level did not influence the players' physical response to dual tasks, and a secondary motor task was more disruptive to physical performance than either of two types of secondary cognitive tasks. Cognitive tasks can be incorporated into soccer training without compromising physical performance.

在这项研究中,我们考察了经验水平和各种运动与认知双重任务或单一任务是否会影响青少年足球运动员在小场比赛中的体能表现。参与者是来自 U-13 组(36 人)和 U-17 组(36 人)的 72 名球员,他们在四种实验条件下参加了三对一的小场比赛:对照组、二级运动任务、额外的相关二级认知任务和额外的二级非特定任务。我们使用全球定位系统(GPS)设备测量不同阈值下的运动表现,包括运动距离和加速度。我们发现玩家经验和任务条件之间没有明显的交互效应(p = .540),这意味着双重任务对不同经验水平的玩家产生的效果相当。经验水平(p < .001)和条件(p < .001)对大多数身体相关变量都有明显的主效应。年长球员的表现优于年轻球员,尤其是在高强度动作方面。虽然次要运动任务会降低体能表现,但次要认知任务,无论其特殊性如何,都不会影响运动员的表现。总之,经验水平并不影响球员对双重任务的身体反应,而次要运动任务比两种次要认知任务中的任何一种都更影响身体表现。在足球训练中加入认知任务不会影响身体表现。
{"title":"Dual-Tasks in Soccer: Effects of Players' Experience and Task Condition on Physical Performance.","authors":"Gibson Moreira Praça, Pedro Henrique de Almeida Oliveira, Vitor Hugo Santos Resende","doi":"10.1177/00315125241257398","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241257398","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we examined whether experience level and various dual motor and cognitive or single tasks influenced young soccer players' physical performance during small-sided games. Participants were 72 players from U-13 (<i>n</i> = 36) and U-17 (<i>n</i> = 36) groups who participated in 3-to-a-side small-sided games under four experimental conditions: control, a secondary motor task, an additional related secondary cognitive task, and an additional secondary non-specific task. We used GPS devices to measure physical performance in terms of distances covered and accelerations at different thresholds. We found no significant interaction effect between player experience and task condition (<i>p</i> = .540), meaning that dual tasks had comparable effects on players of different experience levels. There were significant main effects of both experience level (<i>p</i> < .001) and condition (<i>p</i> < .001) on most physically related variables. Older players outperformed younger ones, particularly in high-intensity actions. While secondary motor tasks decreased physical performance, secondary cognitive tasks, irrespective of specificity, did not impair players' performances. In conclusion, experience level did not influence the players' physical response to dual tasks, and a secondary motor task was more disruptive to physical performance than either of two types of secondary cognitive tasks. Cognitive tasks can be incorporated into soccer training without compromising physical performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1378-1397"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141158572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impaired Perception of Body-Weight Distribution Marks Functional Mobility Problems in Patients Undergoing Total Hip Arthroplasty. 接受全髋关节置换术的患者对身体重量分布的感知障碍标志着功能性活动障碍。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241256405
Davide De Leo, Federico Temporiti, Carlotta Bleggi, Moreno La Guardia, Paola Adamo, Roberto Gatti

Hip osteoarthritis and total hip arthroplasty imply damaged articular and periarticular structures responsible for proprioception, and this damage may impair the accurate perception of body-weight distribution. In this study, we investigated proprioceptive abilities and accuracy perceiving body-weight distribution in patients undergoing total hip arthroplasty, and we assessed the associations between these abilities and body perception accuracy with functional mobility testing in 20 patients scheduled for total hip arthroplasty and 20 age-matched healthy participants. We assessed (a) absolute error in hip joint position sense (AE-JPS), (b) absolute error in body-weight distribution (AE-BWD) during standing and sit-to-stand tasks with open and closed eyes, and (c) functional mobility with the Timed Up and Go Test (TUG). We assessed patients undergoing hip arthroplasty before (T0) and five days after their surgery (T1), while control participants underwent a single evaluation. Relative to controls, participants undergoing surgery showed higher AE-JPS at 15° of hip flexion at T0 (p = .003) and at T1 (p = .007), greater AE-BWD during sit-to-stand with open eyes at T1 (p = .014) and with closed eyes at both T0 (p = .014) and at T1 (p < .001), and worse TUG at both T0 (p = .009) and T1 (p < .001). AE-BWD during sit-to-stand with closed eyes positively correlated with TUG at T0 (r = 0.55, p = .011) and at T1 (r = 0.51, p = .027). These findings suggested that impairments in body-weight distribution perception were evident both before and immediately after total hip arthroplasty, suggesting that these impairments may regularly mark these patients' functional mobility problems.

