首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
The dual role of culture for reconstructing early sapiens cognition. 文化对重建早期智人认知的双重作用。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000462
Andrea Bender, Larissa Mendoza Straffon, John B Gatewood, Sieghard Beller

Questions on early sapiens cognition, the cognitive abilities of our ancestors, are intriguing but notoriously hard to tackle. Leaving no hard traces in the archeological record, these abilities need to be inferred from indirect evidence, informed by our understanding of present-day cognition. Most of such attempts acknowledge the role that culture, as a faculty, has played for human evolution, but they underrate or even disregard the role of distinct cultural traditions and the ensuing diversity, both in present-day humans and as a dimension of past cognition. We argue that culture has exerted a profound impact on human cognition from the start in a dual manner: It scaffolds cognition through both development and evolution, and it thereby continually diversifies the form and content of human thinking. To unveil early sapiens cognition and retrace its evolutionary trajectories, this cognitive diversity must be considered. We present two strategies to achieve this: large-scale extrapolation and phylogenetic comparison. The former aims at filtering out diversity to determine what is basic and universal versus culturally shaped (illustrated for theory of mind abilities). The latter capitalizes on the diversity to reconstruct evolutionary trajectories (illustrated for religious beliefs). The two methods, in combination, advance our understanding of the cognitive abilities of our early sapiens ancestors and of how these abilities emerged and evolved. To conclude, we discuss the implications of this approach for our insights into early cognition itself and its scientific investigation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

关于早期智人的认知,也就是我们祖先的认知能力,这些问题很有意思,但却很难解决。考古记录中没有留下任何确凿的痕迹,这些能力需要我们根据对当今认知的理解,从间接证据中推断出来。大多数此类尝试都承认文化作为一种能力对人类进化所起的作用,但它们低估甚至无视独特文化传统的作用以及随之而来的多样性,无论是在当今人类身上还是作为过去认知的一个维度。我们认为,文化从一开始就以双重方式对人类认知产生了深远影响:它通过发展和进化为认知提供支架,从而不断丰富人类思维的形式和内容。要揭示早期智人的认知并追溯其进化轨迹,就必须考虑这种认知的多样性。为此,我们提出了两种策略:大规模外推法和系统发育比较法。前者旨在筛选出多样性,以确定哪些是基本的、普遍的,哪些是文化塑造的(以心智理论能力为例)。后者利用多样性重建进化轨迹(以宗教信仰为例)。这两种方法结合起来,有助于我们了解早期智人祖先的认知能力,以及这些能力是如何出现和进化的。最后,我们讨论了这种方法对我们了解早期认知本身及其科学研究的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The dual role of culture for reconstructing early sapiens cognition.","authors":"Andrea Bender, Larissa Mendoza Straffon, John B Gatewood, Sieghard Beller","doi":"10.1037/rev0000462","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000462","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Questions on early sapiens cognition, the cognitive abilities of our ancestors, are intriguing but notoriously hard to tackle. Leaving no hard traces in the archeological record, these abilities need to be inferred from indirect evidence, informed by our understanding of present-day cognition. Most of such attempts acknowledge the role that culture, as a faculty, has played for human evolution, but they underrate or even disregard the role of distinct cultural traditions and the ensuing diversity, both in present-day humans and as a dimension of past cognition. We argue that culture has exerted a profound impact on human cognition from the start in a dual manner: It scaffolds cognition through both development and evolution, and it thereby continually diversifies the form and content of human thinking. To unveil early sapiens cognition and retrace its evolutionary trajectories, this cognitive diversity must be considered. We present two strategies to achieve this: large-scale extrapolation and phylogenetic comparison. The former aims at filtering out diversity to determine what is basic and universal versus culturally shaped (illustrated for theory of mind abilities). The latter capitalizes on the diversity to reconstruct evolutionary trajectories (illustrated for religious beliefs). The two methods, in combination, advance our understanding of the cognitive abilities of our early sapiens ancestors and of how these abilities emerged and evolved. To conclude, we discuss the implications of this approach for our insights into early cognition itself and its scientific investigation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138807774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relation between learning and stimulus-response binding. 学习与刺激-反应结合之间的关系。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000449
