首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
Bouncing back from life's perturbations: Formalizing psychological resilience from a complex systems perspective. 从生活的干扰中反弹:从复杂系统的角度将心理复原力正规化。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000497
Gabriela Lunansky, George A Bonanno, Tessa F Blanken, Claudia D van Borkulo, Angélique O J Cramer, Denny Borsboom

Experiencing stressful or traumatic events can lead to a range of responses, from mild disruptions to severe and persistent mental health issues. Understanding the various trajectories of response to adversity is crucial for developing effective interventions and support systems. Researchers have identified four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity, from which the resilient is the most common one. Resilience refers to the maintenance of healthy psychological functioning despite facing adversity. However, it remains an open question how to understand and anticipate resilience, due to its dynamic and multifactorial nature. This article presents a novel formalized framework to conceptualize resilience from a complex systems perspective. We use the network theory of psychopathology, which states that mental disorders are self-sustaining endpoints of direct symptom-symptom interactions organized in a network system. The internal structure of the network determines the most likely trajectory of symptom development. We introduce the resilience quadrant, which organizes the state of symptom networks on two domains: (1) healthy versus dysfunctional and (2) stable versus unstable. The quadrant captures the four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity along those dimensions: resilient trajectories in the face of adversity, as well as persistent symptoms despite treatment interventions. Subsequently, an empirical illustration, by means of a proof-of-principle, shows how simulated observations from four different network architectures lead to the four commonly observed responses to adversity. As such, we present a novel outlook on resilience by combining existing statistical symptom network models with simulation techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

经历压力或创伤事件会导致一系列反应,从轻微的干扰到严重和持续的心理健康问题。了解对逆境的各种反应轨迹对于制定有效的干预措施和支持系统至关重要。研究人员发现了四种常见的逆境反应轨迹,其中复原力是最常见的一种。复原力是指在面临逆境时仍能保持健康的心理功能。然而,由于抗逆力的动态性和多因素性,如何理解和预测抗逆力仍是一个悬而未决的问题。本文提出了一个新颖的形式化框架,从复杂系统的角度对复原力进行概念化。我们采用精神病理学的网络理论,该理论认为精神障碍是症状与症状之间直接相互作用的自我维持终点,并在网络系统中组织起来。网络的内部结构决定了症状最可能的发展轨迹。我们引入了 "恢复力象限"(resilience quadrant),它将症状网络的状态分为两个领域:(1) 健康与功能障碍;(2) 稳定与不稳定。该象限从这些维度捕捉了四种常见的逆境反应轨迹:面对逆境时的恢复力轨迹,以及在治疗干预后仍持续存在的症状。随后,我们通过原理验证进行了实证说明,展示了从四种不同的网络架构中模拟观察到的结果如何导致四种常见的逆境反应。因此,我们通过将现有的统计症状网络模型与模拟技术相结合,为复原力提出了一个新的视角。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Bouncing back from life's perturbations: Formalizing psychological resilience from a complex systems perspective.","authors":"Gabriela Lunansky, George A Bonanno, Tessa F Blanken, Claudia D van Borkulo, Angélique O J Cramer, Denny Borsboom","doi":"10.1037/rev0000497","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000497","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Experiencing stressful or traumatic events can lead to a range of responses, from mild disruptions to severe and persistent mental health issues. Understanding the various trajectories of response to adversity is crucial for developing effective interventions and support systems. Researchers have identified four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity, from which the resilient is the most common one. Resilience refers to the maintenance of healthy psychological functioning despite facing adversity. However, it remains an open question how to understand and anticipate resilience, due to its dynamic and multifactorial nature. This article presents a novel formalized framework to conceptualize resilience from a complex systems perspective. We use the network theory of psychopathology, which states that mental disorders are self-sustaining endpoints of direct symptom-symptom interactions organized in a network system. The internal structure of the network determines the most likely trajectory of symptom development. We introduce the resilience quadrant, which organizes the state of symptom networks on two domains: (1) healthy versus dysfunctional and (2) stable versus unstable. The quadrant captures the four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity along those dimensions: resilient trajectories in the face of adversity, as well as persistent symptoms despite treatment interventions. Subsequently, an empirical illustration, by means of a proof-of-principle, shows how simulated observations from four different network architectures lead to the four commonly observed responses to adversity. As such, we present a novel outlook on resilience by combining existing statistical symptom network models with simulation techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142473481","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bouncing back from life's perturbations: Formalizing psychological resilience from a complex systems perspective. 