髋关节骨关节炎和全髋关节置换术意味着负责本体感觉的关节和关节周围结构受损,这种损伤可能会影响对身体重量分布的准确感知。在这项研究中,我们调查了接受全髋关节置换术的患者的本体感觉能力和感知身体重量分布的准确性,并通过对 20 名计划接受全髋关节置换术的患者和 20 名年龄匹配的健康参与者进行功能活动性测试,评估了这些能力与身体感知准确性之间的关联。我们评估了(a)髋关节位置感的绝对误差(AE-JPS),(b)睁眼和闭眼站立和坐到站任务中身体重量分布的绝对误差(AE-BWD),以及(c)定时上下楼测试(TUG)的功能移动性。我们对接受髋关节置换术的患者进行了术前(T0)和术后五天(T1)的评估,而对照组患者只进行了一次评估。与对照组相比,接受手术的患者在T0(p = .003)和T1(p = .007)时髋关节屈曲15°时的AE-JPS更高,在T1(p = .014)和T0(p = .014)和T1(p < .001)时睁眼坐立时的AE-BWD更高,在T0(p = .009)和T1(p < .001)时闭眼坐立时的AE-BWD更高,而TUG在T0(p = .009)和T1(p < .001)时更差。闭眼坐立时的AE-BWD与T0(r = 0.55,p = .011)和T1(r = 0.51,p = .027)时的TUG呈正相关。这些研究结果表明,在全髋关节置换术前和术后,患者对身体重量分布的感知都存在明显的障碍,这表明这些障碍可能是这些患者功能性活动障碍的固定标志。
{"title":"Impaired Perception of Body-Weight Distribution Marks Functional Mobility Problems in Patients Undergoing Total Hip Arthroplasty.","authors":"Davide De Leo, Federico Temporiti, Carlotta Bleggi, Moreno La Guardia, Paola Adamo, Roberto Gatti","doi":"10.1177/00315125241256405","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241256405","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Hip osteoarthritis and total hip arthroplasty imply damaged articular and periarticular structures responsible for proprioception, and this damage may impair the accurate perception of body-weight distribution. In this study, we investigated proprioceptive abilities and accuracy perceiving body-weight distribution in patients undergoing total hip arthroplasty, and we assessed the associations between these abilities and body perception accuracy with functional mobility testing in 20 patients scheduled for total hip arthroplasty and 20 age-matched healthy participants. We assessed (a) absolute error in hip joint position sense (AE-JPS), (b) absolute error in body-weight distribution (AE-BWD) during standing and sit-to-stand tasks with open and closed eyes, and (c) functional mobility with the Timed Up and Go Test (TUG). We assessed patients undergoing hip arthroplasty before (T0) and five days after their surgery (T1), while control participants underwent a single evaluation. Relative to controls, participants undergoing surgery showed higher AE-JPS at 15° of hip flexion at T0 (<i>p</i> = .003) and at T1 (<i>p</i> = .007), greater AE-BWD during sit-to-stand with open eyes at T1 (<i>p</i> = .014) and with closed eyes at both T0 (<i>p</i> = .014) and at T1 (<i>p</i> < .001), and worse TUG at both T0 (<i>p</i> = .009) and T1 (<i>p</i> < .001). AE-BWD during sit-to-stand with closed eyes positively correlated with TUG at T0 (<i>r</i> = 0.55, <i>p</i> = .011) and at T1 (<i>r</i> = 0.51, <i>p</i> = .027). These findings suggested that impairments in body-weight distribution perception were evident both before and immediately after total hip arthroplasty, suggesting that these impairments may regularly mark these patients' functional mobility problems.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1163-1182"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141162157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does Viewing Mirror-Reflected Body Image Affect Static and Dynamic Standing Balance? 观看镜子反射的身体形象会影响静态和动态站立平衡吗?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241253634
Naoki Hamada, Ryo Tsujinaka, Hitoshi Oda, Shiho Fukuda, Masakazu Matsuoka, Hiroshi Kunimura, Koichi Hiraoka

