Pub Date : 2023-06-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231164553
Markus Wettstein, Hans-Werner Wahl, Johanna Drewelies, Susanne Wurm, Oliver Huxhold, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf
Little is known about historical shifts in subjective age (i.e., how old individuals feel). Moving beyond the very few time-lagged cross-sectional cohort comparisons, we examined historical shifts in within-person trajectories of subjective age from midlife to advanced old age. We used cohort-comparative longitudinal data from middle-age and older adults in the German Ageing Survey (N = 14,928; ~50% female) who lived in Germany and were between 40 and 85 years old when entering the study. They provided up to seven observations over 24 years. Results revealed that being born later in historical time is associated with feeling younger by 2% every birth-year decade and with less intraindividual change toward an older subjective age. Women reported feeling younger than men; this gender gap widened across cohorts. The association of higher education with younger subjective age became weaker across cohorts. Potential reasons for the subjective-rejuvenation effect across cohorts are discussed.
{"title":"Younger Than Ever? Subjective Age Is Becoming Younger and Remains More Stable in Middle-Age and Older Adults Today.","authors":"Markus Wettstein, Hans-Werner Wahl, Johanna Drewelies, Susanne Wurm, Oliver Huxhold, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf","doi":"10.1177/09567976231164553","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231164553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Little is known about historical shifts in subjective age (i.e., how old individuals feel). Moving beyond the very few time-lagged cross-sectional cohort comparisons, we examined historical shifts in within-person trajectories of subjective age from midlife to advanced old age. We used cohort-comparative longitudinal data from middle-age and older adults in the German Ageing Survey (<i>N</i> = 14,928; ~50% female) who lived in Germany and were between 40 and 85 years old when entering the study. They provided up to seven observations over 24 years. Results revealed that being born later in historical time is associated with feeling younger by 2% every birth-year decade and with less intraindividual change toward an older subjective age. Women reported feeling younger than men; this gender gap widened across cohorts. The association of higher education with younger subjective age became weaker across cohorts. Potential reasons for the <i>subjective-rejuvenation</i> effect across cohorts are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"647-656"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9640278","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-06-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231165267
Rebecca Zhu, Alison Gopnik
Although adults use metaphors to guide their thinking and reasoning, less is known about whether metaphors might facilitate cognition earlier in development. Previous research shows that preschoolers understand metaphors, but less is known about whether preschoolers can learn from metaphors. The current preregistered experiment investigated whether adults (n = 64) and 3- and 4-year-olds (n = 128) can use metaphors to make new inferences. In a between-subjects design, participants heard information about novel artifacts, conveyed through either only positive metaphors (e.g., "Daxes are suns") or positive and negative metaphors (e.g., "Daxes are suns. Daxes are not clouds."). In both conditions, participants of all ages successfully formed metaphor-consistent inferences about abstract, functional features of the artifacts (e.g., that daxes light up rather than let out water). Moreover, participants frequently provided explanations appealing to the metaphors when justifying their responses. Consequently, metaphors may be a powerful learning mechanism from early childhood onward.
{"title":"Preschoolers and Adults Learn From Novel Metaphors.","authors":"Rebecca Zhu, Alison Gopnik","doi":"10.1177/09567976231165267","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231165267","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although adults use metaphors to guide their thinking and reasoning, less is known about whether metaphors might facilitate cognition earlier in development. Previous research shows that preschoolers understand metaphors, but less is known about whether preschoolers can learn from metaphors. The current preregistered experiment investigated whether adults (<i>n</i> = 64) and 3- and 4-year-olds (<i>n</i> = 128) can use metaphors to make new inferences. In a between-subjects design, participants heard information about novel artifacts, conveyed through either only positive metaphors (e.g., \"Daxes are suns\") or positive and negative metaphors (e.g., \"Daxes are suns. Daxes are not clouds.\"). In both conditions, participants of all ages successfully formed metaphor-consistent inferences about abstract, functional features of the artifacts (e.g., that daxes light up rather than let out water). Moreover, participants frequently provided explanations appealing to the metaphors when justifying their responses. Consequently, metaphors may be a powerful learning mechanism from early childhood onward.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"696-704"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9646177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-06-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231163877
Louis A Schmidt, Christina A Brook, Raha Hassan, Taigan MacGowan, Kristie L Poole, Michelle K Jetha
Generation Z (1997-2012) has been characterized in the popular media as more socially inhibited, cautious, and risk averse than prior generations, but are these differences found between generations on an empirical level? And, if so, are these differences observable within generations in response to acute events such as the COVID-19 pandemic? Using a simplified time-lagged design to control for age effects, we examined between-group differences in self-reported shyness in young adult participants (N = 806, age: 17-25 years) at the same developmental age and university from the millennial generation (tested: 1999-2001; n = 266, Mage = 19.67 years, 72.9% female) and Generation Z (tested: 2018-2020), the latter generation stratified into prepandemic (n = 263, M = 18.86 years, 82.4% female) and midpandemic (n = 277, Mage = 18.67 years, 79.6% female) groups. After first establishing measurement invariance to ensure trustworthy group comparisons, we found significantly higher mean levels of shyness across each successive cohort, starting with millennials, through Generation Z before the pandemic, to Generation Z during the pandemic.
