首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
No Fixed Limit for Storing Simple Visual Features: Realistic Objects Provide an Efficient Scaffold for Holding Features in Mind. 存储简单的视觉特征没有固定的限制:现实的对象为记住特征提供了一个有效的支架。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231171339
Yong Hoon Chung, Timothy F Brady, Viola S Störmer

Prominent theories of visual working memory postulate that the capacity to maintain a particular visual feature is fixed. In contrast to these theories, recent studies have demonstrated that meaningful objects are better remembered than simple, nonmeaningful stimuli. Here, we tested whether this is solely because meaningful stimuli can recruit additional features-and thus more storage capacity-or whether simple visual features that are not themselves meaningful can also benefit from being part of a meaningful object. Across five experiments (30 young adults each), we demonstrated that visual working memory capacity for color is greater when colors are part of recognizable real-world objects compared with unrecognizable objects. Our results indicate that meaningful stimuli provide a potent scaffold to help maintain simple visual feature information, possibly because they effectively increase the objects' distinctiveness from each other and reduce interference.

著名的视觉工作记忆理论假设,维持特定视觉特征的能力是固定的。与这些理论相反,最近的研究表明,有意义的物体比简单、无意义的刺激更容易被记住。在这里,我们测试了这是否仅仅是因为有意义的刺激可以吸收额外的特征——从而增加存储容量——或者是否简单的视觉特征本身没有意义,也可以从有意义的物体的一部分中受益。通过五个实验(每个实验30个年轻人),我们证明了当颜色是可识别的现实世界物体的一部分时,与不可识别的物体相比,颜色的视觉工作记忆容量更大。我们的研究结果表明,有意义的刺激为维持简单的视觉特征信息提供了强有力的支撑,可能是因为它们有效地增加了物体之间的独特性,减少了干扰。
{"title":"No Fixed Limit for Storing Simple Visual Features: Realistic Objects Provide an Efficient Scaffold for Holding Features in Mind.","authors":"Yong Hoon Chung,&nbsp;Timothy F Brady,&nbsp;Viola S Störmer","doi":"10.1177/09567976231171339","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231171339","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prominent theories of visual working memory postulate that the capacity to maintain a particular visual feature is fixed. In contrast to these theories, recent studies have demonstrated that meaningful objects are better remembered than simple, nonmeaningful stimuli. Here, we tested whether this is solely because meaningful stimuli can recruit additional features-and thus more storage capacity-or whether simple visual features that are not themselves meaningful can also benefit from being part of a meaningful object. Across five experiments (30 young adults each), we demonstrated that visual working memory capacity for color is greater when colors are part of recognizable real-world objects compared with unrecognizable objects. Our results indicate that meaningful stimuli provide a potent scaffold to help maintain simple visual feature information, possibly because they effectively increase the objects' distinctiveness from each other and reduce interference.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 7","pages":"784-793"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9793808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Harm Hypervigilance in Public Reactions to Scientific Evidence. 危害公众对科学证据反应的过度警惕。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231168777
Cory J Clark, Maja Graso, Ilana Redstone, Philip E Tetlock

Two preregistered studies from two different platforms with representative U.S. adult samples (N = 1,865) tested the harm-hypervigilance hypothesis in risk assessments of controversial behavioral science. As expected, across six sets of scientific findings, people consistently overestimated others' harmful reactions (medium to large average effect sizes) and underestimated helpful ones, even when incentivized for accuracy. Additional analyses found that (a) harm overestimations were associated with support for censoring science, (b) people who were more offended by scientific findings reported greater difficulty understanding them, and (c) evidence was moderately consistent for an association between more conservative ideology and harm overestimations. These findings are particularly relevant because journals have begun evaluating potential downstream harms of scientific findings. We discuss implications of our work and invite scholars to develop rigorous tests of (a) the social pressures that lead science astray and (b) the actual costs and benefits of publishing or not publishing potentially controversial conclusions.