In the present study, we examined the immediate effect of allowing healthy participants to view their mirror-reflected body image on static and dynamic balance. We placed a mirror to allow participants to frontally view their own body image while maintaining a quiet stance or while engaged in a dynamic postural standing task. On measures of body sway during quiet stance, there were no effects of this visual feedback, supporting the view that human beings have no central mechanism for viewing the mirror-reflected body image to control body sway during quiet stance. However, the body deviated forward during quiet stance while viewing the mirror-reflected body image, indicating that viewing the mirror-reflected body image contributed to the anterior-posterior positioning of the body, as mediated by an ankle control strategy. For the dynamic standing task, viewing the body image induced unstable peaks of rhythmic lateral shifting of the body weight over the feet. This indicates that viewing the body image caused unstable motor commands for rhythmic lateral weight shifting. When participants made a transition from a bipedal to a unipedal stance in response to a cue, viewing the body image shortened the onset latency of the body sway. Accordingly, viewing the body image seemed to accelerate the motor execution involved in lateral weight shifting, possibly due to predictive activation of the motor system before movement onset. Considered collectively, we found static and dynamic stance balance to be influenced by viewing one's mirror-reflected body image. Viewing the mirror-reflected body image may be a means of changing static and dynamic balance in patients with impaired postural control.

在本研究中,我们考察了让健康参与者观看镜像反射的身体形象对静态和动态平衡的直接影响。我们放置了一面镜子,让参与者在保持安静姿态或进行动态姿势站立任务时正面观看自己的身体影像。在测量安静站立时的身体摇摆时,这种视觉反馈没有任何影响,这支持了这样一种观点,即人类在观看镜子反射的身体影像时没有控制安静站立时身体摇摆的中心机制。然而,在观看镜面反射的身体影像时,身体在安静站立时会向前偏离,这表明观看镜面反射的身体影像有助于身体的前后定位,这是由踝关节控制策略介导的。在动态站立任务中,观察身体影像会引起身体重心有节奏地向脚部横向移动,并达到不稳定的峰值。这表明,观看身体图像会导致身体重心有节奏地横向移动的运动指令不稳定。当参与者根据提示从双足站立过渡到单足站立时,观看身体图像会缩短身体摇摆的开始潜伏期。因此,观看身体图像似乎加快了横向重心转移的运动执行速度,这可能是由于在运动开始前运动系统的预测性激活。综合来看,我们发现静态和动态的站立平衡都会受到观看镜像反射身体图像的影响。观看镜面反射体像可能是改变姿势控制受损患者静态和动态平衡的一种方法。
{"title":"Does Viewing Mirror-Reflected Body Image Affect Static and Dynamic Standing Balance?","authors":"Naoki Hamada, Ryo Tsujinaka, Hitoshi Oda, Shiho Fukuda, Masakazu Matsuoka, Hiroshi Kunimura, Koichi Hiraoka","doi":"10.1177/00315125241253634","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241253634","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the present study, we examined the immediate effect of allowing healthy participants to view their mirror-reflected body image on static and dynamic balance. We placed a mirror to allow participants to frontally view their own body image while maintaining a quiet stance or while engaged in a dynamic postural standing task. On measures of body sway during quiet stance, there were no effects of this visual feedback, supporting the view that human beings have no central mechanism for viewing the mirror-reflected body image to control body sway during quiet stance. However, the body deviated forward during quiet stance while viewing the mirror-reflected body image, indicating that viewing the mirror-reflected body image contributed to the anterior-posterior positioning of the body, as mediated by an ankle control strategy. For the dynamic standing task, viewing the body image induced unstable peaks of rhythmic lateral shifting of the body weight over the feet. This indicates that viewing the body image caused unstable motor commands for rhythmic lateral weight shifting. When participants made a transition from a bipedal to a unipedal stance in response to a cue, viewing the body image shortened the onset latency of the body sway. Accordingly, viewing the body image seemed to accelerate the motor execution involved in lateral weight shifting, possibly due to predictive activation of the motor system before movement onset. Considered collectively, we found static and dynamic stance balance to be influenced by viewing one's mirror-reflected body image. Viewing the mirror-reflected body image may be a means of changing static and dynamic balance in patients with impaired postural control.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1025-1040"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899022","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Initial Validation of the Coach-Athlete Relationship Questionnaire in a Sample of Portuguese Athletes. 在葡萄牙运动员样本中初步验证教练与运动员关系问卷。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241254437
Ana Pinho, Diogo Monteiro, Miguel Jacinto, Rui Matos, Filipe Rodrigues, Nuno Amaro, Pedro Teques, Teresa Fonseca, Raúl Antunes

