首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
Humans reconfigure target and distractor processing to address distinct task demands. 人类重新配置目标和干扰处理,以满足不同的任务需求。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000442
Harrison Ritz, Amitai Shenhav

When faced with distraction, we can focus more on goal-relevant information (targets) or focus less on goal-conflicting information (distractors). How people use cognitive control to distribute attention across targets and distractors remains unclear. We address this question by developing a novel Parametric Attentional Control Task that can "tag" participants' sensitivity to target and distractor information. We use these precise measures of attention to develop a novel process model that can explain how participants control attention toward targets and distractors. Across three experiments, we find that participants met the demands of this task by independently controlling their processing of target and distractor information, exhibiting distinct adaptations to manipulations of incentives and conflict. Whereas incentives preferentially led to target enhancement, conflict in the previous trial preferentially led to distractor suppression. These distinct drivers of control altered sensitivity to targets and distractors early in the trial, promptly followed by reactive reconfiguration toward task-appropriate feature sensitivity. To provide a process-level account of these empirical findings, we develop a novel neural network model of evidence accumulation with attractor dynamics over feature weights that reconfigure target and distractor processing. These results provide a computational account of control reconfiguration that provides new insights into how multivariate attentional signals are optimized to achieve task goals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当面对分心时,我们可以更多地关注与目标相关的信息(目标),或者更少地关注与目的冲突的信息(分心物)。人们如何利用认知控制来将注意力分散在目标和干扰物之间,目前尚不清楚。我们通过开发一种新的参数注意力控制任务来解决这个问题,该任务可以“标记”参与者对目标和干扰信息的敏感性。我们使用这些精确的注意力测量来开发一个新的过程模型,该模型可以解释参与者如何控制对目标和分心物的注意力。在三个实验中,我们发现参与者通过独立控制他们对目标和干扰信息的处理来满足这项任务的要求,对激励和冲突的操纵表现出不同的适应能力。尽管激励优先导致目标增强,但先前试验中的冲突优先导致干扰抑制。在试验的早期,这些不同的控制驱动因素改变了对目标和干扰物的敏感性,随后迅速进行反应性重新配置,以达到任务适当的特征敏感性。为了提供这些经验发现的过程级说明,我们开发了一种新的证据积累神经网络模型,该模型具有特征权重上的吸引子动力学,可以重新配置目标和干扰物处理。这些结果提供了控制重构的计算说明,为如何优化多变量注意力信号以实现任务目标提供了新的见解。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Humans reconfigure target and distractor processing to address distinct task demands.","authors":"Harrison Ritz, Amitai Shenhav","doi":"10.1037/rev0000442","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When faced with distraction, we can focus more on goal-relevant information (targets) or focus less on goal-conflicting information (distractors). How people use cognitive control to distribute attention across targets and distractors remains unclear. We address this question by developing a novel Parametric Attentional Control Task that can \"tag\" participants' sensitivity to target and distractor information. We use these precise measures of attention to develop a novel process model that can explain how participants control attention toward targets and distractors. Across three experiments, we find that participants met the demands of this task by independently controlling their processing of target and distractor information, exhibiting distinct adaptations to manipulations of incentives and conflict. Whereas incentives preferentially led to target enhancement, conflict in the previous trial preferentially led to distractor suppression. These distinct drivers of control altered sensitivity to targets and distractors early in the trial, promptly followed by reactive reconfiguration toward task-appropriate feature sensitivity. To provide a process-level account of these empirical findings, we develop a novel neural network model of evidence accumulation with attractor dynamics over feature weights that reconfigure target and distractor processing. These results provide a computational account of control reconfiguration that provides new insights into how multivariate attentional signals are optimized to achieve task goals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"349-372"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11193598/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10152529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In search of better practice in executive functions assessment: Methodological issues and potential solutions. 寻求更好的执行功能评估方法:方法问题和潜在解决方案。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-24 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000434
Marc Yangüez, Benoit Bediou, Julien Chanal, Daphne Bavelier