两项预先注册的研究来自两个不同的平台,有代表性的美国成人样本(N = 1865),在有争议的行为科学的风险评估中检验了伤害-过度警惕假说。正如预期的那样,在六组科学发现中,人们总是高估他人的有害反应(中等到较大的平均效应大小),低估有益的反应,即使在鼓励准确的情况下也是如此。进一步的分析发现(a)危害高估与支持审查科学有关,(b)被科学发现冒犯的人报告更难以理解它们,(c)更保守的意识形态与危害高估之间的关联的证据是适度一致的。这些发现尤其重要,因为期刊已经开始评估科学发现的潜在下游危害。我们讨论了我们工作的影响,并邀请学者对(a)导致科学误入歧途的社会压力和(b)发表或不发表可能有争议的结论的实际成本和收益进行严格的测试。
{"title":"Harm Hypervigilance in Public Reactions to Scientific Evidence.","authors":"Cory J Clark,&nbsp;Maja Graso,&nbsp;Ilana Redstone,&nbsp;Philip E Tetlock","doi":"10.1177/09567976231168777","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231168777","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two preregistered studies from two different platforms with representative U.S. adult samples (<i>N</i> = 1,865) tested the harm-hypervigilance hypothesis in risk assessments of controversial behavioral science. As expected, across six sets of scientific findings, people consistently overestimated others' harmful reactions (medium to large average effect sizes) and underestimated helpful ones, even when incentivized for accuracy. Additional analyses found that (a) harm overestimations were associated with support for censoring science, (b) people who were more offended by scientific findings reported greater difficulty understanding them, and (c) evidence was moderately consistent for an association between more conservative ideology and harm overestimations. These findings are particularly relevant because journals have begun evaluating potential downstream harms of scientific findings. We discuss implications of our work and invite scholars to develop rigorous tests of (a) the social pressures that lead science astray and (b) the actual costs and benefits of publishing or not publishing potentially controversial conclusions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 7","pages":"834-848"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9851731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Time Course of Person Perception From Voices: A Behavioral Study. 从声音中感知人的时间过程:一项行为研究。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231161565
Nadine Lavan

Listeners spontaneously form impressions of a person from their voice: Is someone old or young? Trustworthy or untrustworthy? Some studies suggest that these impressions emerge rapidly (e.g., < 400 ms for traits), but it is unclear just how rapidly different impressions can emerge and whether the time courses differ across characteristics. I presented 618 adult listeners with voice recordings ranging from 25 ms to 800 ms in duration and asked them to rate physical (age, sex, health), trait (trustworthiness, dominance, attractiveness), and social (educatedness, poshness, professionalism) characteristics. I then used interrater agreement as an index for impression formation. Impressions of physical characteristics and dominance emerged fastest, showing high agreement after only 25 ms of exposure. In contrast, agreement for trait and social characteristics was initially low to moderate and gradually increased. Such a staggered time course suggests that there could be a temporo-perceptual hierarchy for person perception in which faster impressions could influence later ones.

听众会自发地从他们的声音中形成对一个人的印象:这个人是老还是年轻?值得信任还是不值得信任?一些研究表明,这些印象的形成速度很快(例如,特征的形成时间小于400毫秒),但目前尚不清楚不同印象的形成速度有多快,以及不同特征之间的时间过程是否不同。我向618名成年听众展示了时长从25毫秒到800毫秒不等的录音,并要求他们对身体特征(年龄、性别、健康状况)、性格特征(可信度、支配力、吸引力)和社会特征(受教育程度、优雅程度、专业程度)进行评分。然后,我使用译者之间的一致性作为印象形成的指标。身体特征和支配地位的印象出现得最快,仅在暴露25毫秒后就显示出高度一致。相比之下,特质和社会特征的一致性从低到中等,逐渐增加。这样一个交错的时间过程表明,人的知觉可能存在一个时间-知觉层次,在这个层次中,更快的印象可能会影响后来的印象。
{"title":"The Time Course of Person Perception From Voices: A Behavioral Study.","authors":"Nadine Lavan","doi":"10.1177/09567976231161565","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231161565","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Listeners spontaneously form impressions of a person from their voice: Is someone old or young? Trustworthy or untrustworthy? Some studies suggest that these impressions emerge rapidly (e.g., < 400 ms for traits), but it is unclear just how rapidly different impressions can emerge and whether the time courses differ across characteristics. I presented 618 adult listeners with voice recordings ranging from 25 ms to 800 ms in duration and asked them to rate physical (age, sex, health), trait (trustworthiness, dominance, attractiveness), and social (educatedness, poshness, professionalism) characteristics. I then used interrater agreement as an index for impression formation. Impressions of physical characteristics and dominance emerged fastest, showing high agreement after only 25 ms of exposure. In contrast, agreement for trait and social characteristics was initially low to moderate and gradually increased. Such a staggered time course suggests that there could be a temporo-perceptual hierarchy for person perception in which faster impressions could influence later ones.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 7","pages":"771-783"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9793805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
We Are Still Here: Omission and Perceived Discrimination Galvanized Civic Engagement Among Native Americans. 我们还在这里:忽视和察觉到的歧视激发了美国原住民的公民参与。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231165271
J Doris Dai, Jamie L Yellowtail, Ariana Munoz-Salgado, Julisa J Lopez, Emma Ward-Griffin, Crystal Echo Hawk, Judith LeBlanc, Nikki Santos, Adam Farero, Arianne E Eason, Stephanie A Fryberg

