首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
Why Do Children Think Words Are Mutually Exclusive? 为什么孩子们认为词语是相互排斥的?
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-21 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241287732
Gabor Brody, Roman Feiman, Athulya Aravind

How do children learn what a word means when its uses are consistent with many possible meanings? One influential idea is that children rely on an inductive bias that ensures that novel words get assigned distinct meanings from known words-mutual exclusivity. Here, we explore the possibility that mutual-exclusivity phenomena do not reflect a bias but rather information encoded in the message. Learners might effectively be told when (and when not) to assume that word meanings are mutually exclusive. In three experiments (N = 106 from across the United States; ages 2 years, 0 months-2 years, 11 months), we show that 2-year-olds only assumed that novel words have distinct meanings if the words were spoken with focus, an information-structural marker of contrast. Without focus, we found no mutual exclusivity; novel words were understood to label familiar objects. These results provide a novel account of mutual exclusivity and demonstrate an early emerging understanding of focus and information structure.

当一个词的用法与许多可能的含义一致时,儿童是如何学会这个词的含义的呢?一种有影响力的观点认为,儿童依赖于一种归纳偏差--互斥性,这种偏差能确保新词被赋予与已知词不同的含义。在这里,我们探讨了一种可能性,即互斥现象并不反映偏见,而是反映了信息中的编码信息。学习者可能会被告知何时(以及何时不)应该认为词义是相互排斥的。在三项实验中(N = 106,来自美国各地;年龄为 2 岁 0 个月-2 岁 11 个月),我们发现只有在说话时突出重点(一种对比的信息结构标记)的情况下,2 岁儿童才会认为新词具有不同的含义。在没有焦点的情况下,我们发现没有互斥性;新词被理解为对熟悉物体的标记。这些结果为互斥性提供了一个新的解释,并证明了幼儿对焦点和信息结构的早期理解。
{"title":"Why Do Children Think Words Are Mutually Exclusive?","authors":"Gabor Brody, Roman Feiman, Athulya Aravind","doi":"10.1177/09567976241287732","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241287732","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do children learn what a word means when its uses are consistent with many possible meanings? One influential idea is that children rely on an inductive bias that ensures that novel words get assigned distinct meanings from known words-<i>mutual exclusivity</i>. Here, we explore the possibility that mutual-exclusivity phenomena do not reflect a bias but rather information encoded in the message. Learners might effectively be told when (and when not) to assume that word meanings are mutually exclusive. In three experiments (<i>N</i> = 106 from across the United States; ages 2 years, 0 months-2 years, 11 months), we show that 2-year-olds only assumed that novel words have distinct meanings if the words were spoken with <i>focus</i>, an information-structural marker of contrast. Without focus, we found no mutual exclusivity; novel words were understood to label familiar objects. These results provide a novel account of mutual exclusivity and demonstrate an early emerging understanding of focus and information structure.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1315-1324"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142682574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is Mansplaining Gendered? The Effects of Unsolicited, Generic, and Prescriptive Advice on U.S. Women. 男人的抱怨有性别倾向吗?未经请求的通用性和指令性建议对美国女性的影响》。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-25 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241268630
Erik Santoro, Hazel Rose Markus

In light of popular accounts in the United States of "mansplaining," we investigated the effects on women when others give them "unresponsive" advice (i.e., unsolicited, generic, and prescriptive recommendations). We show using both vignettes (Study 1) and live interactions (Study 2) that unresponsive advice (vs. responsive questions) from men negatively affected women's self-perceptions, leaving them feeling less respected, powerful, and trusting and having a smaller size of self. The advice giver's gender did not moderate these self-perception outcomes (Study 3), although women anticipated greater stereotype threat only when men, and not when women, gave them unresponsive advice. Similar effects were found using responsive advice instead of questions as the comparison condition (Study 4). Overall, these findings (N = 4,394 U.S. adult women) suggest that it is the unresponsive nature of advice-and for certain outcomes the advice giver's gender-that explain its effects on women. They point to the value of a responsive suggestion or question during conversations, particularly during cross-gender ones.

