首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
Divergent and Convergent Creativity Are Different Kinds of Foraging. 发散型创造力和聚合型创造力是不同类型的觅食。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241245695
Soran Malaie, Michael J Spivey, Tyler Marghetis

According to accounts of neural reuse and embodied cognition, higher-level cognitive abilities recycle evolutionarily ancient mechanisms for perception and action. Here, building on these accounts, we investigate whether creativity builds on our capacity to forage in space ("creativity as strategic foraging"). We report systematic connections between specific forms of creative thinking-divergent and convergent-and corresponding strategies for searching in space. U.S. American adults completed two tasks designed to measure creativity. Before each creativity trial, participants completed an unrelated search of a city map. Between subjects, we manipulated the search pattern, with some participants seeking multiple, dispersed spatial locations and others repeatedly converging on the same location. Participants who searched divergently in space were better at divergent thinking but worse at convergent thinking; this pattern reversed for participants who had converged repeatedly on a single location. These results demonstrate a targeted link between foraging and creativity, thus advancing our understanding of the origins and mechanisms of high-level cognition.

根据神经再利用和具身认知的说法,高层次的认知能力是对古老的感知和行动机制的进化再利用。在此,我们以这些说法为基础,研究创造力是否建立在我们的空间觅食能力之上("创造力即战略觅食")。我们报告了创造性思维的特定形式--发散和聚合--与相应的空间搜索策略之间的系统联系。美国成年人完成了两项旨在测量创造力的任务。在每次创造力测试之前,受试者都要完成一次与城市地图无关的搜索。在不同的受试者之间,我们对搜索模式进行了调整,让一些受试者寻找多个分散的空间位置,而另一些受试者则反复聚集在同一个位置上。在空间上分散搜索的参与者发散思维能力更强,而聚合思维能力更差;而反复聚合于单一地点的参与者则相反。这些结果表明,觅食与创造力之间存在有针对性的联系,从而推进了我们对高级认知的起源和机制的理解。
{"title":"Divergent and Convergent Creativity Are Different Kinds of Foraging.","authors":"Soran Malaie, Michael J Spivey, Tyler Marghetis","doi":"10.1177/09567976241245695","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241245695","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to accounts of neural reuse and embodied cognition, higher-level cognitive abilities recycle evolutionarily ancient mechanisms for perception and action. Here, building on these accounts, we investigate whether creativity builds on our capacity to forage in space (\"creativity as strategic foraging\"). We report systematic connections between specific forms of creative thinking-divergent and convergent-and corresponding strategies for searching in space. U.S. American adults completed two tasks designed to measure creativity. Before each creativity trial, participants completed an unrelated search of a city map. Between subjects, we manipulated the search pattern, with some participants seeking multiple, dispersed spatial locations and others repeatedly converging on the same location. Participants who searched divergently in space were better at divergent thinking but worse at convergent thinking; this pattern reversed for participants who had converged repeatedly on a single location. These results demonstrate a targeted link between foraging and creativity, thus advancing our understanding of the origins and mechanisms of high-level cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"749-759"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140850345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preregistered Replication and Extension of "Moral Hypocrisy: Social Groups and the Flexibility of Virtue". 道德虚伪:社会群体与美德的灵活性"。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241246552
Claire E Robertson, Madison Akles, Jay J Van Bavel

The tendency for people to consider themselves morally good while behaving selfishly is known as moral hypocrisy. Influential work by Valdesolo and DeSteno (2007) found evidence for intergroup moral hypocrisy such that people were more forgiving of transgressions when they were committed by an in-group member than an out-group member. We conducted two experiments to examine moral hypocrisy and group membership in an online paradigm with Prolific workers from the United States: a direct replication of the original work with minimal groups (N = 610; nationally representative) and a conceptual replication with political groups (N = 606; 50% Democrats and 50% Republicans). Although the results did not replicate the original findings, we observed evidence of in-group favoritism in minimal groups and out-group derogation in political groups. The current research finds mixed evidence of intergroup moral hypocrisy and has implications for understanding the contextual dependencies of intergroup bias and partisanship.