The multicomponent nature of executive functions (EF) has long been recognized, pushing for a better understanding of both the commonalities and the diversity between EF components. Despite the advances made, the operationalization of performance in EF tasks remains rather heterogeneous, and the structure of EF as modeled by confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) is still a topic of debate (Karr et al., 2018). The present work demonstrates these two issues are related, showing how different operationalizations in task-based performance indicators impact the resulting models of EF structure with CFA. Using bootstrapped data from 294 children (8-12 years old) and nine EF tasks (tapping inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility), we first show improved model convergence and acceptance when operationalizing EF through single tasks' scores (e.g., incongruent trials, Flanker task) relative to difference scores (e.g., incongruent minus congruent trials, Flanker task). Furthermore, we show that response times exhibit poor model convergence and acceptance compared not only to accuracy but also drift rate. The latter, a well-known indicator in drift-diffusion models, is found to present the best trade-off between convergence and acceptance to model EF with CFA. Finally, we examine how various operationalizations of performance in EF tasks impact CFA model comparison in the assessment of EF structure and discuss the theoretical foundations for these results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们早已认识到执行功能(EF)的多成分性,这促使人们更好地理解EF成分之间的共性和多样性。尽管取得了进展,但EF任务中表现的操作化仍然相当不统一,而通过确证因子分析(CFA)建模的EF结构仍然是一个争论不休的话题(Karr等人,2018)。本研究证明了这两个问题之间的关联,展示了基于任务的绩效指标的不同操作方法如何影响CFA得出的EF结构模型。利用来自 294 名儿童(8-12 岁)的引导数据和九项 EF 任务(挖掘抑制、工作记忆和认知灵活性),我们首先展示了通过单一任务得分(如不一致试验、Flanker 任务)来操作 EF 时,相对于差异得分(如不一致试验减去一致试验、Flanker 任务),模型的收敛性和接受度有所提高。此外,我们还发现,与准确率和漂移率相比,反应时间表现出较低的模型收敛性和接受性。漂移率是漂移扩散模型中的一个著名指标,我们发现它在收敛性和接受性之间做出了最佳权衡,从而可以用 CFA 建立 EF 模型。最后,我们研究了在评估 EF 结构时,EF 任务中各种绩效操作对 CFA 模型比较的影响,并讨论了这些结果的理论基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"In search of better practice in executive functions assessment: Methodological issues and potential solutions.","authors":"Marc Yangüez, Benoit Bediou, Julien Chanal, Daphne Bavelier","doi":"10.1037/rev0000434","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000434","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The multicomponent nature of executive functions (EF) has long been recognized, pushing for a better understanding of both the commonalities and the diversity between EF components. Despite the advances made, the operationalization of performance in EF tasks remains rather heterogeneous, and the structure of EF as modeled by confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) is still a topic of debate (Karr et al., 2018). The present work demonstrates these two issues are related, showing how different operationalizations in task-based performance indicators impact the resulting models of EF structure with CFA. Using bootstrapped data from 294 children (8-12 years old) and nine EF tasks (tapping inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility), we first show improved model convergence and acceptance when operationalizing EF through single tasks' scores (e.g., incongruent trials, Flanker task) relative to difference scores (e.g., incongruent minus congruent trials, Flanker task). Furthermore, we show that response times exhibit poor model convergence and acceptance compared not only to accuracy but also drift rate. The latter, a well-known indicator in drift-diffusion models, is found to present the best trade-off between convergence and acceptance to model EF with CFA. Finally, we examine how various operationalizations of performance in EF tasks impact CFA model comparison in the assessment of EF structure and discuss the theoretical foundations for these results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"402-430"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10416084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A probabilistic successor representation for context-dependent learning. 用于上下文相关学习的概率继任表征。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000414
Jesse P Geerts, Samuel J Gershman, Neil Burgess, Kimberly L Stachenfeld

Two of the main impediments to learning complex tasks are that relationships between different stimuli, including rewards, can be uncertain and context-dependent. Reinforcement learning (RL) provides a framework for learning, by predicting total future reward directly (model-free RL), or via predictions of future states (model-based RL). Within this framework, "successor representation" (SR) predicts total future occupancy of all states. A recent theoretical proposal suggests that the hippocampus encodes the SR in order to facilitate prediction of future reward. However, this proposal does not take into account how learning should adapt under uncertainty and switches of context. Here, we introduce a theory of learning SRs using prediction errors which includes optimally balancing uncertainty in new observations versus existing knowledge. We then generalize that approach to a multicontext setting, allowing the model to learn and maintain multiple task-specific SRs and infer which one to use at any moment based on the accuracy of its predictions. Thus, the context used for predictions can be determined by both the contents of the states themselves and the distribution of transitions between them. This probabilistic SR model captures animal behavior in tasks which require contextual memory and generalization, and unifies previous SR theory with hippocampal-dependent contextual decision-making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学习复杂任务的两个主要障碍是,不同刺激(包括奖励)之间的关系可能是不确定的,并且与情境有关。强化学习(RL)通过直接预测未来总奖励(无模型强化学习)或通过预测未来状态(基于模型强化学习)为学习提供了一个框架。在这一框架内,"后继表征"(SR)预测所有状态的未来总占用率。最近的一项理论建议认为,海马编码 SR 是为了促进对未来奖励的预测。然而,这一建议并没有考虑到学习应如何适应不确定性和情境的转换。在这里,我们将介绍一种利用预测误差学习 SR 的理论,其中包括在新的观察结果与现有知识之间实现不确定性的最佳平衡。然后,我们将这种方法推广到多情境设置中,允许模型学习和维护多个特定任务的 SR,并根据预测的准确性推断在任何时刻使用哪一个。因此,预测所使用的情境可以由状态本身的内容和状态之间的转换分布来决定。这种概率SR模型捕捉到了需要情境记忆和概括的任务中的动物行为,并将以前的SR理论与依赖海马的情境决策统一起来。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A probabilistic successor representation for context-dependent learning.","authors":"Jesse P Geerts, Samuel J Gershman, Neil Burgess, Kimberly L Stachenfeld","doi":"10.1037/rev0000414","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000414","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two of the main impediments to learning complex tasks are that relationships between different stimuli, including rewards, can be uncertain and context-dependent. Reinforcement learning (RL) provides a framework for learning, by predicting total future reward directly (model-free RL), or via predictions of future states (model-based RL). Within this framework, \"successor representation\" (SR) predicts total future occupancy of all states. A recent theoretical proposal suggests that the hippocampus encodes the SR in order to facilitate prediction of future reward. However, this proposal does not take into account how learning should adapt under uncertainty and switches of context. Here, we introduce a theory of learning SRs using prediction errors which includes optimally balancing uncertainty in new observations versus existing knowledge. We then generalize that approach to a multicontext setting, allowing the model to learn and maintain multiple task-specific SRs and infer which one to use at any moment based on the accuracy of its predictions. Thus, the context used for predictions can be determined by both the contents of the states themselves and the distribution of transitions between them. This probabilistic SR model captures animal behavior in tasks which require contextual memory and generalization, and unifies previous SR theory with hippocampal-dependent contextual decision-making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"578-597"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9796965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The autocorrelated Bayesian sampler: A rational process for probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choices, confidence judgments, and response times. 自相关贝叶斯采样器:概率判断、估计、置信区间、选择、置信判断和响应时间的合理过程。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000427
Jian-Qiao Zhu, Joakim Sundh, Jake Spicer, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn

Normative models of decision-making that optimally transform noisy (sensory) information into categorical decisions qualitatively mismatch human behavior. Indeed, leading computational models have only achieved high empirical corroboration by adding task-specific assumptions that deviate from normative principles. In response, we offer a Bayesian approach that implicitly produces a posterior distribution of possible answers (hypotheses) in response to sensory information. But we assume that the brain has no direct access to this posterior, but can only sample hypotheses according to their posterior probabilities. Accordingly, we argue that the primary problem of normative concern in decision-making is integrating stochastic hypotheses, rather than stochastic sensory information, to make categorical decisions. This implies that human response variability arises mainly from posterior sampling rather than sensory noise. Because human hypothesis generation is serially correlated, hypothesis samples will be autocorrelated. Guided by this new problem formulation, we develop a new process, the Autocorrelated Bayesian Sampler (ABS), which grounds autocorrelated hypothesis generation in a sophisticated sampling algorithm. The ABS provides a single mechanism that qualitatively explains many empirical effects of probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choice, confidence judgments, response times, and their relationships. Our analysis demonstrates the unifying power of a perspective shift in the exploration of normative models. It also exemplifies the proposal that the "Bayesian brain" operates using samples not probabilities, and that variability in human behavior may primarily reflect computational rather than sensory noise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

将嘈杂(感官)信息最优化地转化为分类决策的规范决策模型与人类行为在本质上并不匹配。事实上,领先的计算模型只有通过添加偏离规范原则的特定任务假设,才能获得较高的经验佐证。作为回应,我们提供了一种贝叶斯方法,这种方法会根据感官信息隐含地生成可能答案(假设)的后验分布。但我们假定,大脑无法直接获取这种后验分布,而只能根据后验概率对假设进行抽样。因此,我们认为,决策中规范性关注的首要问题是整合随机假设,而不是随机感官信息,以做出分类决策。这意味着人类反应的变异性主要来自于后验取样,而非感觉噪声。由于人类假设的产生具有序列相关性,因此假设样本将具有自相关性。在这一新问题表述的指导下,我们开发了一种新的程序--自相关贝叶斯采样器(ABS),它将自相关假设的生成置于复杂的采样算法中。自相关贝叶斯取样器提供了一种单一的机制,可以定性地解释概率判断、估计、置信区间、选择、置信判断、响应时间及其关系的许多经验效应。我们的分析展示了在探索规范模型时视角转换的统一力量。它还例证了 "贝叶斯大脑 "使用样本而非概率进行运作的提议,以及人类行为的可变性可能主要反映了计算噪音而非感官噪音。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The autocorrelated Bayesian sampler: A rational process for probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choices, confidence judgments, and response times.","authors":"Jian-Qiao Zhu, Joakim Sundh, Jake Spicer, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn","doi":"10.1037/rev0000427","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000427","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Normative models of decision-making that optimally transform noisy (sensory) information into categorical decisions qualitatively mismatch human behavior. Indeed, leading computational models have only achieved high empirical corroboration by adding task-specific assumptions that deviate from normative principles. In response, we offer a Bayesian approach that implicitly produces a posterior distribution of possible answers (hypotheses) in response to sensory information. But we assume that the brain has no direct access to this posterior, but can only <i>sample</i> hypotheses according to their posterior probabilities. Accordingly, we argue that the primary problem of normative concern in decision-making is integrating stochastic <i>hypotheses</i>, rather than stochastic sensory information, to make categorical decisions. This implies that human response variability arises mainly from posterior sampling rather than sensory noise. Because human hypothesis generation is serially correlated, hypothesis samples will be autocorrelated. Guided by this new problem formulation, we develop a new process, the Autocorrelated Bayesian Sampler (ABS), which grounds autocorrelated hypothesis generation in a sophisticated sampling algorithm. The ABS provides a single mechanism that qualitatively explains many empirical effects of probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choice, confidence judgments, response times, and their relationships. Our analysis demonstrates the unifying power of a perspective shift in the exploration of normative models. It also exemplifies the proposal that the \"Bayesian brain\" operates using samples not probabilities, and that variability in human behavior may primarily reflect computational rather than sensory noise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"456-493"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11115360/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9586788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Updating, evidence evaluation, and operator availability: A theoretical framework for understanding belief. 更新、证据评估和操作者可用性:理解信念的理论框架。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000444
Joseph Sommer, Julien Musolino, Pernille Hemmer