Our primary objectives in this study were to translate and provide psychometric support for the Coach Athlete Relationship Questionnaire (CART-Q) Portuguese version, assess its invariance across sex, and explore its nomological validity in relation to enjoyment. Our sample participants were 470 athletes (226 females, 244 males) aged between 16 to 39 years from various individual and team sports. We found that the translated Portuguese version of the CART-Q exhibited satisfactory test-retest reliability and can serve as a reliable tool for evaluating the core constructs of the coach-athlete relationship - closeness, commitment, and complementarity. Moreover, this instrument showed evidence of nomological validity through significant positive correlations between its underlying factors and athletes' enjoyment with their sport. The proposed model for explaining item variance was also found to be invariant between male and female respondents. We recommend further use of this instrument in research and practical applications.

本研究的主要目的是翻译葡萄牙语版的 "教练与运动员关系问卷"(CART-Q)并为其提供心理测量支持,评估其在不同性别间的不变性,并探讨其与乐趣相关的名义效度。我们的样本参与者是 470 名运动员(女性 226 人,男性 244 人),年龄在 16 岁至 39 岁之间,来自不同的个人和团体运动项目。我们发现,CART-Q 的葡萄牙语翻译版显示出令人满意的测试-再测试可靠性,可以作为评估教练-运动员关系的核心结构--亲密性、承诺性和互补性--的可靠工具。此外,该工具的基本因素与运动员对其运动的喜爱程度之间存在显著的正相关,这也证明了该工具的名义效度。我们还发现,所提出的解释项目变异的模型在男性和女性受访者之间是不变的。我们建议在研究和实际应用中进一步使用该工具。
{"title":"Initial Validation of the Coach-Athlete Relationship Questionnaire in a Sample of Portuguese Athletes.","authors":"Ana Pinho, Diogo Monteiro, Miguel Jacinto, Rui Matos, Filipe Rodrigues, Nuno Amaro, Pedro Teques, Teresa Fonseca, Raúl Antunes","doi":"10.1177/00315125241254437","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241254437","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our primary objectives in this study were to translate and provide psychometric support for the Coach Athlete Relationship Questionnaire (CART-Q) Portuguese version, assess its invariance across sex, and explore its nomological validity in relation to enjoyment. Our sample participants were 470 athletes (226 females, 244 males) aged between 16 to 39 years from various individual and team sports. We found that the translated Portuguese version of the CART-Q exhibited satisfactory test-retest reliability and can serve as a reliable tool for evaluating the core constructs of the coach-athlete relationship - closeness, commitment, and complementarity. Moreover, this instrument showed evidence of nomological validity through significant positive correlations between its underlying factors and athletes' enjoyment with their sport. The proposed model for explaining item variance was also found to be invariant between male and female respondents. We recommend further use of this instrument in research and practical applications.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1360-1377"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11295406/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140958651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Comparison of the Functioning and Disability Levels of Children With Hemiplegic and Diplegic Cerebral Palsy Based on ICF-CY Components. 基于 ICF-CY 各组成部分的偏瘫和双瘫脑瘫儿童功能和残疾程度比较。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-12 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241254130
Hasan Bingöl, Dilan Demirtaş Karaoba