人们在自私自利的同时却认为自己道德高尚,这种倾向被称为道德虚伪。Valdesolo 和 DeSteno(2007 年)的研究发现了群体间道德虚伪的证据,即当内群体成员比外群体成员犯下过错时,人们会更加宽容。我们进行了两项实验,以美国 Prolific 工作者为研究对象,在在线范例中研究道德虚伪性和群体成员身份:一项是直接复制原研究成果,研究对象为最小群体(N = 610;具有全国代表性);另一项是概念复制,研究对象为政治群体(N = 606;50% 为民主党人,50% 为共和党人)。尽管研究结果没有重复最初的发现,但我们观察到在最小群体中存在群体内偏袒的证据,而在政治群体中则存在群体外贬损的证据。目前的研究发现了群体间道德虚伪的混合证据,并对理解群体间偏见和党派倾向的背景依赖性产生了影响。
{"title":"Preregistered Replication and Extension of \"Moral Hypocrisy: Social Groups and the Flexibility of Virtue\".","authors":"Claire E Robertson, Madison Akles, Jay J Van Bavel","doi":"10.1177/09567976241246552","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241246552","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The tendency for people to consider themselves morally good while behaving selfishly is known as moral hypocrisy. Influential work by Valdesolo and DeSteno (2007) found evidence for intergroup moral hypocrisy such that people were more forgiving of transgressions when they were committed by an in-group member than an out-group member. We conducted two experiments to examine moral hypocrisy and group membership in an online paradigm with Prolific workers from the United States: a direct replication of the original work with minimal groups (<i>N</i> = 610; nationally representative) and a conceptual replication with political groups (<i>N</i> = 606; 50% Democrats and 50% Republicans). Although the results did not replicate the original findings, we observed evidence of in-group favoritism in minimal groups and out-group derogation in political groups. The current research finds mixed evidence of intergroup moral hypocrisy and has implications for understanding the contextual dependencies of intergroup bias and partisanship.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"798-813"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140922953","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural Signatures of Evidence Accumulation Encode Subjective Perceptual Confidence Independent of Performance. 证据积累的神经特征编码与表现无关的主观感知信心。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241246561
Wei Dou, Lleymi J Martinez Arango, Olenka Graham Castaneda, Leopoldo Arellano, Emily Mcintyre, Claire Yballa, Jason Samaha

Confidence is an adaptive computation when environmental feedback is absent, yet there is little consensus regarding how perceptual confidence is computed in the brain. Difficulty arises because confidence correlates with other factors, such as accuracy, response time (RT), or evidence quality. We investigated whether neural signatures of evidence accumulation during a perceptual choice predict subjective confidence independently of these factors. Using motion stimuli, a central-parietal positive-going electroencephalogram component (CPP) behaves as an accumulating decision variable that predicts evidence quality, RT, accuracy, and confidence (Experiment 1, N = 25 adults). When we psychophysically varied confidence while holding accuracy constant (Experiment 2, N = 25 adults), the CPP still predicted confidence. Statistically controlling for RT, accuracy, and evidence quality (Experiment 3, N = 24 adults), the CPP still explained unique variance in confidence. The results indicate that a predecision neural signature of evidence accumulation, the CPP, encodes subjective perceptual confidence in decision-making independent of task performance.