Leading up to the 2020 U.S. presidential election, Native American organizations and tribes launched get-out-the-vote campaigns that motivated Native peoples to vote in record numbers and helped flip battleground states. We conducted four studies (total N = 11,661 Native American adults) to examine the social and cultural factors explaining this historic Native civic engagement (e.g., campaigning). Results revealed that the more participants identified as being Native, the more they reported (a) engaging in civic activities, including get-out-the-vote behaviors during the 2020 election (Study 1); (b) civic engagement more broadly across a 5-year period (pilot study, Study 2); and (c) intentions to engage in civic activities in the future (Study 3). Moreover, participants who more strongly identified as Native were more likely to recognize the omission of their group from society and perceive greater group discrimination, which both independently and serially predicted greater civic engagement. These results suggest that leveraging the link between Native identification and group injustices can motivate action.

在2020年美国总统大选之前,美国原住民组织和部落发起了一场动员投票的运动,激励原住民以创纪录的数量投票,并帮助扭转了战场州的局面。我们进行了四项研究(总共N = 11,661名美洲原住民成年人),以检查解释这种历史性的土著公民参与(例如,竞选活动)的社会和文化因素。结果显示,被认为是本地人的参与者越多,他们报告的(a)参与公民活动的次数就越多,包括在2020年大选期间的投票行为(研究1);(b)在五年期间更广泛的公民参与(试点研究,研究2);(c)未来参与公民活动的意愿(研究3)。此外,更强烈地认同原住民身份的参与者更有可能认识到他们的群体被社会忽视,并感受到更大的群体歧视,这既独立又连续地预示着更大的公民参与。这些结果表明,利用土著认同和群体不公正之间的联系可以激励行动。
{"title":"We Are Still Here: Omission and Perceived Discrimination Galvanized Civic Engagement Among Native Americans.","authors":"J Doris Dai,&nbsp;Jamie L Yellowtail,&nbsp;Ariana Munoz-Salgado,&nbsp;Julisa J Lopez,&nbsp;Emma Ward-Griffin,&nbsp;Crystal Echo Hawk,&nbsp;Judith LeBlanc,&nbsp;Nikki Santos,&nbsp;Adam Farero,&nbsp;Arianne E Eason,&nbsp;Stephanie A Fryberg","doi":"10.1177/09567976231165271","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231165271","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Leading up to the 2020 U.S. presidential election, Native American organizations and tribes launched get-out-the-vote campaigns that motivated Native peoples to vote in record numbers and helped flip battleground states. We conducted four studies (total <i>N</i> = 11,661 Native American adults) to examine the social and cultural factors explaining this historic Native civic engagement (e.g., campaigning). Results revealed that the more participants identified as being Native, the more they reported (a) engaging in civic activities, including get-out-the-vote behaviors during the 2020 election (Study 1); (b) civic engagement more broadly across a 5-year period (pilot study, Study 2); and (c) intentions to engage in civic activities in the future (Study 3). Moreover, participants who more strongly identified as Native were more likely to recognize the omission of their group from society and perceive greater group discrimination, which both independently and serially predicted greater civic engagement. These results suggest that leveraging the link between Native identification and group injustices can motivate action.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 7","pages":"739-753"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9863628","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Adaptive Encoding Speed in Working Memory. 工作记忆中的自适应编码速度。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231173902
Joost de Jong, Hedderik van Rijn, Elkan G Akyürek

Humans can adapt when complex patterns unfold at a faster or slower pace, for instance when remembering a grocery list that is dictated at an increasingly fast rate. Integrating information over such timescales crucially depends on working memory, but although recent findings have shown that working memory capacity can be flexibly adapted, such adaptations have not yet been demonstrated for encoding speed. In a series of experiments, we found that young adults encoded at a faster rate when they were adapted to overall and recent stimulus duration. Interestingly, our participants were unable to use explicit cues to speed up encoding, even though these cues were objectively more informative than statistical information. Our findings suggest that adaptive tuning of encoding speed in working memory is a fundamental but largely implicit mechanism underlying our ability to keep up with the pace of our surroundings.