Decades of findings in psychology suggest that human belief is thoroughly irrational. At best, beliefs might be formed by heuristic processes that predictably lead to suboptimal outcomes. At worst, they are slaves to motivated reasoning, which allows people to come to whichever conclusions they prefer. In this article, we suggest that belief updating, narrowly construed, may be a rational process that is uniquely sensitive to evidence and cognitively impenetrable to desires or incentives. Before any updating can occur, however, a series of processes mediate between information in the world and subjectively compelling evidence. We distinguish between updating proper and processes of evidence search, acceptance, hypothesis specification, integration of relevant information, and reasoning. We review research highlighting the computational difficulty inherent to each of these problems and conclude that solutions must be heuristic and fallible. Beyond incidental failures, evidence evaluation processes-unlike updating-are penetrable to motivation and as such, may be biased by people's desires and goals. In light of this distinction, we propose a theoretical framework for integrating research on belief which divides the cognitive processes involved in belief into two distinct levels. At Level 1, updating is suggested to be approximately Bayesian and impenetrable to desires and goals. In contrast, Level 2 processes, which search for and evaluate evidence, are cognitively penetrable. In addition, we emphasize that Level 2 processes are necessarily heuristic and exhibit bounded rationality (Simon, 1956) given the difficulty of the problems they have to solve. Finally, we specify an additional set of relatively invariant characteristics, which influence how Level 2 processes are employed by making different methods of information processing available. Our framework offers a more nuanced understanding of belief, permits a granular localization of irrationality, and may help reconcile extant debates in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

心理学数十年的研究结果表明,人类的信念是完全非理性的。在最好的情况下,信念可能是由启发式过程形成的,可预见地导致次优结果。在最坏的情况下,它们是动机推理的奴隶,动机推理允许人们得出自己喜欢的结论。在本文中,我们提出,狭义上的信念更新可能是一种理性过程,它对证据具有独特的敏感性,并且在认知上不受欲望或动机的影响。然而,在任何更新发生之前,世界上的信息和主观上令人信服的证据之间需要一系列的过程进行调解。我们将更新本身与证据搜索、接受、假设规范、整合相关信息和推理等过程区分开来。我们回顾了强调这些问题中每个问题内在计算难度的研究,并得出结论:解决方案必须是启发式和易错的。除了偶然的失误之外,证据评估过程与更新不同,可以渗透到动机中,因此可能会受到人们的愿望和目标的影响。根据这一区别,我们提出了一个整合信念研究的理论框架,将信念所涉及的认知过程分为两个不同的层次。在第一层次,更新被认为是近似贝叶斯的,不受欲望和目标的影响。与此相反,第二层次的过程,即搜索和评估证据的过程,在认知上是可以穿透的。此外,我们还强调,第二级过程必然是启发式的,鉴于其所要解决的问题的难度,第二级过程表现出有限理性(Simon,1956 年)。最后,我们还明确了一组相对不变的特征,这些特征通过提供不同的信息处理方法来影响第二级过程的使用方式。我们的框架提供了对信念更细致入微的理解,允许对非理性进行细化定位,并可能有助于调和文献中的现有争论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Updating, evidence evaluation, and operator availability: A theoretical framework for understanding belief.","authors":"Joseph Sommer, Julien Musolino, Pernille Hemmer","doi":"10.1037/rev0000444","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Decades of findings in psychology suggest that human belief is thoroughly irrational. At best, beliefs might be formed by heuristic processes that predictably lead to suboptimal outcomes. At worst, they are slaves to motivated reasoning, which allows people to come to whichever conclusions they prefer. In this article, we suggest that belief updating, narrowly construed, may be a rational process that is uniquely sensitive to evidence and cognitively impenetrable to desires or incentives. Before any updating can occur, however, a series of processes mediate between information in the world and subjectively compelling evidence. We distinguish between updating proper and processes of evidence search, acceptance, hypothesis specification, integration of relevant information, and reasoning. We review research highlighting the computational difficulty inherent to each of these problems and conclude that solutions must be heuristic and fallible. Beyond incidental failures, evidence evaluation processes-unlike updating-are penetrable to motivation and as such, may be biased by people's desires and goals. In light of this distinction, we propose a theoretical framework for integrating research on belief which divides the cognitive processes involved in belief into two distinct levels. At Level 1, updating is suggested to be approximately Bayesian and impenetrable to desires and goals. In contrast, Level 2 processes, which search for and evaluate evidence, are cognitively penetrable. In addition, we emphasize that Level 2 processes are necessarily heuristic and exhibit bounded rationality (Simon, 1956) given the difficulty of the problems they have to solve. Finally, we specify an additional set of relatively invariant characteristics, which influence how Level 2 processes are employed by making different methods of information processing available. Our framework offers a more nuanced understanding of belief, permits a granular localization of irrationality, and may help reconcile extant debates in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"373-401"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10005538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
PONG: A computational model of visual word recognition through bihemispheric activation. PONG:通过双半球激活进行视觉单词识别的计算模型。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000461
Joshua Snell