当环境反馈缺失时,置信度是一种适应性计算,然而对于大脑是如何计算知觉置信度的却鲜有共识。由于信心与其他因素(如准确性、反应时间(RT)或证据质量)相关,因此出现了困难。我们研究了知觉选择过程中证据积累的神经特征是否能预测主观信心,而不受这些因素的影响。使用运动刺激时,中央顶叶正向脑电图成分(CPP)表现为一个累积决策变量,可预测证据质量、反应时间、准确性和信心(实验 1,N = 25 名成人)。当我们用心理物理学方法改变信心,同时保持准确性不变时(实验 2,25 名成人),CPP 仍能预测信心。在对反应时间、准确性和证据质量进行统计控制后(实验 3,N = 24 名成人),CPP 仍能解释信心的独特差异。结果表明,证据积累的决策前神经特征(CPP)编码决策中的主观知觉信心,而与任务表现无关。
{"title":"Neural Signatures of Evidence Accumulation Encode Subjective Perceptual Confidence Independent of Performance.","authors":"Wei Dou, Lleymi J Martinez Arango, Olenka Graham Castaneda, Leopoldo Arellano, Emily Mcintyre, Claire Yballa, Jason Samaha","doi":"10.1177/09567976241246561","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241246561","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Confidence is an adaptive computation when environmental feedback is absent, yet there is little consensus regarding how perceptual confidence is computed in the brain. Difficulty arises because confidence correlates with other factors, such as accuracy, response time (RT), or evidence quality. We investigated whether neural signatures of evidence accumulation during a perceptual choice predict subjective confidence independently of these factors. Using motion stimuli, a central-parietal positive-going electroencephalogram component (CPP) behaves as an accumulating decision variable that predicts evidence quality, RT, accuracy, and confidence (Experiment 1, <i>N</i> = 25 adults). When we psychophysically varied confidence while holding accuracy constant (Experiment 2, <i>N</i> = 25 adults), the CPP still predicted confidence. Statistically controlling for RT, accuracy, and evidence quality (Experiment 3, <i>N</i> = 24 adults), the CPP still explained unique variance in confidence. The results indicate that a predecision neural signature of evidence accumulation, the CPP, encodes subjective perceptual confidence in decision-making independent of task performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"760-779"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adolescents' Perceptions of Household Chaos Predict Their Adult Mental Health: A Twin-Difference Longitudinal Cohort Study. 青少年对家庭混乱的看法可预测其成年后的心理健康:双胞胎差分纵向队列研究》。
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241242105
Sophie von Stumm

This study tested whether adolescents who perceived less household chaos in their family's home than their same-aged, same-sex sibling achieved more favorable developmental outcomes in young adulthood, independent of parent-reported household chaos and family-level confounding. Data came from 4,732 families from the Twins Early Development Study, a longitudinal, U.K.-population representative cohort study of families with twins born in 1994 through 1996 in England and Wales. Adolescents who reported experiencing greater household chaos than their sibling at the age of 16 years suffered significantly poorer mental-health outcomes at the age of 23 years, independent of family-level confounding. Mental-health predictions from perceived household chaos at earlier ages were not significant, and neither were predictions for other developmental outcomes in young adulthood, including socioeconomic status indicators, sexual risk taking, cannabis use, and conflict with the law. The findings suggest that altering children's subjective perceptions of their rearing environments may help improve their adult mental health.

本研究测试了与同龄同性兄弟姐妹相比,那些认为自己家中的家庭混乱程度较低的青少年是否能在成年后获得更有利的发展结果,而这与父母报告的家庭混乱程度和家庭层面的混杂因素无关。这项研究是英国一项具有人口代表性的纵向队列研究,研究对象是英格兰和威尔士1994年至1996年出生的双胞胎家庭。与家庭层面的混杂因素无关,16 岁时家庭混乱程度高于其兄弟姐妹的青少年在 23 岁时的心理健康状况明显较差。早年感知到的家庭混乱对心理健康的预测并不显著,对青年期其他发展结果的预测也不显著,包括社会经济地位指标、性冒险、大麻使用和与法律的冲突。研究结果表明,改变儿童对其抚养环境的主观认知可能有助于改善他们成年后的心理健康。
{"title":"Adolescents' Perceptions of Household Chaos Predict Their Adult Mental Health: A Twin-Difference Longitudinal Cohort Study.","authors":"Sophie von Stumm","doi":"10.1177/09567976241242105","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241242105","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study tested whether adolescents who perceived less household chaos in their family's home than their same-aged, same-sex sibling achieved more favorable developmental outcomes in young adulthood, independent of parent-reported household chaos and family-level confounding. Data came from 4,732 families from the Twins Early Development Study, a longitudinal, U.K.-population representative cohort study of families with twins born in 1994 through 1996 in England and Wales. Adolescents who reported experiencing greater household chaos than their sibling at the age of 16 years suffered significantly poorer mental-health outcomes at the age of 23 years, independent of family-level confounding. Mental-health predictions from perceived household chaos at earlier ages were not significant, and neither were predictions for other developmental outcomes in young adulthood, including socioeconomic status indicators, sexual risk taking, cannabis use, and conflict with the law. The findings suggest that altering children's subjective perceptions of their rearing environments may help improve their adult mental health.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"736-748"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140877186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Next Chapter for Psychological Science. 心理科学的下一章