当复杂的模式以更快或更慢的速度展开时,人类可以适应,例如,在记住以越来越快的速度口述的购物清单时。在这样的时间尺度上整合信息至关重要地取决于工作记忆,但尽管最近的研究结果表明,工作记忆容量可以灵活地调整,但这种调整尚未被证明适用于编码速度。在一系列的实验中,我们发现,当年轻人适应了整体和近期的刺激持续时间时,他们的编码速度更快。有趣的是,我们的参与者无法使用明确的线索来加速编码,即使这些线索客观上比统计信息更有信息量。我们的研究结果表明,工作记忆中编码速度的适应性调整是一种基本的、但在很大程度上是隐性的机制,是我们跟上周围环境节奏的能力的基础。
{"title":"Adaptive Encoding Speed in Working Memory.","authors":"Joost de Jong,&nbsp;Hedderik van Rijn,&nbsp;Elkan G Akyürek","doi":"10.1177/09567976231173902","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231173902","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans can adapt when complex patterns unfold at a faster or slower pace, for instance when remembering a grocery list that is dictated at an increasingly fast rate. Integrating information over such timescales crucially depends on working memory, but although recent findings have shown that working memory capacity can be flexibly adapted, such adaptations have not yet been demonstrated for encoding speed. In a series of experiments, we found that young adults encoded at a faster rate when they were adapted to overall and recent stimulus duration. Interestingly, our participants were unable to use explicit cues to speed up encoding, even though these cues were objectively more informative than statistical information. Our findings suggest that adaptive tuning of encoding speed in working memory is a fundamental but largely implicit mechanism underlying our ability to keep up with the pace of our surroundings.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 7","pages":"822-833"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9851734","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Just Dead, Not Alive: Reconsidering Belief in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories. 只是死了,不是活着:重新考虑相互矛盾的阴谋论的信仰。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231158570
Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Iris Wahring, Laura Mausolf, Nicole Mulas, Shayda Shwan

A well-established finding is that beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories (e.g., Princess Diana was murdered vs. faked her own death) are positively correlated. This is commonly interpreted as evidence that people systematically believe blatant inconsistencies. Here, we propose that the field has insufficiently acknowledged a compelling alternative explanation: Disbelieving both conspiracy theories also yields a positive correlation. In four preregistered studies (total N = 7,641 adults), online participants evaluated 28 sets of contradictory conspiracy theories. Although the positive correlation was replicated in all cases, this was mostly due to participants who believed the official versions of these events (e.g., Princess Diana died in a car accident). Among participants who disbelieved these official stories, the correlation was inconsistent at best. A mini meta-analysis revealed a negative correlation among these participants, which was particularly due to the dead-or-alive cases. Apparently, researchers should reconsider the notion of systematic belief in contradictory conspiracy theories.