We compared children with hemiplegic and diplegic cerebral palsy (CP) using the conceptual framework of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: Child and Youth version (ICF-CY). We enrolled 42 children with CP aged 5 - 13 years old (M age = 9.57, SD = 2.8 years). We assessed their trunk control and dynamic balance with the Trunk Control Measurement Scale (TCMS) and the Timed Up and Go test (TUG), and we used ABILHAND-Kids and Assessment of Life Habits (Life-H) to assess their manual ability and participation with activities of daily living. We administered the European Child Environment Questionnaire (ECEQ) to identify relevant environmental factors. We employed structural equation modeling (SEM) to identify specific factors contributing to potential differences between these CP groups. Children with hemiplegic CP demonstrated significantly better outcomes in terms of trunk control, dynamic balance, and environmental factors compared to those with diplegic CP (p < .05). In contrast, children with diplegic CP demonstrated superior outcomes regarding manual ability, compared to those with hemiplegic CP (p < .001). In our structural equation models, trunk control strongly predicted both dynamic balance (0.75) and environmental factors (0.74). Moreover, the relationships between trunk control and participation in daily and social activities were 0.54 and 0.47, respectively. Impaired trunk control and dynamic balance were significant contributors to increased activity restrictions and environmental barriers in children with diplegic CP. This suggests that improving disability and functioning in children with diplegic CP requires a focus on trunk control training and dynamic balance exercises.

我们采用国际功能、残疾和健康分类(ICF-CY)的概念框架,对偏瘫和双瘫脑瘫(CP)儿童进行了比较:儿童和青少年版》(ICF-CY)的概念框架进行了比较。我们共招募了 42 名 5-13 岁的 CP 儿童(中位年龄 = 9.57 岁,标准差 = 2.8 岁)。我们使用躯干控制测量量表(TCMS)和定时起立行走测试(TUG)评估了他们的躯干控制和动态平衡能力,并使用ABILHAND-Kids和生活习惯评估(Life-H)评估了他们的动手能力和参与日常生活的情况。我们采用欧洲儿童环境问卷(ECEQ)来确定相关的环境因素。我们采用结构方程模型(SEM)来确定导致这些 CP 组之间潜在差异的具体因素。与偏瘫型 CP 儿童相比,偏瘫型 CP 儿童在躯干控制、动态平衡和环境因素方面的表现明显更好(p < .05)。相反,与偏瘫型脊髓灰质炎患儿相比,双瘫型脊髓灰质炎患儿在徒手能力方面表现出更好的结果(p < .001)。在我们的结构方程模型中,躯干控制对动态平衡(0.75)和环境因素(0.74)都有很强的预测作用。此外,躯干控制与参与日常活动和社会活动之间的关系分别为 0.54 和 0.47。躯干控制和动态平衡受损是导致双瘫 CP 儿童活动限制和环境障碍增加的重要因素。这表明,要改善双瘫儿童的残疾和功能,就必须重视躯干控制训练和动态平衡练习。
{"title":"A Comparison of the Functioning and Disability Levels of Children With Hemiplegic and Diplegic Cerebral Palsy Based on ICF-CY Components.","authors":"Hasan Bingöl, Dilan Demirtaş Karaoba","doi":"10.1177/00315125241254130","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241254130","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We compared children with hemiplegic and diplegic cerebral palsy (CP) using the conceptual framework of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: Child and Youth version (ICF-CY). We enrolled 42 children with CP aged 5 - 13 years old (<i>M</i> age = 9.57, <i>SD</i> = 2.8 years). We assessed their trunk control and dynamic balance with the Trunk Control Measurement Scale (TCMS) and the Timed Up and Go test (TUG), and we used ABILHAND-Kids and Assessment of Life Habits (Life-H) to assess their manual ability and participation with activities of daily living. We administered the European Child Environment Questionnaire (ECEQ) to identify relevant environmental factors. We employed structural equation modeling (SEM) to identify specific factors contributing to potential differences between these CP groups. Children with hemiplegic CP demonstrated significantly better outcomes in terms of trunk control, dynamic balance, and environmental factors compared to those with diplegic CP (<i>p</i> < .05). In contrast, children with diplegic CP demonstrated superior outcomes regarding manual ability, compared to those with hemiplegic CP (<i>p</i> < .001). In our structural equation models, trunk control strongly predicted both dynamic balance (0.75) and environmental factors (0.74). Moreover, the relationships between trunk control and participation in daily and social activities were 0.54 and 0.47, respectively. Impaired trunk control and dynamic balance were significant contributors to increased activity restrictions and environmental barriers in children with diplegic CP. This suggests that improving disability and functioning in children with diplegic CP requires a focus on trunk control training and dynamic balance exercises.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1145-1162"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140910742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bibliometric Mapping of Psychological Flexibility Research: Trends and Future Directions. 心理弹性研究的文献计量图:趋势与未来方向》。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241253946
Walton Wider, Nicholas Tze Ping Pang, Jiaming Lin, Muhammad Ashraf Fauzi, Leilei Jiang, Jem Cloyd M Tanucan