IF 4.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-27 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231221558
Simine Vazire
{"title":"The Next Chapter for <i>Psychological Science</i>.","authors":"Simine Vazire","doi":"10.1177/09567976231221558","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231221558","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"703-707"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139049190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Drawing Generalizable Conclusions From Multilevel Models: Commentary on 从多层次模型中得出可推广的结论:评论
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241245411
Joshua L. Fiechter
In a recently published article, Van de Calseyde and Efendić (2022) argue that inner-crowd wisdom (i.e., the reduction in error afforded by aggregating two estimates from a given person relative to a single initial estimate from that person) is enhanced when people are instructed to adopt the perspective of someone with whom they disagree prior to making a second estimate. Here, I present a reanalysis of Van de Calseyde and Efendić’s data and argue that evidence supporting their primary claim spuriously arises from anticonservative multilevel models. Specifically, Van de Calseyde and Efendić assess their data via random-intercept models and fail to account for item-level effects of experimental condition. Such an approach generally allows analysts to reap the enhanced statistical power of multilevel models without implementing appropriate checks on that power; in this case, underestimation of item-level variance appears to have driven an illusory benefit of perspective taking.
在最近发表的一篇文章中,Van de Calseyde 和 Efendić(2022 年)认为,当人们在进行第二次估计之前,被指示采用与他们意见相左的人的观点时,内部人群智慧(即相对于一个人的单一初始估计,通过汇总一个人的两次估计而减少的误差)会得到增强。在此,我将对范-德-卡尔塞德和埃芬迪奇的数据进行重新分析,并指出支持他们主要观点的证据来自反保守的多层次模型。具体来说,Van de Calseyde 和 Efendić 通过随机截距模型评估了他们的数据,但没有考虑到实验条件在项目层面的影响。这种方法通常允许分析人员获得多层次模型所增强的统计能力,而不对这种能力进行适当的检验;在这种情况下,低估项目水平的方差似乎导致了透视法的虚幻好处。
{"title":"Drawing Generalizable Conclusions From Multilevel Models: Commentary on","authors":"Joshua L. Fiechter","doi":"10.1177/09567976241245411","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976241245411","url":null,"abstract":"In a recently published article, Van de Calseyde and Efendić (2022) argue that inner-crowd wisdom (i.e., the reduction in error afforded by aggregating two estimates from a given person relative to a single initial estimate from that person) is enhanced when people are instructed to adopt the perspective of someone with whom they disagree prior to making a second estimate. Here, I present a reanalysis of Van de Calseyde and Efendić’s data and argue that evidence supporting their primary claim spuriously arises from anticonservative multilevel models. Specifically, Van de Calseyde and Efendić assess their data via random-intercept models and fail to account for item-level effects of experimental condition. Such an approach generally allows analysts to reap the enhanced statistical power of multilevel models without implementing appropriate checks on that power; in this case, underestimation of item-level variance appears to have driven an illusory benefit of perspective taking.","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"53 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Misplaced Divides? Discussing Political Disagreement With Strangers Can Be Unexpectedly Positive. 分歧错位?与陌生人讨论政治分歧会产生意想不到的积极效果。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241230005
Kristina A Wald, Michael Kardas, Nicholas Epley

Differences of opinion between people are common in everyday life, but discussing those differences openly in conversation may be unnecessarily rare. We report three experiments (N = 1,264 U.S.-based adults) demonstrating that people's interest in discussing important but potentially divisive topics is guided by their expectations about how positively the conversation will unfold, leaving them more interested in having a conversation with someone who agrees versus disagrees with them. People's expectations about their conversations, however, were systematically miscalibrated such that people underestimated how positive these conversations would be-especially in cases of disagreement. Miscalibrated expectations stemmed from underestimating the degree of common ground that would emerge in conversation and from failing to appreciate the power of social forces in conversation that create social connection. Misunderstanding the outcomes of conversation could lead people to avoid discussing disagreements more often, creating a misplaced barrier to learning, social connection, free inquiry, and free expression.