鉴于美国流行的 "男人抱怨 "的说法,我们调查了当他人给女性提供 "无回应 "建议(即未经请求的、通用的和指令性的建议)时对女性的影响。我们通过小故事(研究 1)和现场互动(研究 2)表明,来自男性的无回应建议(相对于有回应的问题)会对女性的自我认知产生负面影响,让她们感觉自己更不被尊重、更没有力量、更不被信任,自我规模也更小。虽然只有当男性而非女性向女性提供无回应的建议时,女性才会预期到更大的刻板印象威胁,但建议提供者的性别并没有调节这些自我认知结果(研究 3)。使用有回应的建议而不是问题作为对比条件,也发现了类似的效果(研究 4)。总之,这些研究结果(N = 4,394 名美国成年女性)表明,正是建议的无反应性--以及在某些结果中建议者的性别--解释了建议对女性的影响。他们指出,在对话中,尤其是在跨性别对话中,有反应的建议或问题很有价值。
{"title":"Is Mansplaining Gendered? The Effects of Unsolicited, Generic, and Prescriptive Advice on U.S. Women.","authors":"Erik Santoro, Hazel Rose Markus","doi":"10.1177/09567976241268630","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241268630","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In light of popular accounts in the United States of \"mansplaining,\" we investigated the effects on women when others give them \"unresponsive\" advice (i.e., unsolicited, generic, and prescriptive recommendations). We show using both vignettes (Study 1) and live interactions (Study 2) that unresponsive advice (vs. responsive questions) from men negatively affected women's self-perceptions, leaving them feeling less respected, powerful, and trusting and having a smaller size of self. The advice giver's gender did not moderate these self-perception outcomes (Study 3), although women anticipated greater stereotype threat only when men, and not when women, gave them unresponsive advice. Similar effects were found using responsive advice instead of questions as the comparison condition (Study 4). Overall, these findings (<i>N</i> = 4,394 U.S. adult women) suggest that it is the unresponsive nature of advice-and for certain outcomes the advice giver's gender-that explain its effects on women. They point to the value of a responsive suggestion or question during conversations, particularly during cross-gender ones.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1395-1415"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142710919","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Well-Being Costs of Informal Caregiving. 非正规护理的幸福成本。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-25 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241279203
Michael D Krämer, Wiebke Bleidorn

How does informal care affect caregivers' well-being? Theories and existing research provide conflicting answers to this question, partly because the temporal processes and conditions under which different aspects of well-being are affected are unknown. Here, we used longitudinal data from Dutch, German, and Australian representative panels (281,884 observations, 28,663 caregivers) to examine theoretically derived hypotheses about changes in caregivers' life satisfaction, affective experiences, depression/anxiety, and loneliness. Overall, results provided evidence for negative well-being effects after the transition into a caregiver role, with more pronounced and longer-lasting well-being losses in women than in men. We further found that well-being losses were larger with more time spent on caregiving, in both men and women. These results were robust across moderators of the caregiving context (care tasks, relationship with care recipient, and full-time employment). Together, the present findings support predictions of stress theory and highlight lingering questions in theoretical frameworks of care-related well-being costs.