Christian Frings, Anna Foerster, Birte Moeller, Bernhard Pastötter, Roland Pfister

Perception and action rely on integrating or binding different features of stimuli and responses. Such bindings are short-lived, but they can be retrieved for a limited amount of time if any of their features is reactivated. This is particularly true for stimulus-response bindings, allowing for flexible recycling of previous action plans. A relation to learning of stimulus-response associations suggests itself, and previous accounts have proposed binding as an initial step of forging associations in long-term memory. The evidence for this claim is surprisingly mixed, however. Here we propose a framework that explains previous failures to detect meaningful relations of binding and learning by highlighting the joint contribution of three variables: (a) decay, (b) the number of repetitions, and (c) the time elapsing between repetitions. Accounting for the interplay of these variables provides a promising blueprint for innovative experimental designs that bridge the gap between immediate bindings on the one hand and lasting associations in memory on the other hand. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

感知和行动依赖于整合或绑定刺激和反应的不同特征。这种绑定是短暂的,但如果其中任何一个特征被重新激活,它们就可以在有限的时间内恢复。这一点在刺激-反应绑定中尤为明显,可以灵活地循环使用以前的行动计划。这与刺激-反应联想的学习有关,以前的观点认为绑定是在长时记忆中建立联想的第一步。然而,这一观点的证据却出人意料地参差不齐。在这里,我们提出了一个框架,通过强调三个变量的共同作用来解释之前未能发现有意义的绑定和学习关系的原因:(a)衰减,(b)重复次数,以及(c)重复之间的时间间隔。考虑到这些变量之间的相互作用,为创新实验设计提供了一个前景广阔的蓝图,从而在即时绑定与记忆中的持久联想之间架起了一座桥梁。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The relation between learning and stimulus-response binding.","authors":"Christian Frings, Anna Foerster, Birte Moeller, Bernhard Pastötter, Roland Pfister","doi":"10.1037/rev0000449","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000449","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perception and action rely on integrating or binding different features of stimuli and responses. Such bindings are short-lived, but they can be retrieved for a limited amount of time if any of their features is reactivated. This is particularly true for stimulus-response bindings, allowing for flexible recycling of previous action plans. A relation to learning of stimulus-response associations suggests itself, and previous accounts have proposed binding as an initial step of forging associations in long-term memory. The evidence for this claim is surprisingly mixed, however. Here we propose a framework that explains previous failures to detect meaningful relations of binding and learning by highlighting the joint contribution of three variables: (a) decay, (b) the number of repetitions, and (c) the time elapsing between repetitions. Accounting for the interplay of these variables provides a promising blueprint for innovative experimental designs that bridge the gap between immediate bindings on the one hand and lasting associations in memory on the other hand. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138807777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Unique Effects of Sedatives, Dissociatives, Psychedelics, Stimulants, and Cannabinoids on Episodic Memory: A Review and Reanalysis of Acute Drug Effects on Recollection, Familiarity, and Metamemory 镇静剂、解离剂、迷幻剂、兴奋剂和大麻类药物对外显记忆的独特影响》补充材料:回顾并重新分析急性药物对回忆、熟悉感和元记忆的影响
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000455.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Unique Effects of Sedatives, Dissociatives, Psychedelics, Stimulants, and Cannabinoids on Episodic Memory: A Review and Reanalysis of Acute Drug Effects on Recollection, Familiarity, and Metamemory","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/rev0000455.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000455.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138973302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Spatial Versus Graphical Representation of Distributional Semantic Knowledge 分布语义知识的空间与图形表示补充材料