人与人之间的意见分歧在日常生活中司空见惯,但在谈话中公开讨论这些分歧却可能少之又少。我们报告了三项实验(N = 1,264 位美国成年人),结果表明,人们在讨论重要但可能引起分歧的话题时,其兴趣会受到他们对谈话将如何积极展开的预期的引导,从而使他们更有兴趣与同意或不同意他们观点的人进行交谈。然而,人们对谈话的预期被系统性地误判了,以至于人们低估了谈话的积极程度--尤其是在意见不一致的情况下。预期失准的原因是低估了谈话中出现的共同点的程度,以及没有认识到谈话中产生社会联系的社会力量的力量。对谈话结果的误解可能会导致人们更频繁地避免讨论分歧,从而对学习、社会联系、自由探究和自由表达造成错误的障碍。
{"title":"Misplaced Divides? Discussing Political Disagreement With Strangers Can Be Unexpectedly Positive.","authors":"Kristina A Wald, Michael Kardas, Nicholas Epley","doi":"10.1177/09567976241230005","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241230005","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Differences of opinion between people are common in everyday life, but discussing those differences openly in conversation may be unnecessarily rare. We report three experiments (<i>N =</i> 1,264 U.S.-based adults) demonstrating that people's interest in discussing important but potentially divisive topics is guided by their expectations about how positively the conversation will unfold, leaving them more interested in having a conversation with someone who agrees versus disagrees with them. People's expectations about their conversations, however, were systematically miscalibrated such that people underestimated how positive these conversations would be-especially in cases of disagreement. Miscalibrated expectations stemmed from underestimating the degree of common ground that would emerge in conversation and from failing to appreciate the power of social forces in conversation that create social connection. Misunderstanding the outcomes of conversation could lead people to avoid discussing disagreements more often, creating a misplaced barrier to learning, social connection, free inquiry, and free expression.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"471-488"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140319073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct Constellations of Common Risk Factors Differentially Relate to Executive-Function Ability in Children. 常见风险因素的不同组合与儿童执行功能能力的关系各不相同。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241235931
Eliya Ben-Asher, Blaire M Porter, Jessica A Church

Executive functioning (EF) has been shown to relate to academic achievement and well-being. Independent bodies of work have aimed to understand what environmental or personal attributes influence EF ability. However, most research has not considered how constellations of risk factors create distinct patterns of influence on EF ability. The current study tested a sample of children aged 9 to 10 years from the United States (N = 10,323, 48.06% female, Mage = 9.9 years, age range = 8.9-11.08 years) using a latent profile analysis (LPA) to detect subgroups that varied in their combinations of various risk factors. Six distinct groups of risk factors for children emerged, which in turn related to different average EF abilities. We found that family socioeconomic measures related to a subgroup having above- or below-average EF ability, but we also found an effect on EF across different risk factors. These results inform our understanding of individual variations in EF ability and highlight the idea that EF interventions should consider risk holistically.