一个公认的发现是,对相互矛盾的阴谋论的信仰(例如,戴安娜王妃是被谋杀的还是伪造自己的死亡)是正相关的。这通常被解释为人们系统性地相信明显不一致的证据。在这里,我们提出,该领域还没有充分承认一个令人信服的替代解释:不相信这两个阴谋论也产生了正相关。在四项预先登记的研究中(总共7641名成年人),在线参与者评估了28组相互矛盾的阴谋论。尽管这种正相关性在所有情况下都得到了证实,但这主要是由于参与者相信这些事件的官方版本(例如,戴安娜王妃死于车祸)。在不相信这些官方故事的参与者中,这种相关性充其量是不一致的。一项小型荟萃分析显示,这些参与者之间存在负相关,特别是由于死或活的病例。显然,研究人员应该重新考虑系统地相信相互矛盾的阴谋论的概念。
{"title":"Just Dead, Not Alive: Reconsidering Belief in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories.","authors":"Jan-Willem van Prooijen,&nbsp;Iris Wahring,&nbsp;Laura Mausolf,&nbsp;Nicole Mulas,&nbsp;Shayda Shwan","doi":"10.1177/09567976231158570","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231158570","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A well-established finding is that beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories (e.g., Princess Diana was murdered vs. faked her own death) are positively correlated. This is commonly interpreted as evidence that people systematically believe blatant inconsistencies. Here, we propose that the field has insufficiently acknowledged a compelling alternative explanation: Disbelieving both conspiracy theories also yields a positive correlation. In four preregistered studies (total <i>N</i> = 7,641 adults), online participants evaluated 28 sets of contradictory conspiracy theories. Although the positive correlation was replicated in all cases, this was mostly due to participants who believed the official versions of these events (e.g., Princess Diana died in a car accident). Among participants who disbelieved these official stories, the correlation was inconsistent at best. A mini meta-analysis revealed a negative correlation among these participants, which was particularly due to the dead-or-alive cases. Apparently, researchers should reconsider the notion of systematic belief in contradictory conspiracy theories.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"670-682"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9998561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Intelligence Polygenic Score Is More Predictive of Crystallized Measures: Evidence From the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. 智力多基因分数更能预测结晶测量结果:青少年大脑认知发展(ABCD)研究的证据。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-05 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231160702
Robert J Loughnan, Clare E Palmer, Wesley K Thompson, Anders M Dale, Terry L Jernigan, Chun Chieh Fan

Findings in adults have shown that crystallized measures of intelligence, which are more culturally sensitive than fluid intelligence measures, have greater heritability; however, these results have not been found in children. The present study used data from 8,518 participants between 9 and 11 years old from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. We found that polygenic predictors of intelligence test performance (based on genome-wide association meta-analyses of data from 269,867 individuals) and of educational attainment (based on data from 1.1 million individuals) predicted neurocognitive performance. We found that crystallized measures were more strongly associated with both polygenic predictors than were fluid measures. This mirrored heritability differences reported previously in adults and suggests similar associations in children. This may be consistent with a prominent role of gene-environment correlation in cognitive development measured by crystallized intelligence tests. Environmental and experiential mediators may represent malleable targets for improving cognitive outcomes.

对成人的研究结果表明,与流体智力测验相比,对文化更为敏感的固化智力测验具有更大的遗传性;然而,在儿童身上却没有发现这些结果。本研究使用了青少年大脑认知发展(ABCD)研究中 8518 名 9 至 11 岁参与者的数据。我们发现,智力测验成绩的多基因预测因子(基于对 269,867 人的数据进行的全基因组关联荟萃分析)和教育程度的多基因预测因子(基于 110 万人的数据)可预测神经认知成绩。我们发现,与流体测量相比,结晶测量与这两个多基因预测因子的相关性更强。这反映了之前报道的成人遗传性差异,并表明在儿童中也存在类似的关联。这可能与基因-环境相关性在固化智力测验所测量的认知发展中的突出作用相一致。环境和经验中介因素可能是改善认知结果的可塑目标。
{"title":"Intelligence Polygenic Score Is More Predictive of Crystallized Measures: Evidence From the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study.","authors":"Robert J Loughnan, Clare E Palmer, Wesley K Thompson, Anders M Dale, Terry L Jernigan, Chun Chieh Fan","doi":"10.1177/09567976231160702","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976231160702","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Findings in adults have shown that crystallized measures of intelligence, which are more culturally sensitive than fluid intelligence measures, have greater heritability; however, these results have not been found in children. The present study used data from 8,518 participants between 9 and 11 years old from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. We found that polygenic predictors of intelligence test performance (based on genome-wide association meta-analyses of data from 269,867 individuals) and of educational attainment (based on data from 1.1 million individuals) predicted neurocognitive performance. We found that crystallized measures were more strongly associated with both polygenic predictors than were fluid measures. This mirrored heritability differences reported previously in adults and suggests similar associations in children. This may be consistent with a prominent role of gene-environment correlation in cognitive development measured by crystallized intelligence tests. Environmental and experiential mediators may represent malleable targets for improving cognitive outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"714-725"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10404902/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10328656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thinking About God Encourages Prosociality Toward Religious Outgroups: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. 思考上帝鼓励对宗教外群体的亲社会:一项跨文化调查。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231158576
Michael H Pasek, John Michael Kelly, Crystal Shackleford, Cindel J M White, Allon Vishkin, Julia M Smith, Ara Norenzayan, Azim Shariff, Jeremy Ginges