Orthographic processing is an open problem. Decades of visual word recognition research have fueled the development of various theoretical frameworks. Although these frameworks have had good explanatory power, various recent results cannot be satisfactorily captured in any model. In order to account for old and new phenomena alike, here I present a new theory of how the brain computes letter positions. According to PONG (which describes the Positional Ordering of N-Grams), each hemisphere of the brain comprises a set of mono- and multigram detectors. The crux is that the detectors for a given N-gram are activated to different extents in their respective hemispheres, depending on where in the visual field the N-gram is located. This differential activity allows the brain to estimate the leftness or rightness of that N-gram, whereby word activation is a function of the N-gram's identity plus its laterality relative to that of other activated N-grams. Simulations with PONG suggest that the framework effectively accounts for classic phenomena, as well as newer phenomena and cross-linguistic differences that cannot be explained by other models. I also reflect on the neurophysiological plausibility of the model and avenues for future inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

正字法处理是一个尚未解决的问题。数十年的视觉单词识别研究推动了各种理论框架的发展。尽管这些框架具有很好的解释力,但最近的各种结果却无法在任何模型中得到令人满意的体现。为了解释新旧现象,我在此提出了大脑如何计算字母位置的新理论。根据 PONG(描述 N 字符的位置排序)理论,大脑的每个半球都由一组单字符和多字符检测器组成。问题的关键在于,特定 N 字符的检测器在各自半球的激活程度不同,这取决于 N 字符在视野中的位置。这种不同的活动使大脑能够估算出该 N-gram的左度或右度,因此单词激活是该 N-gram的特性加上其相对于其他激活 N-gram的侧向性的函数。用 PONG 进行的模拟表明,该框架能有效解释经典现象以及其他模型无法解释的新现象和跨语言差异。我还对该模型的神经生理学合理性和未来的研究方向进行了思考。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"PONG: A computational model of visual word recognition through bihemispheric activation.","authors":"Joshua Snell","doi":"10.1037/rev0000461","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000461","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Orthographic processing is an open problem. Decades of visual word recognition research have fueled the development of various theoretical frameworks. Although these frameworks have had good explanatory power, various recent results cannot be satisfactorily captured in any model. In order to account for old and new phenomena alike, here I present a new theory of how the brain computes letter positions. According to <i>PONG</i> (which describes the <i>Positional Ordering of N-Grams</i>), each hemisphere of the brain comprises a set of mono- and multigram detectors. The crux is that the detectors for a given N-gram are activated to different extents in their respective hemispheres, depending on where in the visual field the N-gram is located. This differential activity allows the brain to estimate the leftness or rightness of that N-gram, whereby word activation is a function of the N-gram's identity plus its laterality relative to that of other activated N-grams. Simulations with PONG suggest that the framework effectively accounts for classic phenomena, as well as newer phenomena and cross-linguistic differences that cannot be explained by other models. I also reflect on the neurophysiological plausibility of the model and avenues for future inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139973304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How getting in sync is curative: Insights gained from research in psychotherapy. 同步是如何治疗的?从心理治疗研究中获得的启示。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000471
Sigal Zilcha-Mano