非正式护理如何影响护理者的幸福感?理论和现有研究对这一问题给出了相互矛盾的答案,部分原因是不同方面的幸福感受到影响的时间过程和条件尚不清楚。在此,我们使用了来自荷兰、德国和澳大利亚代表性小组的纵向数据(281884 个观察值,28663 名照顾者),研究了从理论上得出的关于照顾者生活满意度、情感体验、抑郁/焦虑和孤独感变化的假设。总体而言,研究结果表明,在过渡到照顾者角色后,照顾者的幸福感会受到负面影响,女性的幸福感损失比男性更明显、更持久。我们还发现,无论男性还是女性,护理时间越长,幸福感损失越大。这些结果在照顾环境的调节因素(照顾任务、与照顾对象的关系和全职工作)中都是稳健的。总之,本研究结果支持压力理论的预测,并强调了与护理相关的幸福感成本理论框架中仍然存在的问题。
{"title":"The Well-Being Costs of Informal Caregiving.","authors":"Michael D Krämer, Wiebke Bleidorn","doi":"10.1177/09567976241279203","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241279203","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does informal care affect caregivers' well-being? Theories and existing research provide conflicting answers to this question, partly because the temporal processes and conditions under which different aspects of well-being are affected are unknown. Here, we used longitudinal data from Dutch, German, and Australian representative panels (281,884 observations, 28,663 caregivers) to examine theoretically derived hypotheses about changes in caregivers' life satisfaction, affective experiences, depression/anxiety, and loneliness. Overall, results provided evidence for negative well-being effects after the transition into a caregiver role, with more pronounced and longer-lasting well-being losses in women than in men. We further found that well-being losses were larger with more time spent on caregiving, in both men and women. These results were robust across moderators of the caregiving context (care tasks, relationship with care recipient, and full-time employment). Together, the present findings support predictions of stress theory and highlight lingering questions in theoretical frameworks of care-related well-being costs.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1382-1394"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142710921","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender Differences in Climbing up the Ladder: Why Experience Closes the Ambition Gender Gap. 攀登高峰时的性别差异:为什么经验会缩小性别之间的野心差距?
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-11 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241266778
Kristina A Wald, Mabel Abraham, Brian Pike, Adam D Galinsky

Women are unequally represented in the highest positions in society. Beyond discrimination and bias, women are missing from the top because they are less likely to pursue high-ranking opportunities. We propose that experience is a critical moderator of gender differences in pursuing leadership opportunities, with low-experience women being particularly unlikely to seek higher level positions. We used field analyses of 96 years of U.S. senator and governor elections to examine male and female politicians' propensity to run for higher political offices. As predicted, among those with little political experience, women were less likely than men to run for higher office, but experience closed this gender gap. A preregistered experiment among U.S.-based adults replicated the field findings and revealed that it was the increased self-confidence of experienced women that reduced the gender gap. The findings suggest experience, and the self-confidence that comes with it, is one lever for closing the gender gap in seeking to climb professional hierarchies.

妇女担任社会最高职位的比例不平等。除了歧视和偏见之外,女性在高层职位上的缺失还因为她们不太可能追求高级职位的机会。我们提出,经验是性别差异在寻求领导机会方面的关键调节因素,经验不足的女性尤其不可能寻求更高级别的职位。我们对 96 年的美国参议员和州长选举进行了实地分析,以研究男性和女性政治家竞选更高政治职位的倾向。正如预测的那样,在那些政治经验不足的人中,女性比男性更不可能竞选更高级别的职位,但经验弥补了这一性别差距。在美国成年人中进行的一项预先登记的实验复制了这一实地研究结果,并揭示出是经验丰富的女性自信心的增强缩小了性别差距。研究结果表明,经验以及随之而来的自信心是缩小性别差异的一个杠杆。
{"title":"Gender Differences in Climbing up the Ladder: Why Experience Closes the Ambition Gender Gap.","authors":"Kristina A Wald, Mabel Abraham, Brian Pike, Adam D Galinsky","doi":"10.1177/09567976241266778","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241266778","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Women are unequally represented in the highest positions in society. Beyond discrimination and bias, women are missing from the top because they are less likely to pursue high-ranking opportunities. We propose that experience is a critical moderator of gender differences in pursuing leadership opportunities, with low-experience women being particularly unlikely to seek higher level positions. We used field analyses of 96 years of U.S. senator and governor elections to examine male and female politicians' propensity to run for higher political offices. As predicted, among those with little political experience, women were less likely than men to run for higher office, but experience closed this gender gap. A preregistered experiment among U.S.-based adults replicated the field findings and revealed that it was the increased self-confidence of experienced women that reduced the gender gap. The findings suggest experience, and the self-confidence that comes with it, is one lever for closing the gender gap in seeking to climb professional hierarchies.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1287-1307"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401088","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Susceptibility to Attentional Capture by Target-Matching Distractors Predicts High Visual Working Memory Capacity. 易受目标匹配干扰物的注意力捕获影响可预测高视觉工作记忆能力
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241279520
Chupeng Zhong, Zhe Qu, Nan Yang, Mingze Sun, Yajie Wang, Yulong Ding