Most humans believe in a god or gods, a belief that may promote prosociality toward coreligionists. A critical question is whether such enhanced prosociality is primarily parochial and confined to the religious ingroup or whether it extends to members of religious outgroups. To address this question, we conducted field and online experiments with Christian, Muslim, Hindu, and Jewish adults in the Middle East, Fiji, and the United States (N = 4,753). Participants were given the opportunity to share money with anonymous strangers from different ethno-religious groups. We manipulated whether they were asked to think about their god before making their choice. Thinking about God increased giving by 11% (4.17% of the total stake), an increase that was extended equally to ingroup and outgroup members. This suggests that belief in a god or gods may facilitate intergroup cooperation, particularly in economic transactions, even in contexts with heightened intergroup tension.

大多数人相信一个或多个神,这种信仰可能会促进对同宗教者的亲社会性。一个关键的问题是,这种增强的亲社会性是否主要局限于宗教内部群体,还是延伸到宗教外部群体的成员。为了解决这个问题,我们对中东、斐济和美国的基督徒、穆斯林、印度教和犹太教成年人进行了实地和在线实验(N = 4,753)。参与者有机会与来自不同种族和宗教团体的匿名陌生人分享金钱。我们操纵了他们在做出选择之前是否被要求思考他们的上帝。对上帝的思考使捐赠增加了11%(占总股份的4.17%),群体内和群体外的成员都有同样的增加。这表明,对一个或多个神的信仰可能会促进群体间的合作,特别是在经济交易中,即使是在群体间紧张关系加剧的背景下。
{"title":"Thinking About God Encourages Prosociality Toward Religious Outgroups: A Cross-Cultural Investigation.","authors":"Michael H Pasek,&nbsp;John Michael Kelly,&nbsp;Crystal Shackleford,&nbsp;Cindel J M White,&nbsp;Allon Vishkin,&nbsp;Julia M Smith,&nbsp;Ara Norenzayan,&nbsp;Azim Shariff,&nbsp;Jeremy Ginges","doi":"10.1177/09567976231158576","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231158576","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most humans believe in a god or gods, a belief that may promote prosociality toward coreligionists. A critical question is whether such enhanced prosociality is primarily parochial and confined to the religious ingroup or whether it extends to members of religious outgroups. To address this question, we conducted field and online experiments with Christian, Muslim, Hindu, and Jewish adults in the Middle East, Fiji, and the United States (<i>N</i> = 4,753). Participants were given the opportunity to share money with anonymous strangers from different ethno-religious groups. We manipulated whether they were asked to think about their god before making their choice. Thinking about God increased giving by 11% (4.17% of the total stake), an increase that was extended equally to ingroup and outgroup members. This suggests that belief in a god or gods may facilitate intergroup cooperation, particularly in economic transactions, even in contexts with heightened intergroup tension.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"657-669"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9633952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Surviving Racism and Sexism: What Votes in the Television Program Survivor Reveal About Discrimination. 幸存的种族主义和性别歧视:电视节目《幸存者》中的投票揭示了歧视。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1177/09567976231165665
Erin M O'Mara Kunz, Jennifer L Howell, Nicole Beasley

We examined whether there is evidence for racial and gender bias in the voting patterns of contestants on Survivor, a reality-television zero-sum game in which contestants compete for up to 39 days to win $1 million. Among 731 contestants across 40 seasons, we found evidence of racial and gender bias at multiple stages of Survivor. Compared with men, women were more likely to be voted out of their tribe first and were less likely to make it to the individual-competition stage of the game (i.e., the "merge"). They were also less likely to win Survivor. Black, Indigenous, and people of color (BIPOC) contestants, compared with White contestants, were more likely to be voted out of their tribe first and were less likely to make it to the individual-competition stage of the game. These findings suggest a systemic bias in favor of White men and against women of color.