从生活的干扰中反弹:从复杂系统的角度将心理复原力正规化。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000497
Gabriela Lunansky,George A Bonanno,Tessa F Blanken,Claudia D van Borkulo,Angélique O J Cramer,Denny Borsboom
Experiencing stressful or traumatic events can lead to a range of responses, from mild disruptions to severe and persistent mental health issues. Understanding the various trajectories of response to adversity is crucial for developing effective interventions and support systems. Researchers have identified four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity, from which the resilient is the most common one. Resilience refers to the maintenance of healthy psychological functioning despite facing adversity. However, it remains an open question how to understand and anticipate resilience, due to its dynamic and multifactorial nature. This article presents a novel formalized framework to conceptualize resilience from a complex systems perspective. We use the network theory of psychopathology, which states that mental disorders are self-sustaining endpoints of direct symptom-symptom interactions organized in a network system. The internal structure of the network determines the most likely trajectory of symptom development. We introduce the resilience quadrant, which organizes the state of symptom networks on two domains: (1) healthy versus dysfunctional and (2) stable versus unstable. The quadrant captures the four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity along those dimensions: resilient trajectories in the face of adversity, as well as persistent symptoms despite treatment interventions. Subsequently, an empirical illustration, by means of a proof-of-principle, shows how simulated observations from four different network architectures lead to the four commonly observed responses to adversity. As such, we present a novel outlook on resilience by combining existing statistical symptom network models with simulation techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
经历压力或创伤事件会导致一系列反应,从轻微的干扰到严重和持续的心理健康问题。了解对逆境的各种反应轨迹对于制定有效的干预措施和支持系统至关重要。研究人员发现了四种常见的逆境反应轨迹,其中复原力是最常见的一种。复原力是指在面临逆境时仍能保持健康的心理功能。然而,由于抗逆力的动态性和多因素性,如何理解和预测抗逆力仍是一个悬而未决的问题。本文提出了一个新颖的形式化框架,从复杂系统的角度对复原力进行概念化。我们采用精神病理学的网络理论,该理论认为精神障碍是症状与症状之间直接相互作用的自我维持终点,并在网络系统中组织起来。网络的内部结构决定了症状最可能的发展轨迹。我们引入了 "恢复力象限"(resilience quadrant),它将症状网络的状态分为两个领域:(1) 健康与功能障碍;(2) 稳定与不稳定。该象限从这些维度捕捉了四种常见的逆境反应轨迹:面对逆境时的恢复力轨迹,以及在治疗干预后仍持续存在的症状。随后,我们通过原理验证进行了实证说明,展示了从四种不同的网络架构中模拟观察到的结果如何导致四种常见的逆境反应。因此,我们通过将现有的统计症状网络模型与模拟技术相结合,为复原力提出了一个新的视角。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Bouncing back from life's perturbations: Formalizing psychological resilience from a complex systems perspective.","authors":"Gabriela Lunansky,George A Bonanno,Tessa F Blanken,Claudia D van Borkulo,Angélique O J Cramer,Denny Borsboom","doi":"10.1037/rev0000497","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000497","url":null,"abstract":"Experiencing stressful or traumatic events can lead to a range of responses, from mild disruptions to severe and persistent mental health issues. Understanding the various trajectories of response to adversity is crucial for developing effective interventions and support systems. Researchers have identified four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity, from which the resilient is the most common one. Resilience refers to the maintenance of healthy psychological functioning despite facing adversity. However, it remains an open question how to understand and anticipate resilience, due to its dynamic and multifactorial nature. This article presents a novel formalized framework to conceptualize resilience from a complex systems perspective. We use the network theory of psychopathology, which states that mental disorders are self-sustaining endpoints of