{"title":"iGen or <i>shy</i>Gen? Generational Differences in Shyness.","authors":"Louis A Schmidt, Christina A Brook, Raha Hassan, Taigan MacGowan, Kristie L Poole, Michelle K Jetha","doi":"10.1177/09567976231163877","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231163877","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Generation Z (1997-2012) has been characterized in the popular media as more socially inhibited, cautious, and risk averse than prior generations, but are these differences found between generations on an empirical level? And, if so, are these differences observable within generations in response to acute events such as the COVID-19 pandemic? Using a simplified time-lagged design to control for age effects, we examined between-group differences in self-reported shyness in young adult participants (<i>N</i> = 806, age: 17-25 years) at the same developmental age and university from the millennial generation (tested: 1999-2001; <i>n</i> = 266, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 19.67 years, 72.9% female) and Generation Z (tested: 2018-2020), the latter generation stratified into <i>prepandemic</i> (<i>n</i> = 263, <i>M</i> = 18.86 years, 82.4% female) and <i>midpandemic</i> (<i>n</i> = 277, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 18.67 years, 79.6% female) groups. After first establishing measurement invariance to ensure trustworthy group comparisons, we found significantly higher mean levels of shyness across each successive cohort, starting with millennials, through Generation Z before the pandemic, to Generation Z during the pandemic.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"705-713"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9696040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-06-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231160773
This article has been revised and republished due to substantial changes to the text of the original article, as published Online First on April 28, 2022. The article was revised after authors, Michael Rosenblum, Drew Jacoby-Senghor, and Derek Brown, contacted the journal on October 28, 2022 to express concerns about data inaccuracies in Experiments 1a, 1b, 2, and 3 referenced in the original Online First article. The authors first found that an incorrect variable was used in the analyses for Table 1 and then subsequently conducted a full audit of all studies in the paper. All errors discovered through this process were under the sole purview of Rosenblum’s contributions to the paper. After reviewing the results of the audited studies, authors Rosenblum, Jacoby-Senghor, and Brown, submitted revisions, as well as all data, to Psychologi cal Science’s Editor-in-Chief. The paper was then reviewed by them as well as a Senior and an Associate editor. The revisions in the republished article are intended to clearly update the results of the studies. Tables 1, 3 and 4, and Figures 3, 4, 5, and 6 have been updated with the results of the audited studies, as have the relevant references to these analyses in the text. Figure 2 has also been removed as it was found to be extraneous as it depicts non-significant results. The number of stimuli used in Experiment 1b has been corrected. Additionally, a new paragraph in the general discussion has been added to discuss newly apparent limitations of the paper. Due to the substantial number of edits necessary to address the above revisions, the journal determined republication of the article would allow readers to follow the article more effectively than a separate notice of the changes. Appended to the end of this republication notice is a watermarked version of the Online First article as published on April 28, 2022, so that interested readers may reference the original version of the article and note changes. 1160773 PSSXXX10.1177/09567976231160773 correction2023
{"title":"Corrigendum to \"Detecting Prejudice From Egalitarianism: Why Black Americans Don't Trust White Egalitarians' Claims\".","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/09567976231160773","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231160773","url":null,"abstract":"This article has been revised and republished due to substantial changes to the text of the original article, as published Online First on April 28, 2022. The article was revised after authors, Michael Rosenblum, Drew Jacoby-Senghor, and Derek Brown, contacted the journal on October 28, 2022 to express concerns about data inaccuracies in Experiments 1a, 1b, 2, and 3 referenced in the original Online First article. The authors first found that an incorrect variable was used in the analyses for Table 1 and then subsequently conducted a full audit of all studies in the paper. All errors discovered through this process were under the sole purview of Rosenblum’s contributions to the paper. After reviewing the results of the audited studies, authors Rosenblum, Jacoby-Senghor, and Brown, submitted revisions, as well as all data, to Psychologi cal Science’s Editor-in-Chief. The paper was then reviewed by them as well as a Senior and an Associate editor. The revisions in the republished