Recent event-related potential (ERP) studies showed that individuals with low visual working memory (VWM) capacity are more susceptible to salience-driven attentional capture than high-capacity individuals are, with the latter being able to proactively suppress salient but irrelevant distractors. However, it remains unclear whether and how contingent attentional capture by distractors that possess a task-relevant (target) feature is related to VWM capacity. Here, we adopted a central focused-attention task that contained peripheral target-matching distractors to investigate this issue (N = 51 adults). Surprisingly, we revealed that target-matching distractors elicited both a larger N2-posterior-contralateral (N2pc) and a larger post-N2pc distractor positivity (PD) component in high-capacity individuals than in low-capacity ones, meaning that high-capacity individuals are less able to ignore such distractors initially, though they could call on a stronger reactive suppression mechanism afterward. These findings illustrate that high-capacity individuals are more (rather than less or equally) susceptible to contingent attention capture compared with low-capacity ones.

最近的事件相关电位(ERP)研究表明,视觉工作记忆(VWM)容量低的个体比容量高的个体更容易受到突出性驱动的注意捕获的影响,后者能够主动抑制突出但不相关的分心物。然而,具有任务相关(目标)特征的分心物的或然注意捕获是否以及如何与 VWM 容量相关,目前仍不清楚。在此,我们采用了一项包含外围目标匹配分心物的中心集中注意任务来研究这个问题(N = 51 名成人)。出乎意料的是,我们发现,与低能力者相比,高能力者在目标匹配分心物引起的N2-后部-外侧(N2pc)和N2pc后分心物阳性(PD)成分都更大,这意味着高能力者最初忽略此类分心物的能力较弱,但之后他们可以调用更强的反应性抑制机制。这些发现说明,与低能力者相比,高能力者更容易(而不是较少或同样)受到或然注意捕获的影响。
{"title":"Susceptibility to Attentional Capture by Target-Matching Distractors Predicts High Visual Working Memory Capacity.","authors":"Chupeng Zhong, Zhe Qu, Nan Yang, Mingze Sun, Yajie Wang, Yulong Ding","doi":"10.1177/09567976241279520","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241279520","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent event-related potential (ERP) studies showed that individuals with low visual working memory (VWM) capacity are more susceptible to salience-driven attentional capture than high-capacity individuals are, with the latter being able to proactively suppress salient but irrelevant distractors. However, it remains unclear whether and how contingent attentional capture by distractors that possess a task-relevant (target) feature is related to VWM capacity. Here, we adopted a central focused-attention task that contained peripheral target-matching distractors to investigate this issue (<i>N</i> = 51 adults). Surprisingly, we revealed that target-matching distractors elicited both a larger N2-posterior-contralateral (N2pc) and a larger post-N2pc distractor positivity (P<sub>D</sub>) component in high-capacity individuals than in low-capacity ones, meaning that high-capacity individuals are less able to ignore such distractors initially, though they could call on a stronger reactive suppression mechanism afterward. These findings illustrate that high-capacity individuals are more (rather than less or equally) susceptible to contingent attention capture compared with low-capacity ones.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1203-1216"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142473226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrigendum to "A Practical Significance Bias in Laypeople's Evaluation of Scientific Findings". 外行评价科学发现中的实用意义偏差》的更正。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241275068
{"title":"Corrigendum to \"A Practical Significance Bias in Laypeople's Evaluation of Scientific Findings\".","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/09567976241275068","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241275068","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1308-1309"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142372696","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People Place Larger Bets When Risky Choices Provide a Postbet Option to Cash Out. 当风险选择提供投注后兑现选择时,人们会下更大的赌注。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241266516
Daniel Bennett, Lucy Albertella, Laura Forbes, Ty Hayes, Antonio Verdejo-Garcia, Lukasz Walasek, Elliot A Ludvig

After a risky choice, decision makers must frequently wait out a delay period before the outcome of their choice becomes known. In contemporary sports-betting apps, decision makers can "cash out" of their bet during this delay period by accepting a discounted immediate payout. An important open question is how availability of a postchoice cash-out option alters choice. We investigated this question using a novel gambling task that incorporated a cash-out option during the delay between bet and outcome. Across two experiments (N = 240 adults, recruited via Prolific), cash-out availability increased participants' bet amounts by up to 35%. Participants who were more likely to cash out when odds deteriorated were less likely to cash out when odds improved. Furthermore, the effect of cash-out availability on bet amounts was positively correlated with individual differences in cash-out propensity for bets with deteriorating odds only. These results suggest that cash-out availability may promote larger bets by allowing bettors to avoid losing their entire stake.