We are all constantly going in and out of sync with the people we meet in our lives: significant others, incidental encounters, and strangers. Synchrony is a ubiquitous phenomenon, considered an evolution-based mechanism of survival. In recent years, technological development has made it possible to collect much data on synchrony across disciplines. The collected data show great potential to shed light on the benefits of this universal phenomenon. At the same time, mixed results emerged, stressing the need for a theory to navigate research inquiries and discoveries. It is proposed here that synchrony serves as an individual-specific mechanism for making relationships curative in all life circumstances, especially therapeutic ones-hence its special relevance for psychotherapy. A synthesis of the majority of the literature across disciplines reveals two implicit assumptions about synchrony, resulting in two separate bodies of knowledge: (a) synchrony is a trait-like signature characterizing individuals; and (b) synchrony is a state-like phenomenon that can be manipulated in the lab. It is proposed here to personalize synchrony research by integrating the two assumptions into a comprehensive theory according to which individuals have a trait-like signature for getting in sync, which determines their physical and mental health, and that this deterministic reality can be subject to state-like manipulation. Individuals can deviate from their trait-like signature. When the deviation is toward normative activation, mental health improves, and the state-like changes are defined as therapeutic. This article calls for research to investigate how trait-like signature of synchrony develops and how it can be therapeutically changed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们经常与生活中遇到的人同步或不同步,包括重要的人、偶遇的人和陌生人。同步是一种无处不在的现象,被认为是一种基于进化的生存机制。近年来,技术的发展使得收集大量跨学科的同步数据成为可能。收集到的数据显示出巨大的潜力,可以揭示这一普遍现象的益处。与此同时,也出现了好坏参半的结果,这就强调了需要一种理论来指导研究调查和发现。我们在此提出,同步性是一种针对个体的机制,它能使人际关系在所有生活环境中,尤其是治疗环境中发挥治疗作用,因此它与心理治疗特别相关。综合各学科的大部分文献,我们发现了关于同步性的两个隐含假设,并由此形成了两个独立的知识体系:(a)同步性是个人的特质特征;(b)同步性是一种可在实验室中操控的状态现象。本文建议将这两个假设整合为一个全面的理论,从而实现同步研究的个性化。根据该理论,个人具有获得同步的类特质特征,这决定了他们的身心健康,而且这种决定性的现实可以受到类状态的操纵。个体可以偏离其特质特征。当这种偏离趋于正常激活时,心理健康就会得到改善,而这种类似状态的变化被定义为治疗性的。这篇文章呼吁开展研究,调查同步性的特质特征是如何形成的,以及如何通过治疗改变这种特征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"How getting in sync is curative: Insights gained from research in psychotherapy.","authors":"Sigal Zilcha-Mano","doi":"10.1037/rev0000471","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000471","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We are all constantly going in and out of sync with the people we meet in our lives: significant others, incidental encounters, and strangers. Synchrony is a ubiquitous phenomenon, considered an evolution-based mechanism of survival. In recent years, technological development has made it possible to collect much data on synchrony across disciplines. The collected data show great potential to shed light on the benefits of this universal phenomenon. At the same time, mixed results emerged, stressing the need for a theory to navigate research inquiries and discoveries. It is proposed here that synchrony serves as an individual-specific mechanism for making relationships curative in all life circumstances, especially therapeutic ones-hence its special relevance for psychotherapy. A synthesis of the majority of the literature across disciplines reveals two implicit assumptions about synchrony, resulting in two separate bodies of knowledge: (a) synchrony is a trait-like signature characterizing individuals; and (b) synchrony is a state-like phenomenon that can be manipulated in the lab. It is proposed here to personalize synchrony research by integrating the two assumptions into a comprehensive theory according to which individuals have a trait-like signature for getting in sync, which determines their physical and mental health, and that this deterministic reality can be subject to state-like manipulation. Individuals can deviate from their trait-like signature. When the deviation is toward normative activation, mental health improves, and the state-like changes are defined as therapeutic. This article calls for research to investigate how trait-like signature of synchrony develops and how it can be therapeutically changed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The controllosphere: The neural origin of cognitive effort. 控制圈:认知努力的神经起源
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000467
Clay B Holroyd

Why do some mental activities feel harder than others? The answer to this question is surprisingly controversial. Current theories propose that cognitive effort affords a computational benefit, such as instigating a switch from an activity with low reward value to a different activity with higher reward value. By contrast, in this article, I relate cognitive effort to the fact that brain neuroanatomy and neurophysiology render some neural states more energy-efficient than others. I introduce the concept of the "controllosphere," an energy-inefficient region of neural state space associated with high control, which surrounds the better known "intrinsic manifold," an energy-efficient subspace associated with low control. Integration of control-theoretic principles with classic neurocomputational models of cognitive control suggests that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) implements a controller that can drive the system state into the controllosphere, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) implements an observer that monitors changes of state of the controlled system, and cognitive effort reflects a mismatch between DLPFC and ACC energies for control and observation. On this account, cognitive effort scales with the energetic demands of the DLPFC control signal, especially when the consequences of the control are unobservable by ACC. Further, I propose that neural transitions through the controllosphere lead to a buildup of neural waste. Cognitive effort therefore prevents against neural damage by discouraging extended periods of high control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