direct symptom-symptom interactions organized in a network system. The internal structure of the network determines the most likely trajectory of symptom development. We introduce the resilience quadrant, which organizes the state of symptom networks on two domains: (1) healthy versus dysfunctional and (2) stable versus unstable. The quadrant captures the four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity along those dimensions: resilient trajectories in the face of adversity, as well as persistent symptoms despite treatment interventions. Subsequently, an empirical illustration, by means of a proof-of-principle, shows how simulated observations from four different network architectures lead to the four commonly observed responses to adversity. As such, we present a novel outlook on resilience by combining existing statistical symptom network models with simulation techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":"66 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142486347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prejudice model 1.0: A predictive model of prejudice. 偏见模型 1.0:偏见预测模型。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000470
Eric Hehman, Rebecca Neel

The present research develops a predictive model of prejudice. For nearly a century, psychology and other fields have sought to scientifically understand and describe the causes of prejudice. Numerous theories of prejudice now exist. Yet these theories are overwhelmingly defined verbally and thus lack the ability to precisely predict when and to what extent prejudice will emerge. The abundance of theory also raises the possibility of undetected overlap between constructs theorized to cause prejudice. Predictive models enable falsification and provide a way for the field to move forward. To this end, here we present 18 studies with ∼5,000 participants in seven phases of model development. After initially identifying major theorized causes of prejudice in the literature, we used a model selection approach to winnow constructs into a parsimonious predictive model of prejudice (Phases I and II). We confirm this model in a preregistered out-of-sample test (Phase III), test variations in operationalizations and boundary conditions (Phases IV and V), and test generalizability on a U.S. representative sample, an Indian sample, and a U.K. sample (Phase VI). Finally, we consulted the predictions of experts in the field to examine how well they align with our results (Phase VII). We believe this initial predictive model is limited and bad, but by developing a model that makes highly specific predictions, drawing on the state of the art, we hope to provide a foundation from which research can build to improve science of prejudice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究建立了一个偏见预测模型。近一个世纪以来,心理学和其他领域一直试图科学地理解和描述偏见的成因。现在已经有了许多关于偏见的理论。然而,这些理论绝大多数都是口头定义的,因此缺乏精确预测偏见何时以及在何种程度上会出现的能力。大量的理论还可能导致未被发现的、被认为会导致偏见的结构之间的重叠。预测模型可以帮助人们进行证伪,并为这一领域的研究提供了前进的方向。为此,我们在此介绍了模型开发七个阶段的18项研究,参与者达5000人。在初步确定了文献中偏见的主要理论成因后,我们使用模型选择法将各种建构物筛选成一个简明的偏见预测模型(第一和第二阶段)。我们在预先登记的样本外测试中确认了这一模型(第三阶段),测试了操作方法和边界条件的变化(第四和第五阶段),并在美国代表性样本、印度样本和英国样本中测试了可推广性(第六阶段)。最后,我们咨询了该领域专家的预测,以检验他们与我们的结果的一致性(第七阶段)。我们相信,这个初始预测模型是有限的,也是糟糕的,但是通过开发一个模型,在借鉴现有技术的基础上做出高度具体的预测,我们希望能够为研究提供一个基础,在此基础上改进偏见科学。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Prejudice model 1.0: A predictive model of prejudice.","authors":"Eric Hehman, Rebecca Neel","doi":"10.1037/rev0000470","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present research develops a predictive model of prejudice. For nearly a century, psychology and other fields have sought to scientifically understand and describe the causes of prejudice. Numerous theories of prejudice now exist. Yet these theories are overwhelmingly defined verbally and thus lack the ability to precisely predict when and to what extent prejudice will emerge. The abundance of theory also raises the possibility of undetected overlap between constructs theorized to cause prejudice. Predictive models enable falsification and provide a way for the field to move forward. To this end, here we present 18 studies with ∼5,000 participants in seven phases of model development. After initially identifying major theorized causes of prejudice in the literature, we used a model selection approach to winnow constructs into a parsimonious predictive model of prejudice (Phases I and II). We confirm this model in a preregistered