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-13 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000451.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Spatial Versus Graphical Representation of Distributional Semantic Knowledge","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/rev0000451.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000451.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134993770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Simultaneous modeling of choice, confidence, and response time in visual perception. 同时模拟视觉感知中的选择、信心和反应时间。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-13 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000411
Sebastian Hellmann, Michael Zehetleitner, Manuel Rausch

How can choice, confidence, and response times be modeled simultaneously? Here, we propose the new dynamical weighted evidence and visibility (dynWEV) model, an extension of the drift-diffusion model of decision-making, to account for choices, reaction times, and confidence simultaneously. The decision process in a binary perceptual task is described as a Wiener process accumulating sensory evidence about the choice options bounded by two constant thresholds. To account for confidence judgments, we assume a period of postdecisional accumulation of sensory evidence and parallel accumulation of information about the reliability of the present stimulus. We examined model fits in two experiments, a motion discrimination task with random dot kinematograms and a postmasked orientation discrimination task. A comparison between the dynWEV model, two-stage dynamical signal detection theory, and several versions of race models of decision-making showed that only dynWEV produced acceptable fits of choices, confidence, and reaction time. This finding suggests that confidence judgments depend not only on choice evidence but also on a parallel estimate of stimulus discriminability and postdecisional accumulation of evidence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

如何同时模拟选择、信心和反应时间?在这里,我们提出了新的动态加权证据和能见度(dynWEV)模型,它是决策漂移-扩散模型的扩展,可以同时考虑选择、反应时间和信心。二元知觉任务中的决策过程被描述为一个维纳过程,在两个恒定阈值的约束下积累有关选择选项的感官证据。为了解释置信度判断,我们假定决策后会有一段感官证据积累期,并同时积累关于当前刺激可靠性的信息。我们在两个实验中检验了模型的拟合情况,一个是随机点运动图的运动辨别任务,另一个是掩蔽后的方位辨别任务。通过比较 dynWEV 模型、两阶段动态信号检测理论和几个版本的决策竞赛模型,我们发现只有 dynWEV 模型对选择、置信度和反应时间的拟合是可以接受的。这一发现表明,信心判断不仅取决于选择证据,还取决于对刺激可辨别性的平行估计和决策后的证据积累。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Simultaneous modeling of choice, confidence, and response time in visual perception.","authors":"Sebastian Hellmann, Michael Zehetleitner, Manuel Rausch","doi":"10.1037/rev0000411","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000411","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How can choice, confidence, and response times be modeled simultaneously? Here, we propose the new dynamical weighted evidence and visibility (dynWEV) model, an extension of the drift-diffusion model of decision-making, to account for choices, reaction times, and confidence simultaneously. The decision process in a binary perceptual task is described as a Wiener process accumulating sensory evidence about the choice options bounded by two constant thresholds. To account for confidence judgments, we assume a period of postdecisional accumulation of sensory evidence and parallel accumulation of information about the reliability of the present stimulus. We examined model fits in two experiments, a motion discrimination task with random dot kinematograms and a postmasked orientation discrimination task. A comparison between the dynWEV model, two-stage dynamical signal detection theory, and several versions of race models of decision-making showed that only dynWEV produced acceptable fits of choices, confidence, and reaction time. This finding suggests that confidence judgments depend not only on choice evidence but also on a parallel estimate of stimulus discriminability and postdecisional accumulation of evidence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9086830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Causal inference methods for intergenerational research using observational data. 利用观察数据进行代际研究的因果推断方法。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-24 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000419
Leonard Frach, Eshim S Jami, Tom A McAdams, Frank Dudbridge, Jean-Baptiste Pingault