我们研究了在真人秀节目《幸存者》(Survivor)中,参赛者的投票模式中是否存在种族和性别偏见的证据。《幸存者》是一款零和游戏,参赛者要在长达39天的比赛中赢得100万美元。在40季的731名参赛者中,我们发现了在《幸存者》的多个阶段存在种族和性别偏见的证据。与男性相比,女性更有可能首先被淘汰出部落,更不可能进入游戏的个人竞争阶段(即“合并”)。他们也不太可能赢得幸存者。与白人选手相比,黑人、土著和有色人种(BIPOC)选手更有可能首先被部落淘汰,更不可能进入比赛的个人竞争阶段。这些发现表明了一种支持白人男性、反对有色人种女性的系统性偏见。
{"title":"Surviving Racism and Sexism: What Votes in the Television Program <i>Survivor</i> Reveal About Discrimination.","authors":"Erin M O'Mara Kunz,&nbsp;Jennifer L Howell,&nbsp;Nicole Beasley","doi":"10.1177/09567976231165665","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976231165665","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined whether there is evidence for racial and gender bias in the voting patterns of contestants on <i>Survivor</i>, a reality-television zero-sum game in which contestants compete for up to 39 days to win $1 million. Among 731 contestants across 40 seasons, we found evidence of racial and gender bias at multiple stages of <i>Survivor.</i> Compared with men, women were more likely to be voted out of their tribe first and were less likely to make it to the individual-competition stage of the game (i.e., the \"merge\"). They were also less likely to win <i>Survivor</i>. Black, Indigenous, and people of color (BIPOC) contestants, compared with White contestants, were more likely to be voted out of their tribe first and were less likely to make it to the individual-competition stage of the game. These findings suggest a systemic bias in favor of White men and against women of color.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"726-735"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9633973","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lonely Individuals Process the World in Idiosyncratic Ways. 孤独的人以奇特的方式处理世界。
IF 8.2 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-07 DOI: 10.1177/09567976221145316
Elisa C Baek, Ryan Hyon, Karina López, Meng Du, Mason A Porter, Carolyn Parkinson

Loneliness is detrimental to well-being and is often accompanied by self-reported feelings of not being understood by other people. What contributes to such feelings in lonely people? We used functional MRI of 66 first-year university students to unobtrusively measure the relative alignment of people's mental processing of naturalistic stimuli and tested whether lonely people actually process the world in idiosyncratic ways. We found evidence for such idiosyncrasy: Lonely individuals' neural responses were dissimilar to those of their peers, particularly in regions of the default-mode network in which similar responses have been associated with shared perspectives and subjective understanding. These relationships persisted when we controlled for demographic similarities, objective social isolation, and individuals' friendships with each other. Our findings raise the possibility that being surrounded by people who see the world differently from oneself, even if one is friends with them, may be a risk factor for loneliness.

孤独感有损身心健康,而且常常伴随着自我报告的不被他人理解的感觉。是什么导致了孤独者的这种感觉?我们对 66 名大学一年级学生进行了功能性核磁共振成像,以不显眼的方式测量了人们对自然刺激进行心理处理的相对一致性,并测试了孤独的人是否真的以特异的方式处理世界。我们发现了这种特异性的证据:孤独者的神经反应与同龄人不同,尤其是在默认模式网络区域,在这些区域,相似的反应与共同的观点和主观理解有关。当我们对人口统计学相似性、客观社会隔离和个体之间的友谊进行控制时,这些关系依然存在。我们的研究结果提出了这样一种可能性,即周围的人对世界的看法与自己不同,即使与他们是朋友,也可能是导致孤独的一个危险因素。
{"title":"Lonely Individuals Process the World in Idiosyncratic Ways.","authors":"Elisa C Baek, Ryan Hyon, Karina López, Meng Du, Mason A Porter, Carolyn Parkinson","doi":"10.1177/09567976221145316","DOIUrl":"10.1177/09567976221145316","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Loneliness is detrimental to well-being and is often accompanied by self-reported feelings of not being understood by other people. What contributes to such feelings in lonely people? We used functional MRI of 66 first-year university students to unobtrusively measure the relative alignment of people's mental processing of naturalistic stimuli and tested whether lonely people actually process the world in idiosyncratic ways. We found evidence for such idiosyncrasy: Lonely individuals' neural responses were dissimilar to those of their peers, particularly in regions of the default-mode network in which similar responses have been associated with shared perspectives and subjective understanding. These relationships persisted when we controlled for demographic similarities, objective social isolation, and individuals' friendships with each other. Our findings raise the possibility that being surrounded by people who see the world differently from oneself, even if one is friends with them, may be a risk factor for loneliness.</p>","PeriodicalId":20745,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Science","volume":"34 6","pages":"683-695"},"PeriodicalIF":8.2,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10404901/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10328169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1