执行功能(EF)已被证明与学业成绩和幸福感有关。独立的研究机构旨在了解环境或个人属性对 EF 能力的影响。然而,大多数研究并未考虑风险因素的组合如何对执行功能能力产生独特的影响模式。本研究以美国 9 至 10 岁儿童为样本(样本数=10,323,女性占 48.06%,年龄=9.9 岁,年龄范围=8.9-11.08 岁),采用潜在特征分析(LPA)来检测各种风险因素组合中存在差异的亚组。结果发现,有六组不同的儿童风险因素,而这些风险因素又与不同的平均EF能力有关。我们发现,家庭社会经济因素与EF能力高于或低于平均水平的亚组有关,但我们也发现了不同风险因素对EF的影响。这些结果有助于我们了解幼儿远足能力的个体差异,并强调了幼儿远足干预应全面考虑风险的观点。
{"title":"Distinct Constellations of Common Risk Factors Differentially Relate to Executive-Function Ability in Children.","authors":"Eliya Ben-Asher, Blaire M Porter, Jessica A Church","doi":"10.1177/09567976241235931","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976241235931","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Executive functioning (EF) has been shown to relate to academic achievement and well-being. Independent bodies of work have aimed to understand what environmental or personal attributes influence EF ability. However, most research has not considered how constellations of risk factors create distinct patterns of influence on EF ability. The current study tested a sample of children aged 9 to 10 years from the United States (<i>N</i> = 10,323, 48.06% female, <i>M<sub>ag</sub></i><sub>e</sub> = 9.9 years, age range = 8.9-11.08 years) using a latent profile analysis (LPA) to detect subgroups that varied in their combinations of various risk factors. Six distinct groups of risk factors for children emerged, which in turn related to different average EF abilities. We found that family socioeconomic measures related to a subgroup having above- or below-average EF ability, but we also found an effect on EF across different risk factors. These results inform our understanding of individual variations in EF ability and highlight the idea that EF interventions should consider risk holistically.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":"489-503"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140185365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do We Become More Lonely With Age? A Coordinated Data Analysis of Nine Longitudinal Studies 随着年龄增长,我们会变得更加孤独吗?九项纵向研究的协调数据分析
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-30 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241242037
Eileen K. Graham, Emorie D. Beck, Kathryn Jackson, Tomiko Yoneda, Chloe McGhee, Lily Pieramici, Olivia E. Atherton, Jing Luo, Emily C. Willroth, Andrew Steptoe, Daniel K. Mroczek, Anthony D. Ong
Loneliness is a pervasive experience with adverse impacts on health and well-being. Despite its significance, notable gaps impede a full understanding of how loneliness changes across the adult life span and what factors influence these changes. To address this, we conducted a coordinated data analysis of nine longitudinal studies encompassing 128,118 participants ages 13 to 103 from over 20 countries. Using harmonized variables and models, we examined loneliness trajectories and predictors. Analyses revealed that loneliness follows a U-shaped curve, decreasing from young adulthood to midlife and increasing in older adulthood. These patterns were consistent across studies. Several baseline factors (i.e., sex, marital status, physical function, education) were linked to loneliness levels, but few moderated the loneliness trajectories. These findings highlight the dynamic nature of loneliness and underscore the need for targeted interventions to reduce social disparities throughout adulthood.
孤独是一种普遍存在的体验,会对健康和幸福产生不利影响。尽管孤独感很重要,但明显的差距阻碍了我们全面了解孤独感在成年人一生中的变化以及影响这些变化的因素。为了解决这个问题,我们对九项纵向研究进行了协调数据分析,这些研究涵盖了来自 20 多个国家的 128,118 名 13 至 103 岁的参与者。利用统一的变量和模型,我们研究了孤独感的轨迹和预测因素。分析表明,孤独感呈 U 型曲线,从青年期到中年期递减,到老年期递增。这些模式在不同的研究中是一致的。一些基线因素(如性别、婚姻状况、身体机能、教育程度)与孤独感水平有关,但很少有因素能调节孤独感的轨迹。这些发现凸显了孤独感的动态性质,并强调有必要采取有针对性的干预措施,以减少整个成年期的社会差异。