article are intended to clearly update the results of the studies. Tables 1, 3 and 4, and Figures 3, 4, 5, and 6 have been updated with the results of the audited studies, as have the relevant references to these analyses in the text. Figure 2 has also been removed as it was found to be extraneous as it depicts non-significant results. The number of stimuli used in Experiment 1b has been corrected. Additionally, a new paragraph in the general discussion has been added to discuss newly apparent limitations of the paper. Due to the substantial number of edits necessary to address the above revisions, the journal determined republication of the article would allow readers to follow the article more effectively than a separate notice of the changes. Appended to the end of this republication notice is a watermarked version of the Online First article as published on April 28, 2022, so that interested readers may reference the original version of the article and note changes. 1160773 PSSXXX10.1177/09567976231160773 correction2023","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"736"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9615525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-05-01Epub Date: 2023-03-28DOI: 10.1177/09567976231156413
Ana Stijovic, Paul A G Forbes, Livia Tomova, Nadine Skoluda, Anja C Feneberg, Giulio Piperno, Ekaterina Pronizius, Urs M Nater, Claus Lamm, Giorgia Silani
Recent evidence suggests that social contact is a basic need governed by a social homeostatic system. Little is known, however, about how conditions of altered social homeostasis affect human psychology and physiology. Here, we investigated the effects of 8 hr of social isolation on psychological and physiological variables and compared this with 8 hr of food deprivation in a lab experiment (N = 30 adult women). Social isolation led to lowered self-reported energetic arousal and heightened fatigue, comparable with food deprivation. To test whether these findings would extend to a real-life setting, we conducted a preregistered field study during a COVID-19 lockdown (N = 87 adults; 47 women). The drop in energetic arousal after social isolation observed in the lab replicated in the field study for participants who lived alone or reported high sociability, suggesting that lowered energy could be part of a homeostatic response to the lack of social contact.
{"title":"Homeostatic Regulation of Energetic Arousal During Acute Social Isolation: Evidence From the Lab and the Field.","authors":"Ana Stijovic, Paul A G Forbes, Livia Tomova, Nadine Skoluda, Anja C Feneberg, Giulio Piperno, Ekaterina Pronizius, Urs M Nater, Claus Lamm, Giorgia Silani","doi":"10.1177/09567976231156413","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231156413","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent evidence suggests that social contact is a basic need governed by a social homeostatic system. Little is known, however, about how conditions of altered social homeostasis affect human psychology and physiology. Here, we investigated the effects of 8 hr of social isolation on psychological and physiological variables and compared this with 8 hr of food deprivation in a lab experiment (<i>N</i> = 30 adult women). Social isolation led to lowered self-reported energetic arousal and heightened fatigue, comparable with food deprivation. To test whether these findings would extend to a real-life setting, we conducted a preregistered field study during a COVID-19 lockdown (<i>N</i> = 87 adults; 47 women). The drop in energetic arousal after social isolation observed in the lab replicated in the field study for participants who lived alone or reported high sociability, suggesting that lowered energy could be part of a homeostatic response to the lack of social contact.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"537-551"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9642536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-05-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231156793
Giovanni Sala, Yukiko Nishita, Chikako Tange, Makiko Tomida, Yasuyuki Gondo, Hiroshi Shimokata, Rei Otsuka
Education has been claimed to reduce aging-associated declines in cognitive function. Given its societal relevance, considerable resources have been devoted to this research. However, because of the difficulty of detecting modest rates of change, findings have been mixed. These discrepancies may stem from methodological shortcomings such as short time spans, few waves, and small samples. The present study overcame these limitations (N = 1,892, nine waves over a period of 20 years). We tested the effect of education level on baseline performance (intercept) and the rate of change (slope) in crystallized and fluid cognitive abilities (gc and gf, respectively) in a sample of Japanese adults. Albeit positively related to both intercepts, education had no impact on either the gc or the gf slope. Furthermore, neither intercept exhibited any appreciable correlation with either slope. These results thus suggest that education has no substantial role (direct or mediated) in aging-related changes in cognition.