Identifying early causal factors leading to the development of poor mental health and behavioral outcomes is essential to design efficient preventive interventions. The substantial associations observed between parental risk factors (e.g., maternal stress in pregnancy, parental education, parental psychopathology, parent-child relationship) and child outcomes point toward the importance of parents in shaping child outcomes. However, such associations may also reflect confounding, including genetic transmission-that is, the child inherits genetic risk common to the parental risk factor and the child outcome. This can generate associations in the absence of a causal effect. As randomized trials and experiments are often not feasible or ethical, observational studies can help to infer causality under specific assumptions. This review aims to provide a comprehensive summary of current causal inference methods using observational data in intergenerational settings. We present the rich causal inference toolbox currently available to researchers, including genetically informed and analytical methods, and discuss their application to child mental health and related outcomes. We outline promising research areas and discuss how existing approaches can be combined or extended to probe the causal nature of intergenerational effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

识别导致不良心理健康和行为结果的早期因果因素,对于设计有效的预防干预措施至关重要。在父母的风险因素(如母亲在怀孕期间的压力、父母的教育程度、父母的精神病理学、亲子关系)和儿童的结果之间观察到的大量关联表明,父母在影响儿童结果方面起着重要作用。然而,这种关联也可能反映了混杂因素,包括遗传传递--即子女继承了父母风险因素和子女结果的共同遗传风险。这可能在没有因果效应的情况下产生关联。由于随机试验和实验往往不可行或不符合伦理道德,观察性研究有助于在特定假设条件下推断因果关系。本综述旨在全面总结目前在代际环境中利用观察数据进行因果推断的方法。我们介绍了目前可供研究人员使用的丰富的因果推断工具箱,包括基因信息和分析方法,并讨论了它们在儿童心理健康及相关结果中的应用。我们概述了前景广阔的研究领域,并讨论了如何结合或扩展现有方法来探究代际效应的因果性质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Causal inference methods for intergenerational research using observational data.","authors":"Leonard Frach, Eshim S Jami, Tom A McAdams, Frank Dudbridge, Jean-Baptiste Pingault","doi":"10.1037/rev0000419","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000419","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Identifying early causal factors leading to the development of poor mental health and behavioral outcomes is essential to design efficient preventive interventions. The substantial associations observed between parental risk factors (e.g., maternal stress in pregnancy, parental education, parental psychopathology, parent-child relationship) and child outcomes point toward the importance of parents in shaping child outcomes. However, such associations may also reflect confounding, including genetic transmission-that is, the child inherits genetic risk common to the parental risk factor and the child outcome. This can generate associations in the absence of a causal effect. As randomized trials and experiments are often not feasible or ethical, observational studies can help to infer causality under specific assumptions. This review aims to provide a comprehensive summary of current causal inference methods using observational data in intergenerational settings. We present the rich causal inference toolbox currently available to researchers, including genetically informed and analytical methods, and discuss their application to child mental health and related outcomes. We outline promising research areas and discuss how existing approaches can be combined or extended to probe the causal nature of intergenerational effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9459809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mood-congruent memory revisited. 重温情绪一致性记忆
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2022-10-06 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000394
Leonard Faul, Kevin S LaBar

Affective experiences are commonly represented by either transient emotional reactions to discrete events or longer term, sustained mood states that are characterized by a more diffuse and global nature. While both have considerable influence in shaping memory, their interaction can produce mood-congruent memory (MCM), a psychological phenomenon where emotional memory is biased toward content affectively congruent with a past or current mood. The study of MCM has direct implications for understanding how memory biases form in daily life, as well as debilitating negative memory schemas that contribute to mood disorders such as depression. To elucidate the factors that influence the presence and strength of MCM, here we systematically review the literature