在做出有风险的选择后,决策者必须经常等待一段延迟期,然后才能知道选择的结果。在当代的体育博彩应用中,决策者可以在这一延迟期内 "兑现 "他们的赌注,即接受打折的即时赔付。一个重要的未决问题是,选择后兑现选项的可用性如何改变选择。我们使用了一种新颖的赌博任务来研究这个问题,这种任务在下注和结果之间的延迟期间包含了一个兑现选项。在两次实验中(N = 240 名成人,通过 Prolific 招募),兑现选项使参与者的投注额增加了 35%。在赔率下降时更有可能兑现的参与者,在赔率上升时兑现的可能性较低。此外,在赔率恶化的投注中,套现对投注额的影响与套现倾向的个体差异呈正相关。这些结果表明,可兑现性可能会让投注者避免损失全部赌注,从而促进更大的投注额。
{"title":"People Place Larger Bets When Risky Choices Provide a Postbet Option to Cash Out.","authors":"Daniel Bennett, Lucy Albertella, Laura Forbes, Ty Hayes, Antonio Verdejo-Garcia, Lukasz Walasek, Elliot A Ludvig","doi":"10.1177/09567976241266516","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241266516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>After a risky choice, decision makers must frequently wait out a delay period before the outcome of their choice becomes known. In contemporary sports-betting apps, decision makers can \"cash out\" of their bet during this delay period by accepting a discounted immediate payout. An important open question is how availability of a postchoice cash-out option alters choice. We investigated this question using a novel gambling task that incorporated a cash-out option during the delay between bet and outcome. Across two experiments (<i>N</i> = 240 adults, recruited via Prolific), cash-out availability increased participants' bet amounts by up to 35%. Participants who were more likely to cash out when odds deteriorated were less likely to cash out when odds improved. Furthermore, the effect of cash-out availability on bet amounts was positively correlated with individual differences in cash-out propensity for bets with deteriorating odds only. These results suggest that cash-out availability may promote larger bets by allowing bettors to avoid losing their entire stake.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1231-1245"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Unequal Burden of Obesity: Obesity's Adverse Consequences Are Contingent on Regional Obesity Prevalence. 肥胖带来的不平等负担:肥胖症的不良后果取决于地区肥胖症患病率。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-02 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241265037
Jana B Berkessel, Tobias Ebert, Jochen E Gebauer, Peter J Rentfrow

Obesity has adverse consequences for those affected. We tested whether the association between obesity and its adverse consequences is reduced in regions in which obesity is prevalent and whether lower weight bias in high-obese regions can account for this reduction. Studies 1 and 2 used data from the United States (N = 2,846,132 adults across 2,546 counties) and United Kingdom (N = 180,615 adults across 380 districts) that assessed obesity's adverse consequences in diverse domains: close relationships, economic outcomes, and health. Both studies revealed that the association between obesity and its adverse consequences is reduced (or absent) in high-obese regions. Study 3 used another large-scale data set (N = 409,837 across 2,928 U.S. counties) and revealed that lower weight bias in high-obese regions seems to account for (i.e., mediate) the reduction in obesity's adverse consequences. Overall, our findings suggest that obesity's adverse consequences are partly social and, thus, not inevitable.