为什么有些智力活动比其他智力活动更难?对于这个问题的答案,争议之大令人吃惊。目前的理论认为,认知努力会带来计算上的好处,比如促使人们从奖励价值低的活动转向奖励价值高的其他活动。相比之下,在本文中,我将认知努力与大脑神经解剖学和神经生理学使某些神经状态比其他神经状态更节能这一事实联系起来。我提出了 "控制圈"(controllosphere)的概念,这是神经状态空间中与高控制相关的低能效区域,它围绕着众所周知的 "内在流形"(insrinsic manifold),这是一个与低控制相关的高能效子空间。将控制理论原理与认知控制的经典神经计算模型相结合,可以发现背外侧前额叶皮层(DLPFC)实现了一个控制器,可以将系统状态驱动到控制区域;前扣带回皮层(ACC)实现了一个观察器,可以监测受控系统的状态变化;认知努力反映了背外侧前额叶皮层和前扣带回皮层在控制和观察方面的能量不匹配。因此,认知努力与 DLPFC 控制信号的能量需求成比例,尤其是当控制的后果无法被 ACC 观察到时。此外,我还提出,通过控制圈的神经转换会导致神经废物的积累。因此,认知努力通过抑制长时间的高度控制来防止神经损伤。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The controllosphere: The neural origin of cognitive effort.","authors":"Clay B Holroyd","doi":"10.1037/rev0000467","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000467","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Why do some mental activities feel harder than others? The answer to this question is surprisingly controversial. Current theories propose that cognitive effort affords a computational benefit, such as instigating a switch from an activity with low reward value to a different activity with higher reward value. By contrast, in this article, I relate cognitive effort to the fact that brain neuroanatomy and neurophysiology render some neural states more energy-efficient than others. I introduce the concept of the \"controllosphere,\" an energy-inefficient region of neural state space associated with high control, which surrounds the better known \"intrinsic manifold,\" an energy-efficient subspace associated with low control. Integration of control-theoretic principles with classic neurocomputational models of cognitive control suggests that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) implements a <i>controller</i> that can drive the system state into the controllosphere, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) implements an <i>observer</i> that monitors changes of state of the controlled system, and cognitive effort reflects a mismatch between DLPFC and ACC energies for control and observation. On this account, cognitive effort scales with the energetic demands of the DLPFC control signal, especially when the consequences of the control are unobservable by ACC. Further, I propose that neural transitions through the controllosphere lead to a buildup of neural waste. Cognitive effort therefore prevents against neural damage by discouraging extended periods of high control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Levels of analysis and explanatory progress in psychology: Integrating frameworks from biology and cognitive science for a more comprehensive science of the mind. 心理学的分析层次和解释进展:整合生物学和认知科学的框架,建立更全面的心理科学。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-22 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000459
Laith Al-Shawaf

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in Psychological Review on Mar 14 2024 (see record 2024-63968-001). Incorrect italic formatting was removed throughout the article, and an unnecessary paragraph of text was removed from the "Levels of Analysis and the Branches of Psychology: What Is Needed for a Complete Explanation of a Behavior or Cognitive System?" section. These were editorial production errors. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Levels of analysis are crucial to the progress of science. They frame the epistemological boundaries of a discipline, chart its explanatory goals, help scientists to avoid needless conflict, and highlight knowledge gaps. Two frameworks in particular, Tinbergen's four questions from biology and Marr's three levels from cognitive science, hold immense potential for psychology. This article proposes ways to integrate the two frameworks and suggests that doing so helps resolve key confusions and unnecessary conflicts in psychology. Integrating these two frameworks clarifies what "mechanism" really means, sheds light on how to test evolutionary hypotheses in psychology, and specifies what is required for a comprehensive explanation of a behavior or cognitive system. Adopting and integrating these two theoretical frameworks has the capacity to spur progress in psychology and to clarify what is needed for a comprehensive science of the mind. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