out-of-sample test (Phase III), test variations in operationalizations and boundary conditions (Phases IV and V), and test generalizability on a U.S. representative sample, an Indian sample, and a U.K. sample (Phase VI). Finally, we consulted the predictions of experts in the field to examine how well they align with our results (Phase VII). We believe this initial predictive model is limited and bad, but by developing a model that makes highly specific predictions, drawing on the state of the art, we hope to provide a foundation from which research can build to improve science of prejudice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1235-1265"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139932658","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Counterfactuals and the logic of causal selection. 反事实和因果选择的逻辑。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000428
Tadeg Quillien, Christopher G Lucas

Everything that happens has a multitude of causes, but people make causal judgments effortlessly. How do people select one particular cause (e.g., the lightning bolt that set the forest ablaze) out of the set of factors that contributed to the event (the oxygen in the air, the dry weather … )? Cognitive scientists have suggested that people make causal judgments about an event by simulating alternative ways things could have happened. We argue that this counterfactual theory explains many features of human causal intuitions, given two simple assumptions. First, people tend to imagine counterfactual possibilities that are both a priori likely and similar to what actually happened. Second, people judge that a factor C caused effect E if C and E are highly correlated across these counterfactual possibilities. In a reanalysis of existing empirical data, and a set of new experiments, we find that this theory uniquely accounts for people's causal intuitions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

任何事情的发生都有多种原因,但人们却能毫不费力地做出因果判断。人们是如何从导致事件发生的一系列因素(空气中的氧气、干燥的天气......)中选择一个特定的原因(例如,使森林燃烧的闪电)呢?认知科学家认为,人们是通过模拟事情可能发生的其他方式来对事件进行因果判断的。我们认为,在两个简单假设的前提下,这种反事实理论可以解释人类因果直觉的许多特征。首先,人们倾向于想象既有先验可能性又与实际情况相似的反事实可能性。其次,如果 C 和 E 在这些反事实可能性中高度相关,人们就会判断因素 C 导致了效应 E。通过重新分析现有的经验数据和一组新的实验,我们发现这一理论能唯一地解释人们的因果直觉。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Counterfactuals and the logic of causal selection.","authors":"Tadeg Quillien, Christopher G Lucas","doi":"10.1037/rev0000428","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000428","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Everything that happens has a multitude of causes, but people make causal judgments effortlessly. How do people select one particular cause (e.g., the lightning bolt that set the forest ablaze) out of the set of factors that contributed to the event (the oxygen in the air, the dry weather … )? Cognitive scientists have suggested that people make causal judgments about an event by simulating alternative ways things could have happened. We argue that this counterfactual theory explains many features of human causal intuitions, given two simple assumptions. First, people tend to imagine counterfactual possibilities that are both a priori likely and similar to what actually happened. Second, people judge that a factor C caused effect E if C and E are highly correlated across these counterfactual possibilities. In a reanalysis of existing empirical data, and a set of new experiments, we find that this theory uniquely accounts for people's causal intuitions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1208-1234"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9593087","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bayesian confidence in optimal decisions. 最佳决策的贝叶斯信心。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000472
Joshua Calder-Travis, Lucie Charles, Rafal Bogacz, Nick Yeung