肥胖会给受影响者带来不良后果。我们测试了在肥胖症流行的地区,肥胖症与其不良后果之间的联系是否会减少,以及高肥胖地区较低的体重偏差是否是造成这种减少的原因。研究 1 和研究 2 使用了来自美国(2546 个县的 2846132 名成年人)和英国(380 个区的 180615 名成年人)的数据,评估了肥胖在不同领域造成的不良后果:亲密关系、经济成果和健康。这两项研究均显示,在高肥胖率地区,肥胖与其不良后果之间的关联性降低(或消失)。研究 3 使用了另一个大规模数据集(N = 409 837,横跨美国 2 928 个县),发现高肥胖地区较低的体重偏差似乎是肥胖不良后果减少的原因(即中介作用)。总之,我们的研究结果表明,肥胖的不良后果部分是社会性的,因此并非不可避免。
{"title":"On the Unequal Burden of Obesity: Obesity's Adverse Consequences Are Contingent on Regional Obesity Prevalence.","authors":"Jana B Berkessel, Tobias Ebert, Jochen E Gebauer, Peter J Rentfrow","doi":"10.1177/09567976241265037","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241265037","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Obesity has adverse consequences for those affected. We tested whether the association between obesity and its adverse consequences is reduced in regions in which obesity is prevalent and whether lower weight bias in high-obese regions can account for this reduction. Studies 1 and 2 used data from the United States (<i>N</i> = 2,846,132 adults across 2,546 counties) and United Kingdom (<i>N</i> = 180,615 adults across 380 districts) that assessed obesity's adverse consequences in diverse domains: close relationships, economic outcomes, and health. Both studies revealed that the association between obesity and its adverse consequences is reduced (or absent) in high-obese regions. Study 3 used another large-scale data set (<i>N</i> = 409,837 across 2,928 U.S. counties) and revealed that lower weight bias in high-obese regions seems to account for (i.e., mediate) the reduction in obesity's adverse consequences. Overall, our findings suggest that obesity's adverse consequences are partly social and, thus, not inevitable.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1260-1277"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142366341","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Maps for a Non-Euclidean Environment: Path Integration and Spatial Memory on a Sphere. 非欧几里得环境下的认知地图:球面上的路径整合与空间记忆
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241279291
Misun Kim, Christian F Doeller

Humans build mental models of the world and utilize them for various cognitive tasks. The exact form of cognitive maps is not fully understood, especially for novel and complex environments beyond the flat Euclidean environment. To address this gap, we investigated path integration-a critical process underlying cognitive mapping-and spatial-memory capacity on the spherical (non-Euclidean) and planar (Euclidean) environments in young healthy adults (N = 20) using immersive virtual reality. We observed a strong Euclidean bias during the path-integration task on the spherical surface, even among participants who possessed knowledge of non-Euclidean geometry. Notably, despite this bias, participants demonstrated reasonable navigation ability on the sphere. This observation and simulation suggest that humans navigate nonflat surfaces by constructing locally confined Euclidean maps and flexibly combining them. This insight sheds light on potential neural mechanisms and behavioral strategies for solving complex cognitive tasks.

人类建立世界的心智模型,并利用这些模型完成各种认知任务。认知映射的确切形式尚不完全清楚,尤其是在平面欧几里得环境之外的新奇复杂环境中。为了填补这一空白,我们利用沉浸式虚拟现实技术研究了年轻健康成年人(20 人)在球形(非欧几里得)和平面(欧几里得)环境中的路径整合(认知映射的关键基础过程)和空间记忆能力。在球形表面的路径整合任务中,我们观察到了强烈的欧几里得偏差,即使是拥有非欧几里得几何知识的参与者也不例外。值得注意的是,尽管存在这种偏差,参与者在球面上仍表现出了合理的导航能力。这一观察和模拟结果表明,人类通过构建局部限制的欧几里得地图并灵活组合它们来导航非平面。这一洞察力揭示了解决复杂认知任务的潜在神经机制和行为策略。
{"title":"Cognitive Maps for a Non-Euclidean Environment: Path Integration and Spatial Memory on a Sphere.","authors":"Misun Kim, Christian F Doeller","doi":"10.1177/09567976241279291","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241279291","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans build mental models of the world and utilize them for various cognitive tasks. The exact form of cognitive maps is not fully understood, especially for novel and complex environments beyond the flat Euclidean environment. To address this gap, we investigated <i>path integration</i>-a critical process underlying cognitive mapping-and spatial-memory capacity on the spherical (non-Euclidean) and planar (Euclidean) environments in young healthy adults (<i>N</i> = 20) using immersive virtual reality. We observed a strong Euclidean bias during the path-integration task on the spherical surface, even among participants who possessed knowledge of non-Euclidean geometry. Notably, despite this bias, participants demonstrated reasonable navigation ability on the sphere. This observation and simulation suggest that humans navigate nonflat surfaces by constructing locally confined Euclidean maps and flexibly combining them. This insight sheds light on potential neural mechanisms and behavioral strategies for solving complex cognitive tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1217-1230"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142506681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Racial Minorities Face Discrimination From Across the Political Spectrum When Seeking to Form Ties on Social Media: Evidence From a Field Experiment. 在社交媒体上寻求建立联系时,少数种族面临来自不同政治光谱的歧视:来自现场实验的证据。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241274738
Krishnan Nair, Mohsen Mosleh, Maryam Kouchaki