{"title":"No Appreciable Effect of Education on Aging-Associated Declines in Cognition: A 20-Year Follow-Up Study.","authors":"Giovanni Sala, Yukiko Nishita, Chikako Tange, Makiko Tomida, Yasuyuki Gondo, Hiroshi Shimokata, Rei Otsuka","doi":"10.1177/09567976231156793","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231156793","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Education has been claimed to reduce aging-associated declines in cognitive function. Given its societal relevance, considerable resources have been devoted to this research. However, because of the difficulty of detecting modest rates of change, findings have been mixed. These discrepancies may stem from methodological shortcomings such as short time spans, few waves, and small samples. The present study overcame these limitations (<i>N</i> = 1,892, nine waves over a period of 20 years). We tested the effect of education level on baseline performance (intercept) and the rate of change (slope) in crystallized and fluid cognitive abilities (<i>gc</i> and <i>gf</i>, respectively) in a sample of Japanese adults. Albeit positively related to both intercepts, education had no impact on either the <i>gc</i> or the <i>gf</i> slope. Furthermore, neither intercept exhibited any appreciable correlation with either slope. These results thus suggest that education has no substantial role (direct or mediated) in aging-related changes in cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"527-536"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9592630","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-05-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231163872
Ido Davidesco, Emma Laurent, Henry Valk, Tessa West, Catherine Milne, David Poeppel, Suzanne Dikker
Much of human learning happens through interaction with other people, but little is known about how this process is reflected in the brains of students and teachers. Here, we concurrently recorded electroencephalography (EEG) data from nine groups, each of which contained four students and a teacher. All participants were young adults from the northeast United States. Alpha-band (8-12 Hz) brain-to-brain synchrony between students predicted both immediate and delayed posttest performance. Further, brain-to-brain synchrony was higher in specific lecture segments associated with questions that students answered correctly. Brain-to-brain synchrony between students and teachers predicted learning outcomes at an approximately 300-ms lag in the students' brain activity relative to the teacher's brain activity, which is consistent with the time course of spoken-language comprehension. These findings provide key new evidence for the importance of collecting brain data simultaneously from groups of learners in ecologically valid settings.
{"title":"The Temporal Dynamics of Brain-to-Brain Synchrony Between Students and Teachers Predict Learning Outcomes.","authors":"Ido Davidesco, Emma Laurent, Henry Valk, Tessa West, Catherine Milne, David Poeppel, Suzanne Dikker","doi":"10.1177/09567976231163872","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231163872","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Much of human learning happens through interaction with other people, but little is known about how this process is reflected in the brains of students and teachers. Here, we concurrently recorded electroencephalography (EEG) data from nine groups, each of which contained four students and a teacher. All participants were young adults from the northeast United States. Alpha-band (8-12 Hz) brain-to-brain synchrony between students predicted both immediate and delayed posttest performance. Further, brain-to-brain synchrony was higher in specific lecture segments associated with questions that students answered correctly. Brain-to-brain synchrony between students and teachers predicted learning outcomes at an approximately 300-ms lag in the students' brain activity relative to the teacher's brain activity, which is consistent with the time course of spoken-language comprehension. These findings provide key new evidence for the importance of collecting brain data simultaneously from groups of learners in ecologically valid settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"633-643"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9589306","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-05-01DOI: 10.1177/09567976231155870
Qiliang He, Elizabeth H Beveridge, Vanesa Vargas, Ashley Salen, Thackery I Brown
The current study investigated how stress affects value-based decision-making during spatial navigation and different types of learning underlying decisions. Eighty-two adult participants (42 females) first learned to find object locations in a virtual environment from a fixed starting location (rigid learning) and then to find the same objects from unpredictable starting locations (flexible learning). Participants then decided whether to reach goal objects from the fixed or unpredictable starting location. We found that stress impairs rigid learning in females, and it does not impair, and even improves, flexible learning when performance with rigid learning is controlled for. Critically, examining how earlier learning influences subsequent decision-making using computational models, we found that stress reduces memory integration, making participants more likely to focus on recent memory and less likely to integrate information from other sources. Collectively, our results show how stress impacts different memory systems and the communication between memory and decision-making.