for studies that assessed MCM by inducing mood in healthy participants. We observe that MCM is often reported as enhanced accuracy for previously encoded mood-congruent content or preferential recall for mood-congruent autobiographical events, but may also manifest as false memory for mood-congruent lures. We discuss the relevant conditions that shape these effects, as well as instances of mood-incongruent recall that facilitate mood repair. Further, we provide guiding methodological and theoretical considerations, emphasizing the limited neuroimaging research in this area and the need for a renewed focus on memory consolidation. Accordingly, we propose a theoretical framework for studying the neural basis of MCM based on the neurobiological underpinnings of mood and emotion. In doing so, we review evidence for associative network models of spreading activation, while also considering alternative models informed by the cognitive neuroscience literature of emotional memory bias. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪体验通常表现为对离散事件的短暂情绪反应,或较长期、持续的情绪状态,后者的特点是更分散、更全面。虽然两者在塑造记忆方面都有相当大的影响,但它们之间的相互作用会产生情绪一致性记忆(MCM),这是一种心理现象,即情绪记忆偏向于与过去或当前情绪相一致的情感内容。研究 MCM 对理解日常生活中记忆偏差的形成以及导致抑郁症等情绪障碍的负面记忆图式具有直接影响。为了阐明影响 MCM 存在和强度的因素,我们在此系统地回顾了通过诱导健康参与者的情绪来评估 MCM 的研究文献。我们观察到,MCM 通常表现为对先前编码的与情绪一致内容的准确性增强,或对与情绪一致的自传体事件的优先回忆,但也可能表现为对与情绪一致的引诱物的错误记忆。我们将讨论形成这些效应的相关条件,以及促进情绪修复的情绪不一致回忆的实例。此外,我们还提供了指导性的方法和理论考虑,强调了这一领域有限的神经影像学研究以及重新关注记忆巩固的必要性。因此,我们基于情绪和情感的神经生物学基础,提出了研究 MCM 神经基础的理论框架。在此过程中,我们回顾了扩散激活关联网络模型的证据,同时也考虑了认知神经科学文献中关于情绪记忆偏差的替代模型。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Mood-congruent memory revisited.","authors":"Leonard Faul, Kevin S LaBar","doi":"10.1037/rev0000394","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000394","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affective experiences are commonly represented by either transient emotional reactions to discrete events or longer term, sustained mood states that are characterized by a more diffuse and global nature. While both have considerable influence in shaping memory, their interaction can produce mood-congruent memory (MCM), a psychological phenomenon where emotional memory is biased toward content affectively congruent with a past or current mood. The study of MCM has direct implications for understanding how memory biases form in daily life, as well as debilitating negative memory schemas that contribute to mood disorders such as depression. To elucidate the factors that influence the presence and strength of MCM, here we systematically review the literature for studies that assessed MCM by inducing mood in healthy participants. We observe that MCM is often reported as enhanced accuracy for previously encoded mood-congruent content or preferential recall for mood-congruent autobiographical events, but may also manifest as false memory for mood-congruent lures. We discuss the relevant conditions that shape these effects, as well as instances of mood-incongruent recall that facilitate mood repair. Further, we provide guiding methodological and theoretical considerations, emphasizing the limited neuroimaging research in this area and the need for a renewed focus on memory consolidation. Accordingly, we propose a theoretical framework for studying the neural basis of MCM based on the neurobiological underpinnings of mood and emotion. In doing so, we review evidence for associative network models of spreading activation, while also considering alternative models informed by the cognitive neuroscience literature of emotional memory bias. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10076454/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9265672","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optimal nudging for cognitively bounded agents: A framework for modeling, predicting, and controlling the effects of choice architectures. 认知受限主体的最优微调:一个用于建模、预测和控制选择架构效果的框架。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000445
Frederick Callaway, Mathew Hardy, Thomas L Griffiths