We conducted a preregistered field experiment examining racial discrimination in tie formation on social media. We randomly assigned research accounts varying on race (Black, White) and politics (liberal/Democrat, conservative/Republican, neutral) to follow a politically balanced sample of Twitter (i.e., X) users (N = 5,951) who were unaware they were in a research study. We examined three predictions from the social and political psychology literatures: i) individuals favor White over Black targets, ii) this tendency is stronger for conservatives/Republicans than for liberals/Democrats, and iii) greater discrimination by conservatives/Republicans is explained by the assumption that racial minorities are liberal/Democrat. We found evidence that individuals were less likely to reciprocate social ties with Black accounts than White accounts. However, this tendency was not moderated by individuals' political orientation, shared partisanship, or partisan mismatch. In sum, this work provides field experimental evidence for racial discrimination in tie formation on social media by individuals across political backgrounds.

我们进行了一项预先登记的实地实验,考察社交媒体上纽带形成过程中的种族歧视。我们随机分配了不同种族(黑人、白人)和政治立场(自由派/民主党、保守派/共和党、中立派)的研究账户,让他们关注政治立场均衡的 Twitter(即 X)用户样本(N = 5951),这些用户并不知道他们正在进行一项研究。我们研究了社会和政治心理学文献中的三个预测:i) 个人偏爱白人而非黑人目标;ii) 保守派/共和党人的这一倾向强于自由派/民主党人;iii) 保守派/共和党人的更大歧视可以用少数种族是自由派/民主党人这一假设来解释。我们发现有证据表明,个人与黑人账户建立互惠社会关系的可能性低于白人账户。然而,这种倾向并不受个人政治倾向、共同党派或党派错配的影响。总之,这项研究为不同政治背景的个体在社交媒体上建立联系时存在种族歧视提供了实地实验证据。
{"title":"Racial Minorities Face Discrimination From Across the Political Spectrum When Seeking to Form Ties on Social Media: Evidence From a Field Experiment.","authors":"Krishnan Nair, Mohsen Mosleh, Maryam Kouchaki","doi":"10.1177/09567976241274738","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241274738","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We conducted a preregistered field experiment examining racial discrimination in tie formation on social media. We randomly assigned research accounts varying on race (Black, White) and politics (liberal/Democrat, conservative/Republican, neutral) to follow a politically balanced sample of Twitter (i.e., X) users (<i>N</i> = 5,951) who were unaware they were in a research study. We examined three predictions from the social and political psychology literatures: i) individuals favor White over Black targets, ii) this tendency is stronger for conservatives/Republicans than for liberals/Democrats, and iii) greater discrimination by conservatives/Republicans is explained by the assumption that racial minorities are liberal/Democrat. We found evidence that individuals were less likely to reciprocate social ties with Black accounts than White accounts. However, this tendency was not moderated by individuals' political orientation, shared partisanship, or partisan mismatch. In sum, this work provides field experimental evidence for racial discrimination in tie formation on social media by individuals across political backgrounds.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"1278-1286"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142392641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1