In this article, we offer an exhaustive analysis of academic work on psychological flexibility using bibliometric techniques. We identify emerging trends in a dataset of 3535 scholarly articles from the Web of Science database. We highlight key publications, map out the field's intellectual framework, and anticipate future research avenues through co-citation and co-word analytics. The co-citation assessment revealed five distinct clusters, while the co-word analysis showed three. Although research regarding psychological flexibility has gained recent popularity, there remains a need for more scholarly initiatives to achieve a nuanced understanding of this subject.

在本文中,我们利用文献计量学技术对有关心理灵活性的学术著作进行了详尽的分析。我们从科学网数据库的 3535 篇学术文章数据集中发现了新趋势。我们强调了主要出版物,绘制了该领域的知识框架,并通过共引和共词分析预测了未来的研究途径。共同引用评估显示了五个不同的群组,而共同词分析则显示了三个群组。尽管有关心理灵活性的研究近来颇受欢迎,但仍需要更多的学术活动来深入了解这一主题。
{"title":"Bibliometric Mapping of Psychological Flexibility Research: Trends and Future Directions.","authors":"Walton Wider, Nicholas Tze Ping Pang, Jiaming Lin, Muhammad Ashraf Fauzi, Leilei Jiang, Jem Cloyd M Tanucan","doi":"10.1177/00315125241253946","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241253946","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this article, we offer an exhaustive analysis of academic work on psychological flexibility using bibliometric techniques. We identify emerging trends in a dataset of 3535 scholarly articles from the Web of Science database. We highlight key publications, map out the field's intellectual framework, and anticipate future research avenues through co-citation and co-word analytics. The co-citation assessment revealed five distinct clusters, while the co-word analysis showed three. Although research regarding psychological flexibility has gained recent popularity, there remains a need for more scholarly initiatives to achieve a nuanced understanding of this subject.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1207-1232"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140898977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Difference Between Expert Dancers' and Non-Dancers Tapping Timing With and Without an Auditory Stimulus at a Slow Tempo. 舞蹈专家和非舞蹈专家在慢速听觉刺激下和不听觉刺激下敲击时间的差异。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241262547
Soo Mi Nam, Ji-Won Park, Ji-Hyun Ko, Min Joo Kim

Our primary purpose in this study was to determine whether trained dancers differed from untrained non-dancers in their ability to accurately control motor timing during finger and heel tapping tasks, both with and without slow isochronous auditory stimuli. Dancers and non-dancers were instructed to synchronize their taps with isochronous auditory stimuli under three conditions: 30, 40, and 50 BPM. After the synchronization phase, participants were asked to continue tapping without the auditory sequences. On the synchronization task, the tapping onset of both groups lagged behind the stimulus onset in all tempo conditions. In all conditions, dancers showed more accurate and stable beat synchronization and continuation than non-dancers. As the tempo condition slowed down (from 50 to 30 BPM), synchronization accuracy decreased while synchronization and continuation variability increased. Unlike for novices, dancers showed no difference between the finger and heel tapping synchronization tasks. During the continuous tasks, their timing accuracy was higher for heel than for finger tapping. Collectively, these findings suggest that dance training, which involves synchronizing bodily movements based on rhythm, may lead to an accumulation of experience that enhances specific sensorimotor skills related to synchronizing movements with external stimuli or continuing rhythmic movements temporally.