分析层次对科学进步至关重要。它们框定了一门学科的认识论界限,勾勒出其解释目标,帮助科学家避免不必要的冲突,并突出知识差距。尤其是两个框架,即生物学中廷伯根的四个问题和认知科学中马尔的三个层次,为心理学带来了巨大的潜力。本文提出了整合这两个框架的方法,并认为这样做有助于解决心理学中的主要困惑和不必要的冲突。整合这两个框架可以澄清 "机制 "的真正含义,阐明如何检验心理学中的进化假设,并明确全面解释行为或认知系统所需的条件。采用和整合这两个理论框架能够推动心理学的进步,并明确全面的心理科学需要什么。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Levels of analysis and explanatory progress in psychology: Integrating frameworks from biology and cognitive science for a more comprehensive science of the mind.","authors":"Laith Al-Shawaf","doi":"10.1037/rev0000459","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000459","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in <i>Psychological Review</i> on Mar 14 2024 (see record 2024-63968-001). Incorrect italic formatting was removed throughout the article, and an unnecessary paragraph of text was removed from the \"Levels of Analysis and the Branches of Psychology: What Is Needed for a Complete Explanation of a Behavior or Cognitive System?\" section. These were editorial production errors. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Levels of analysis are crucial to the progress of science. They frame the epistemological boundaries of a discipline, chart its explanatory goals, help scientists to avoid needless conflict, and highlight knowledge gaps. Two frameworks in particular, <i>Tinbergen's four questions</i> from biology and <i>Marr's three levels</i> from cognitive science, hold immense potential for psychology. This article proposes ways to integrate the two frameworks and suggests that doing so helps resolve key confusions and unnecessary conflicts in psychology. Integrating these two frameworks clarifies what \"mechanism\" really means, sheds light on how to test evolutionary hypotheses in psychology, and specifies what is required for a comprehensive explanation of a behavior or cognitive system. Adopting and integrating these two theoretical frameworks has the capacity to spur progress in psychology and to clarify what is needed for a comprehensive science of the mind. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139513556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Empirical evidence for perspectival similarity. 视角相似性的经验证据。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000403
Jorge Morales, Chaz Firestone

When a circular coin is rotated in depth, is there any sense in which it comes to resemble an ellipse? While this question is at the center of a rich and divided philosophical tradition (with some scholars answering affirmatively and some negatively), Morales et al. (2020, 2021) took an empirical approach, reporting 10 experiments whose results favor such perspectival similarity. Recently, Burge and Burge (2022) offered a vigorous critique of this work, objecting to its approach and conclusions on both philosophical and empirical grounds. Here, we answer these objections on both fronts. We show that Burge and Burge's critique rests on misunderstandings of Morales et al.'s claims; of the relation between the data and conclusions; and of the philosophical context in which the work appears. Specifically, Burge and Burge attribute to us a much stronger (and stranger) view than we hold, involving the introduction of "a new entity" located "in some intermediate position(s) between the distal shape and the retinal image." We do not hold this view. Indeed, once properly understood, most of Burge and Burge's objections favor Morales et al.'s claims rather than oppose them. Finally, we discuss several questions that remain unanswered, and reflect on a productive path forward on these issues of foundational scientific and philosophical interest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当一枚圆形硬币在深度方向上旋转时,它在任何意义上都会像一个椭圆吗?这个问题在哲学传统中有着丰富而又分歧的内涵(有些学者给出了肯定的答案,有些学者则给出了否定的答案),而莫拉莱斯等人(2020,2021)则采用了实证的方法,报告了 10 项实验的结果,这些实验都支持这种视角上的相似性。最近,Burge 和 Burge(2022 年)对这项工作提出了强烈的批评,从哲学和经验两方面反对其方法和结论。在此,我们将从两个方面回答这些反对意见。我们表明,Burge 和 Burge 的批评是基于对莫拉莱斯等人的主张、数据与结论之间的关系以及该著作的哲学背景的误解。具体来说,Burge 和 Burge 将一种比我们更强烈(也更奇怪)的观点归咎于我们,认为我们引入了 "一个新的实体",它位于 "远端形状和视网膜图像之间的某个中间位置"。我们不持这种观点。事实上,一旦正确理解,伯格和伯格的大部分反对意见都有利于而不是反对莫拉莱斯等人的主张。最后,我们讨论了几个仍未得到解答的问题,并思考了在这些具有基础科学和哲学意义的问题上向前迈进的有效途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Empirical evidence for perspectival similarity.","authors":"Jorge Morales, Chaz Firestone","doi":"10.1037/rev0000403","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000403","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When a circular coin is rotated in depth, is there any sense in which it comes to resemble an ellipse? While this question is at the center of a rich and divided philosophical tradition (with some scholars answering affirmatively and some negatively), Morales et al. (2020, 2021) took an empirical approach, reporting 10 experiments whose results favor such perspectival similarity. Recently, Burge and Burge (2022) offered a vigorous critique of this work, objecting to its approach and conclusions on both philosophical and empirical grounds. Here, we answer these objections on both fronts. We show that Burge and Burge's critique rests on misunderstandings of Morales et al.'s claims; of the relation between the data and conclusions; and of the philosophical context in which the work appears. Specifically, Burge and Burge attribute to us a much stronger (and stranger) view than we hold, involving the introduction of \"a new entity\" located \"in some intermediate position(s) between the distal shape and the retinal image.\" We do not hold this view. Indeed, once properly understood, most of Burge and Burge's objections favor Morales et al.'s claims rather than oppose them. Finally, we discuss several questions that remain unanswered, and reflect on a productive path forward on these issues of foundational scientific and philosophical interest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"311-320"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9100355","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1