{"title":"Do We Become More Lonely With Age? A Coordinated Data Analysis of Nine Longitudinal Studies","authors":"Eileen K. Graham, Emorie D. Beck, Kathryn Jackson, Tomiko Yoneda, Chloe McGhee, Lily Pieramici, Olivia E. Atherton, Jing Luo, Emily C. Willroth, Andrew Steptoe, Daniel K. Mroczek, Anthony D. Ong","doi":"10.1177/09567976241242037","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976241242037","url":null,"abstract":"Loneliness is a pervasive experience with adverse impacts on health and well-being. Despite its significance, notable gaps impede a full understanding of how loneliness changes across the adult life span and what factors influence these changes. To address this, we conducted a coordinated data analysis of nine longitudinal studies encompassing 128,118 participants ages 13 to 103 from over 20 countries. Using harmonized variables and models, we examined loneliness trajectories and predictors. Analyses revealed that loneliness follows a U-shaped curve, decreasing from young adulthood to midlife and increasing in older adulthood. These patterns were consistent across studies. Several baseline factors (i.e., sex, marital status, physical function, education) were linked to loneliness levels, but few moderated the loneliness trajectories. These findings highlight the dynamic nature of loneliness and underscore the need for targeted interventions to reduce social disparities throughout adulthood.","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"72 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140827020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual Preference for Socially Relevant Spatial Relations in Humans and Monkeys 人类和猴子对社会相关空间关系的视觉偏好
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1177/09567976241242995
Nicolas Goupil, Holly Rayson, Émilie Serraille, Alice Massera, Pier Francesco Ferrari, Jean-Rémy Hochmann, Liuba Papeo
As a powerful social signal, a body, face, or gaze facing toward oneself holds an individual’s attention. We asked whether, going beyond an egocentric stance, facingness between others has a similar effect and why. In a preferential-looking time paradigm, human adults showed spontaneous preference to look at two bodies facing toward (vs. away from) each other (Experiment 1a, N = 24). Moreover, facing dyads were rated higher on social semantic dimensions, showing that facingness adds social value to stimuli (Experiment 1b, N = 138). The same visual preference was found in juvenile macaque monkeys (Experiment 2, N = 21). Finally, on the human development timescale, this preference emerged by 5 years, although young infants by 7 months of age already discriminate visual scenes on the basis of body positioning (Experiment 3, N = 120). We discuss how the preference for facing dyads—shared by human adults, young children, and macaques—can signal a new milestone in social cognition development, supporting processing and learning from third-party social interactions.
作为一种强有力的社交信号,身体、脸部或目光朝向自己会吸引一个人的注意力。我们想知道,除了以自我为中心的立场之外,面对他人是否也有类似的效果,以及为什么。在一个优先注视时间范式中,人类成年人表现出自发地优先注视两个面对面(相对于背对面)的身体(实验 1a,N = 24)。此外,面对面的两人在社会语义维度上的评分更高,这表明面对面会增加刺激物的社会价值(实验 1b,N = 138)。在幼年猕猴身上也发现了同样的视觉偏好(实验 2,N = 21)。最后,从人类发展的时间尺度来看,尽管7个月大的婴儿已经能根据身体定位来分辨视觉场景,但这种偏好在5岁前就已经出现了(实验3,N = 120)。我们讨论了人类成年人、幼儿和猕猴对面对面二人组的偏好如何标志着社会认知发展的一个新里程碑,支持对第三方社会互动的处理和学习。
{"title":"Visual Preference for Socially Relevant Spatial Relations in Humans and Monkeys","authors":"Nicolas Goupil, Holly Rayson, Émilie Serraille, Alice Massera, Pier Francesco Ferrari, Jean-Rémy Hochmann, Liuba Papeo","doi":"10.1177/09567976241242995","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976241242995","url":null,"abstract":"As a powerful social signal, a body, face, or gaze facing toward oneself holds an individual’s attention. We asked whether, going beyond an egocentric stance, facingness between others has a similar effect and why. In a preferential-looking time paradigm, human adults showed spontaneous preference to look at two bodies facing toward (vs. away from) each other (Experiment 1a, N = 24). Moreover, facing dyads were rated higher on social semantic dimensions, showing that facingness adds social value to stimuli (Experiment 1b, N = 138). The same visual preference was found in juvenile macaque monkeys (Experiment 2, N = 21). Finally, on the human development timescale, this preference emerged by 5 years, although young infants by 7 months of age already discriminate visual scenes on the basis of body positioning (Experiment 3, N = 120). We discuss how the preference for facing dyads—shared by human adults, young children, and macaques—can signal a new milestone in social cognition development, supporting processing and learning from third-party social interactions.","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140827017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1