{"title":"Effects of Acute Stress on Rigid Learning, Flexible Learning, and Value-Based Decision-Making in Spatial Navigation.","authors":"Qiliang He, Elizabeth H Beveridge, Vanesa Vargas, Ashley Salen, Thackery I Brown","doi":"10.1177/09567976231155870","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231155870","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The current study investigated how stress affects value-based decision-making during spatial navigation and different types of learning underlying decisions. Eighty-two adult participants (42 females) first learned to find object locations in a virtual environment from a fixed starting location (rigid learning) and then to find the same objects from unpredictable starting locations (flexible learning). Participants then decided whether to reach goal objects from the fixed or unpredictable starting location. We found that stress impairs rigid learning in females, and it does not impair, and even improves, flexible learning when performance with rigid learning is controlled for. Critically, examining how earlier learning influences subsequent decision-making using computational models, we found that stress reduces memory integration, making participants more likely to focus on recent memory and less likely to integrate information from other sources. Collectively, our results show how stress impacts different memory systems and the communication between memory and decision-making.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"552-567"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9592624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We investigated a novel cognitive-ecological account for misbeliefs about the relationship between food healthiness and tastiness. We propose that different frequencies of healthy and tasty foods in contrasting contexts can trigger perceptions that health and taste are related in ways that diverge from the actual health-taste correlation in the presented food. To investigate this proposal, we conducted three studies (total N = 369), including a taste test, with adult Prolific academic participants from the United Kingdom and undergraduate psychology students from Austria. Our results showed that different frequencies of healthy and tasty food across contrasting contexts can trigger misbeliefs about the relationship between health and taste. These findings demonstrate that properties of the food ecology combined with basic cognitive processes can help explain the formation of beliefs about food such as that unhealthy food tastes better than healthy food. Our study extends the existing explanations for food beliefs and provides a perspective on how they can be changed.
{"title":"Food Is All Around: How Contexts Create Misbeliefs About the Health-Taste Relationship.","authors":"Sonja Kunz, Simona Haasova, Niklas Pivecka, Justus Schmidt, Arnd Florack","doi":"10.1177/09567976231158288","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231158288","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated a novel cognitive-ecological account for misbeliefs about the relationship between food healthiness and tastiness. We propose that different frequencies of healthy and tasty foods in contrasting contexts can trigger perceptions that health and taste are related in ways that diverge from the actual health-taste correlation in the presented food. To investigate this proposal, we conducted three studies (total <i>N</i> = 369), including a taste test, with adult Prolific academic participants from the United Kingdom and undergraduate psychology students from Austria. Our results showed that different frequencies of healthy and tasty food across contrasting contexts can trigger misbeliefs about the relationship between health and taste. These findings demonstrate that properties of the food ecology combined with basic cognitive processes can help explain the formation of beliefs about food such as that unhealthy food tastes better than healthy food. Our study extends the existing explanations for food beliefs and provides a perspective on how they can be changed.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"568-580"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9960926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-05-01Epub Date: 2023-04-07DOI: 10.1177/09567976231158292
Camille Gasser, Lila Davachi
Throughout our lives, the actions we produce are often highly familiar and repetitive (e.g., commuting to work). However, layered upon these routine actions are novel, episodic experiences. Substantial research has shown that prior knowledge can facilitate learning of conceptually related new information. But despite the central role our behavior plays in real-world experience, it remains unclear how engagement in a familiar sequence of actions influences memory for unrelated, nonmotor information coincident with those actions. To investigate this, we had healthy young adults encode novel items while simultaneously following a sequence of actions (key presses) that was either predictable and well-learned or random. Across three experiments (N = 80 each), we found that temporal order memory, but not item memory, was significantly enhanced for novel items encoded while participants executed predictable compared with random action sequences. These results suggest that engaging in familiar behaviors during novel learning scaffolds within-event temporal memory, an essential feature of episodic experiences.
{"title":"Cross-Modal Facilitation of Episodic Memory by Sequential Action Execution.","authors":"Camille Gasser, Lila Davachi","doi":"10.1177/09567976231158292","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231158292","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Throughout our lives, the actions we produce are often highly familiar and repetitive (e.g., commuting to work). However, layered upon these routine actions are novel, episodic experiences. Substantial research has shown that prior knowledge can facilitate learning of conceptually related new information. But despite the central role our behavior plays in real-world experience, it remains unclear how engagement in a familiar sequence of actions influences memory for unrelated, nonmotor information coincident with those actions. To investigate this, we had healthy young adults encode novel items while simultaneously following a sequence of actions (key presses) that was either predictable and well-learned or random. Across three experiments (<i>N</i> = 80 each), we found that temporal order memory, but not item memory, was significantly enhanced for novel items encoded while participants executed predictable compared with random action sequences. These results suggest that engaging in familiar behaviors during novel learning scaffolds within-event temporal memory, an essential feature of episodic experiences.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 5","pages":"581-602"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10331092/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9759844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}