本研究的主要目的是确定在有和没有慢速等时听觉刺激的情况下,受过训练的舞蹈演员与未受过训练的非舞蹈演员在手指和脚跟敲击任务中准确控制运动时间的能力是否存在差异。在三种条件下,舞蹈演员和非舞蹈演员被要求在等时听觉刺激下同步敲击:30、40 和 50 BPM。同步阶段结束后,参与者被要求在没有听觉序列的情况下继续敲击。在同步任务中,在所有节奏条件下,两组参与者的敲击开始时间都落后于刺激开始时间。在所有条件下,跳舞者都比不跳舞者表现出更准确、更稳定的节拍同步和延续性。随着节奏条件的减慢(从 50 BPM 到 30 BPM),同步的准确性降低了,而同步和延续的变异性增加了。与新手不同,舞蹈演员在手指和脚跟敲击同步任务中没有表现出差异。在连续任务中,舞者脚跟敲击的计时准确性高于手指敲击。总之,这些研究结果表明,舞蹈训练涉及根据节奏同步身体动作,可能会导致经验的积累,从而提高与外部刺激同步动作或在时间上延续节奏动作相关的特定感觉运动技能。
{"title":"The Difference Between Expert Dancers' and Non-Dancers Tapping Timing With and Without an Auditory Stimulus at a Slow Tempo.","authors":"Soo Mi Nam, Ji-Won Park, Ji-Hyun Ko, Min Joo Kim","doi":"10.1177/00315125241262547","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241262547","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our primary purpose in this study was to determine whether trained dancers differed from untrained non-dancers in their ability to accurately control motor timing during finger and heel tapping tasks, both with and without slow isochronous auditory stimuli. Dancers and non-dancers were instructed to synchronize their taps with isochronous auditory stimuli under three conditions: 30, 40, and 50 BPM. After the synchronization phase, participants were asked to continue tapping without the auditory sequences. On the synchronization task, the tapping onset of both groups lagged behind the stimulus onset in all tempo conditions. In all conditions, dancers showed more accurate and stable beat synchronization and continuation than non-dancers. As the tempo condition slowed down (from 50 to 30 BPM), synchronization accuracy decreased while synchronization and continuation variability increased. Unlike for novices, dancers showed no difference between the finger and heel tapping synchronization tasks. During the continuous tasks, their timing accuracy was higher for heel than for finger tapping. Collectively, these findings suggest that dance training, which involves synchronizing bodily movements based on rhythm, may lead to an accumulation of experience that enhances specific sensorimotor skills related to synchronizing movements with external stimuli or continuing rhythmic movements temporally.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1398-1414"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141321365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating the Stroop Test With Older Adults: Construct Validity, Short Term Test-Retest Reliability, and Sensitivity to Mental Fatigue. 评估老年人的 Stroop 测试:结构有效性、短期测试-重测可靠性以及对精神疲劳的敏感性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1177/00315125241253425
Larissa Oliveira Faria, Thais Frois, Leonardo de Sousa Fortes, Laiss Bertola, Maicon Rodrigues Albuquerque

We conducted two studies to evaluate the construct validity, short term test-retest reliability, and sensitivity to mental fatigue of the Stroop task when used with older adults. In Study 1, 40 participants visited our lab on two separate days. On the first visit, they took five screening scales, and we measured their height and body mass. On the second visit, they completed the Stroop task twice with a 30-minute interval between assessments. In Study 2, 15 different participants took a 30-minute Flanker/Reverse Flanker task during the interval between the two administrations of the Stroop tasks and they gave subjective ratings of their mental fatigue on the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) prior to taking either of the Stroop tasks. In Study 1, participants showed a ceiling effect on the Stroop accuracy measure, there was strong concurrent validity for the Stroop with significant score differences between the Stroop's congruent and incongruent conditions (p < .001), and there was excellent response time reliability (ICC = 0.926) on day two when participants took the Stroop twice within a 30-minute inter-test interval. However, there were significant test-retest performance differences with respect to cognitive inhibition (p < .001). In Study 2, mental fatigue from the Flanker/Reverse Flanker test resulted in a significantly worse second Stroop performance (p = .045). We concluded that the Stroop task demonstrated strong concurrent validity and response time reliability among older adults, but it showed sensitivity to mental fatigue, and repeated administrations within the short 30-minute test-retest interval revealed that the most important Stroop measure (cognitive inhibition) was unreliable. We discuss the implications of these findings.