People's decisions often deviate from classical notions of rationality, incurring costs to themselves and society. One way to reduce the costs of poor decisions is to redesign the decision problems people face to encourage better choices. While often subtle, these nudges can have dramatic effects on behavior and are increasingly popular in public policy, health care, and marketing. Although nudges are often designed with psychological theories in mind, they are typically not formalized in computational terms and their effects can be hard to predict. As a result, designing nudges can be difficult and time-consuming. To address this challenge, we propose a computational framework for understanding and predicting the effects of nudges. Our approach builds on recent work modeling human decision making as adaptive use of limited cognitive resources, an approach called resource-rational analysis. In our framework, nudges change the metalevel problem the agent faces-that is, the problem of how to make a decision. This changes the optimal sequence of cognitive operations an agent should execute, which in turn influences their behavior. We show that models based on this framework can account for known effects of nudges based on default options, suggested alternatives, and information highlighting. In each case, we validate the model's predictions in an experimental process-tracing paradigm. We then show how the framework can be used to automatically construct optimal nudges, and demonstrate that these nudges improve people's decisions more than intuitive heuristic approaches. Overall, our results show that resource-rational analysis is a promising framework for formally characterizing and constructing nudges. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们的决策往往偏离了传统的理性观念,给自己和社会带来了代价。降低糟糕决策成本的一种方法是重新设计人们面临的决策问题,以鼓励更好的选择。虽然这些轻推往往很微妙,但会对行为产生巨大影响,并在公共政策、医疗保健和营销中越来越受欢迎。尽管轻推通常是在设计时考虑到心理理论的,但它们通常不是用计算的术语形式化的,其影响可能很难预测。因此,设计微调可能既困难又耗时。为了应对这一挑战,我们提出了一个理解和预测轻推效应的计算框架。我们的方法建立在最近的工作基础上,将人类决策建模为对有限认知资源的适应性使用,这种方法被称为资源理性分析。在我们的框架中,推动改变代理面临的元级问题,即如何做出决策的问题。这改变了代理应该执行的认知操作的最佳顺序,这反过来又影响了他们的行为。我们表明,基于该框架的模型可以解释基于默认选项、建议的替代方案和信息突出显示的微调的已知影响。在每种情况下,我们都会在实验过程跟踪范式中验证模型的预测。然后,我们展示了如何使用该框架来自动构建最优微调,并证明这些微调比直观的启发式方法更能改善人们的决策。总体而言,我们的结果表明,资源理性分析是一个很有前途的框架,可以正式表征和构建微调。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Optimal nudging for cognitively bounded agents: A framework for modeling, predicting, and controlling the effects of choice architectures.","authors":"Frederick Callaway, Mathew Hardy, Thomas L Griffiths","doi":"10.1037/rev0000445","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000445","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People's decisions often deviate from classical notions of rationality, incurring costs to themselves and society. One way to reduce the costs of poor decisions is to redesign the decision problems people face to encourage better choices. While often subtle, these <i>nudges</i> can have dramatic effects on behavior and are increasingly popular in public policy, health care, and marketing. Although nudges are often designed with psychological theories in mind, they are typically not formalized in computational terms and their effects can be hard to predict. As a result, designing nudges can be difficult and time-consuming. To address this challenge, we propose a computational framework for understanding and predicting the effects of nudges. Our approach builds on recent work modeling human decision making as adaptive use of limited cognitive resources, an approach called resource-rational analysis. In our framework, nudges change the <i>metalevel</i> problem the agent faces-that is, the problem of how to make a decision. This changes the optimal sequence of cognitive operations an agent should execute, which in turn influences their behavior. We show that models based on this framework can account for known effects of nudges based on default options, suggested alternatives, and information highlighting. In each case, we validate the model's predictions in an experimental process-tracing paradigm. We then show how the framework can be used to automatically construct optimal nudges, and demonstrate that these nudges improve people's decisions more than intuitive heuristic approaches. Overall, our results show that resource-rational analysis is a promising framework for formally characterizing and constructing nudges. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71426424","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Skeleton-based shape similarity. 基于骨架的形状相似性
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-06 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000412