The optimal way to make decisions in many circumstances is to track the difference in evidence collected in favor of the options. The drift diffusion model (DDM) implements this approach and provides an excellent account of decisions and response times. However, existing DDM-based models of confidence exhibit certain deficits, and many theories of confidence have used alternative, nonoptimal models of decisions. Motivated by the historical success of the DDM, we ask whether simple extensions to this framework might allow it to better account for confidence. Motivated by the idea that the brain will not duplicate representations of evidence, in all model variants decisions and confidence are based on the same evidence accumulation process. We compare the models to benchmark results, and successfully apply four qualitative tests concerning the relationships between confidence, evidence, and time, in a new preregistered study. Using computationally cheap expressions to model confidence on a trial-by-trial basis, we find that a subset of model variants also provide a very good to excellent account of precise quantitative effects observed in confidence data. Specifically, our results favor the hypothesis that confidence reflects the strength of accumulated evidence penalized by the time taken to reach the decision (Bayesian readout), with the penalty applied not perfectly calibrated to the specific task context. These results suggest there is no need to abandon the DDM or single accumulator models to successfully account for confidence reports. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在许多情况下,做出决策的最佳方法是跟踪所收集到的有利于各种选择的证据的差异。漂移扩散模型(DDM)实现了这一方法,并对决策和反应时间做出了很好的解释。然而,现有的基于漂移扩散模型的信心模型存在一定缺陷,许多信心理论都使用了替代性的非最佳决策模型。在 DDM 历史性成功的激励下,我们提出了这样一个问题:对这一框架进行简单扩展,是否就能更好地解释信心问题?受大脑不会重复表示证据这一观点的启发,在所有模型变体中,决策和信心都基于相同的证据积累过程。我们将这些模型与基准结果进行了比较,并在一项新的预注册研究中成功应用了有关信心、证据和时间之间关系的四项定性测试。通过使用计算成本低廉的表达式对逐次试验的置信度进行建模,我们发现模型变体的子集也能很好甚至出色地解释置信度数据中观察到的精确定量效应。具体来说,我们的结果倾向于这样一种假设,即信心反映了累积证据的强度,并受到做出决定所需时间的惩罚(贝叶斯读数),而所应用的惩罚并没有完全适应特定的任务情境。这些结果表明,没有必要放弃DDM或单一累积器模型来成功解释置信度报告。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bayesian confidence in optimal decisions.","authors":"Joshua Calder-Travis, Lucie Charles, Rafal Bogacz, Nick Yeung","doi":"10.1037/rev0000472","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000472","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The optimal way to make decisions in many circumstances is to track the difference in evidence collected in favor of the options. The drift diffusion model (DDM) implements this approach and provides an excellent account of decisions and response times. However, existing DDM-based models of confidence exhibit certain deficits, and many theories of confidence have used alternative, nonoptimal models of decisions. Motivated by the historical success of the DDM, we ask whether simple extensions to this framework might allow it to better account for confidence. Motivated by the idea that the brain will not duplicate representations of evidence, in all model variants decisions and confidence are based on the same evidence accumulation process. We compare the models to benchmark results, and successfully apply four qualitative tests concerning the relationships between confidence, evidence, and time, in a new preregistered study. Using computationally cheap expressions to model confidence on a trial-by-trial basis, we find that a subset of model variants also provide a very good to excellent account of precise quantitative effects observed in confidence data. Specifically, our results favor the hypothesis that confidence reflects the strength of accumulated evidence penalized by the time taken to reach the decision (Bayesian readout), with the penalty applied not perfectly calibrated to the specific task context. These results suggest there is no need to abandon the DDM or single accumulator models to successfully account for confidence reports. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1114-1160"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7617410/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relation between learning and stimulus-response binding. 学习与刺激-反应结合之间的关系。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000449
Christian Frings, Anna Foerster, Birte Moeller, Bernhard Pastötter, Roland Pfister

Perception and action rely on integrating or binding different features of stimuli and responses. Such bindings are short-lived, but they can be retrieved for a limited amount of time if any of their features is reactivated. This is particularly true for stimulus-response bindings, allowing for flexible recycling of previous action plans. A relation to learning of stimulus-response associations suggests itself, and previous accounts have proposed binding as an initial step of forging associations in long-term memory. The evidence for this claim is surprisingly mixed, however. Here we propose a framework that explains previous failures to detect meaningful relations of binding and learning by highlighting the joint contribution of three variables: (a) decay, (b) the number of repetitions, and (c) the time elapsing between repetitions. Accounting for the interplay of these variables provides a promising blueprint for innovative experimental designs that bridge the gap between immediate bindings on the one hand and lasting associations in memory on the other hand. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