我们进行了两项研究,以评估 Stroop 任务在用于老年人时的建构效度、短期测试再测可靠性以及对心理疲劳的敏感性。在研究 1 中,40 名参与者分别在两天内访问了我们的实验室。在第一次来访时,他们做了五项筛选量表,我们还测量了他们的身高和体重。第二次来访时,他们完成了两次 Stroop 任务,两次评估之间间隔 30 分钟。在研究 2 中,15 名不同的参与者在两次执行 Stroop 任务的间隔期间参加了一次 30 分钟的 Flanker/Reverse Flanker 任务,他们在参加任何一次 Stroop 任务之前都用视觉模拟量表(VAS)对自己的精神疲劳程度进行了主观评分。在研究 1 中,受试者在 Stroop 准确性测量中表现出了天花板效应;Stroop 的并发效度很高,在 Stroop 的一致和不一致条件下得分差异显著(p < .001);受试者在测试间隔为 30 分钟的情况下进行两次 Stroop 测试,第二天的反应时间可靠性极佳(ICC = 0.926)。然而,在认知抑制方面,测试后的表现存在明显差异(p < .001)。在研究 2 中,Flanker/Reverse Flanker 测试中的心理疲劳导致第二次 Stroop 表现明显较差(p = .045)。我们得出的结论是,Stroop 任务在老年人中表现出很强的并发效度和反应时间可靠性,但它对精神疲劳很敏感,而且在短短 30 分钟的测试-再测间隔内重复施测显示,最重要的 Stroop 测量(认知抑制)并不可靠。我们将讨论这些发现的意义。
{"title":"Evaluating the Stroop Test With Older Adults: Construct Validity, Short Term Test-Retest Reliability, and Sensitivity to Mental Fatigue.","authors":"Larissa Oliveira Faria, Thais Frois, Leonardo de Sousa Fortes, Laiss Bertola, Maicon Rodrigues Albuquerque","doi":"10.1177/00315125241253425","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00315125241253425","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We conducted two studies to evaluate the construct validity, short term test-retest reliability, and sensitivity to mental fatigue of the Stroop task when used with older adults. In Study 1, 40 participants visited our lab on two separate days. On the first visit, they took five screening scales, and we measured their height and body mass. On the second visit, they completed the Stroop task twice with a 30-minute interval between assessments. In Study 2, 15 different participants took a 30-minute Flanker/Reverse Flanker task during the interval between the two administrations of the Stroop tasks and they gave subjective ratings of their mental fatigue on the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) prior to taking either of the Stroop tasks. In Study 1, participants showed a ceiling effect on the Stroop accuracy measure, there was strong concurrent validity for the Stroop with significant score differences between the Stroop's congruent and incongruent conditions (<i>p</i> < .001), and there was excellent response time reliability (ICC = 0.926) on day two when participants took the Stroop twice within a 30-minute inter-test interval. However, there were significant test-retest performance differences with respect to cognitive inhibition (<i>p</i> < .001). In Study 2, mental fatigue from the Flanker/Reverse Flanker test resulted in a significantly worse second Stroop performance (<i>p</i> = .045). We concluded that the Stroop task demonstrated strong concurrent validity and response time reliability among older adults, but it showed sensitivity to mental fatigue, and repeated administrations within the short 30-minute test-retest interval revealed that the most important Stroop measure (cognitive inhibition) was unreliable. We discuss the implications of these findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":19869,"journal":{"name":"Perceptual and Motor Skills","volume":" ","pages":"1120-1144"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140910868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Perceptual and Motor Skills
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1