Nathan Destler, Manish Singh, Jacob Feldman

Many aspects of visual perception, including the classification of shapes into known categories and the induction of new shape categories from examples, are driven by shape similarity. But there is as yet no generally agreed, principled measure of the degree to which two shapes are "similar." Here, we derive a measure of shape similarity based on the Bayesian skeleton estimation framework of Feldman and Singh (2006). The new measure, called generative similarity, is based on the idea that shapes should be considered similar in proportion to the posterior probability that they were generated from a common skeletal model rather than from distinct skeletal models. We report a series of experiments in which subjects were shown a small number (1, 2, or 3) of 2D or 3D "nonsense" shapes (generated randomly in a manner designed to avoid known shape categories) and asked to select other members of the "same" shape class from a larger set of (random) alternatives. We then modeled subjects' choices using a variety of shape similarity measures drawn from the literature, including our new measure, skeletal cross-likelihood, a skeleton-based measure recently proposed by Ayzenberg and Lourenco (2019), a nonskeletal part-based similarity model proposed by Erdogan and Jacobs (2017), and a convolutional neural network model (Vedaldi & Lenc, 2015). We found that our new similarity measure generally predicted subjects' selections better than these competing proposals. These results help explain how the human visual system evaluates shape similarity and open the door to a broader view of the induction of shape categories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉感知的许多方面,包括将形状归入已知类别以及从示例中归纳出新的形状类别,都是由形状相似性驱动的。但是,对于两个形状 "相似 "的程度,至今还没有一个公认的、原则性的测量方法。在此,我们基于费尔德曼和辛格(2006 年)的贝叶斯骨架估计框架,推导出一种形状相似度测量方法。这种新的测量方法被称为生成相似性,它所基于的理念是,形状的相似性应与它们由共同的骨骼模型而非不同的骨骼模型生成的后验概率成正比。我们报告了一系列实验,在这些实验中,我们向受试者展示了少量(1、2 或 3 个)二维或三维 "无意义 "形状(以避免已知形状类别的方式随机生成),并要求受试者从一组更大的(随机)备选形状中选择 "相同 "形状类别的其他成员。然后,我们使用文献中的各种形状相似性测量方法对受试者的选择进行建模,其中包括我们的新测量方法--骨骼交叉似然法、Ayzenberg 和 Lourenco(2019 年)最近提出的基于骨骼的测量方法、Erdogan 和 Jacobs(2017 年)提出的基于非骨骼部分的相似性模型以及卷积神经网络模型(Vedaldi & Lenc,2015 年)。我们发现,我们的新相似性测量方法对受试者选择的预测效果普遍优于这些竞争方案。这些结果有助于解释人类视觉系统是如何评估形状相似性的,并为更广泛地理解形状类别的归纳打开了大门。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Skeleton-based shape similarity.","authors":"Nathan Destler, Manish Singh, Jacob Feldman","doi":"10.1037/rev0000412","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000412","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many aspects of visual perception, including the classification of shapes into known categories and the induction of new shape categories from examples, are driven by <i>shape similarity.</i> But there is as yet no generally agreed, principled measure of the degree to which two shapes are \"similar.\" Here, we derive a measure of shape similarity based on the Bayesian skeleton estimation framework of Feldman and Singh (2006). The new measure, called <i>generative similarity,</i> is based on the idea that shapes should be considered similar in proportion to the posterior probability that they were generated from a common skeletal model rather than from distinct skeletal models. We report a series of experiments in which subjects were shown a small number (1, 2, or 3) of 2D or 3D \"nonsense\" shapes (generated randomly in a manner designed to avoid known shape categories) and asked to select other members of the \"same\" shape class from a larger set of (random) alternatives. We then modeled subjects' choices using a variety of shape similarity measures drawn from the literature, including our new measure, skeletal cross-likelihood, a skeleton-based measure recently proposed by Ayzenberg and Lourenco (2019), a nonskeletal part-based similarity model proposed by Erdogan and Jacobs (2017), and a convolutional neural network model (Vedaldi & Lenc, 2015). We found that our new similarity measure generally predicted subjects' selections better than these competing proposals. These results help explain how the human visual system evaluates shape similarity and open the door to a broader view of the induction of shape categories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10821256","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Serial order depends on item-dependent and item-independent contexts. 序列顺序取决于与项目相关和与项目无关的上下文。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-09 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000422
Gordon D Logan, Gregory E Cox

We address four issues in response to Osth and Hurlstone's (2022) commentary on the context retrieval and updating (CRU) theory of serial order (Logan, 2021). First, we clarify the relations between CRU, chains, and associations. We show that CRU is not equivalent to a chaining theory and uses similarity rather than association to retrieve contexts. Second, we fix an error Logan (2021) made in accounting for the tendency to recall ACB instead of ACD in recalling ABCDEF (fill-in vs. in-fill errors, respectively). When implemented correctly, the idea that subjects mix the current context with an initial list cue after the first order error correctly predicts that fill-in errors are more frequent than in-fill errors. Third, we address position-specific prior-list intrusions, suggesting modifications to CRU and introducing a position-coding model based on CRU representations to account for them. We suggest that position-specific prior-list intrusions are evidence for position coding on some proportion of the trials but are not evidence against item coding on other trials. Finally, we address position-specific between-group intrusions in structured lists, agreeing with Osth and Hurlstone that reasonable modifications to CRU cannot account for them. We suggest that such intrusions support position coding on some proportion of the trials but do not rule out CRU-like item-based codes. We conclude by suggesting that item-independent and item-dependent coding are alternative strategies for serial recall and we stress the importance of accounting for immediate performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

针对 Osth 和 Hurlstone(2022 年)对序列顺序的语境检索和更新(CRU)理论(Logan,2021 年)的评论,我们讨论了四个问题。首先,我们澄清了 CRU、链和关联之间的关系。我们证明 CRU 并不等同于链理论,它使用相似性而非关联性来检索上下文。其次,我们修正了 Logan(2021)在解释回忆 ABCDEF 时回忆 ACB 而非 ACD 的倾向(分别是填入错误和填入错误)时所犯的错误。在正确实施的情况下,受试者在一阶错误后将当前语境与初始列表线索混合的想法可以正确预测填入式错误比填入式错误更频繁。第三,我们讨论了特定位置的先验列表入侵,建议对 CRU 进行修改,并引入一个基于 CRU 表征的位置编码模型来解释这些入侵。我们认为,特定位置的先验列表入侵是部分试验中位置编码的证据,但并不能证明其他试验中的项目编码无效。最后,我们讨论了结构化列表中特定位置的组间干扰,同意 Osth 和 Hurlstone 的观点,即对 CRU 的合理修改无法解释这些干扰。我们认为,这种干扰支持部分试验中的位置编码,但并不排除类似 CRU 的基于项目的编码。最后,我们认为与项目无关的编码和与项目相关的编码是序列回忆的替代策略,并强调了考虑即时表现的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Serial order depends on item-dependent and item-independent contexts.","authors":"Gordon D Logan, Gregory E Cox","doi":"10.1037/rev0000422","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000422","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We address four issues in response to Osth and Hurlstone's (2022) commentary on the context retrieval and updating (CRU) theory of serial order (Logan, 2021). First, we clarify the relations between CRU, chains, and associations. We show that CRU is not equivalent to a chaining theory and uses similarity rather than association to retrieve contexts. Second, we fix an error Logan (2021) made in accounting for the tendency to recall ACB instead of ACD in recalling ABCDEF (fill-in vs. in-fill errors, respectively). When implemented correctly, the idea that subjects mix the current context with an initial list cue after the first order error correctly predicts that fill-in errors are more frequent than in-fill errors. Third, we address position-specific prior-list intrusions, suggesting modifications to CRU and introducing a position-coding model based on CRU representations to account for them. We suggest that position-specific prior-list intrusions are evidence for position coding on some proportion of the trials but are not evidence against item coding on other trials. Finally, we address position-specific between-group intrusions in structured lists, agreeing with Osth and Hurlstone that reasonable modifications to CRU cannot account for them. We suggest that such intrusions support position coding on some proportion of the trials but do not rule out CRU-like item-based codes. We conclude by suggesting that item-independent and item-dependent coding are alternative strategies for serial recall and we stress the importance of accounting for immediate performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10871399","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1