感知和行动依赖于整合或绑定刺激和反应的不同特征。这种绑定是短暂的,但如果其中任何一个特征被重新激活,它们就可以在有限的时间内恢复。这一点在刺激-反应绑定中尤为明显,可以灵活地循环使用以前的行动计划。这与刺激-反应联想的学习有关,以前的观点认为绑定是在长时记忆中建立联想的第一步。然而,这一观点的证据却出人意料地参差不齐。在这里,我们提出了一个框架,通过强调三个变量的共同作用来解释之前未能发现有意义的绑定和学习关系的原因:(a)衰减,(b)重复次数,以及(c)重复之间的时间间隔。考虑到这些变量之间的相互作用,为创新实验设计提供了一个前景广阔的蓝图,从而在即时绑定与记忆中的持久联想之间架起了一座桥梁。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The relation between learning and stimulus-response binding.","authors":"Christian Frings, Anna Foerster, Birte Moeller, Bernhard Pastötter, Roland Pfister","doi":"10.1037/rev0000449","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000449","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perception and action rely on integrating or binding different features of stimuli and responses. Such bindings are short-lived, but they can be retrieved for a limited amount of time if any of their features is reactivated. This is particularly true for stimulus-response bindings, allowing for flexible recycling of previous action plans. A relation to learning of stimulus-response associations suggests itself, and previous accounts have proposed binding as an initial step of forging associations in long-term memory. The evidence for this claim is surprisingly mixed, however. Here we propose a framework that explains previous failures to detect meaningful relations of binding and learning by highlighting the joint contribution of three variables: (a) decay, (b) the number of repetitions, and (c) the time elapsing between repetitions. Accounting for the interplay of these variables provides a promising blueprint for innovative experimental designs that bridge the gap between immediate bindings on the one hand and lasting associations in memory on the other hand. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1290-1296"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138807777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unifying approaches to understanding capacity in change detection. 统一认识变化检测能力的方法。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000466
Lauren C Fong, Anthea G Blunden, Paul M Garrett, Philip L Smith, Daniel R Little

To navigate changes within a highly dynamic and complex environment, it is crucial to compare current visual representations of a scene to previously formed representations stored in memory. This process of mental comparison requires integrating information from multiple sources to inform decisions about changes within the environment. In the present article, we combine a novel systems factorial technology change detection task (Blunden et al., 2022) with a set size manipulation. Participants were required to detect 0, 1, or 2 changes of low and high detectability between a memory and probe array of 1-4 spatially separated luminance discs. Analyses using systems factorial technology indicated that the processing architecture was consistent across set sizes but that capacity was always limited and decreased as the number of distractors increased. We developed a novel model of change detection based on the statistical principles of basic sampling theory (Palmer, 1990; Sewell et al., 2014). The sample size model, instantiated parametrically, predicts the architecture and capacity results a priori and quantitatively accounted for several key results observed in the data: (a) increasing set size acted to decrease sensitivity (d') in proportion to the square root of the number of items in the display; (b) the effect of redundancy benefited performance by a factor of the square root of the number of changes; and (c) the effect of change detectability was separable and independent of the sample size costs and redundancy benefits. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

要在高度动态和复杂的环境中把握变化,就必须将当前场景的视觉表征与记忆中先前形成的表征进行比较。这种心理比较过程需要整合来自多个来源的信息,从而为环境变化决策提供依据。在本文中,我们将新颖的系统因子技术变化检测任务(Blunden 等人,2022 年)与集合大小操纵相结合。参与者需要检测由 1-4 个空间上分离的亮度圆盘组成的记忆阵列和探针阵列之间的 0、1 或 2 个低可检测性和高可检测性变化。使用系统因子技术进行的分析表明,在不同大小的集合中,处理结构是一致的,但能力总是有限的,并且随着分心物数量的增加而降低。我们根据基本抽样理论的统计原理(Palmer,1990 年;Sewell 等人,2014 年)建立了一个新颖的变化检测模型。样本大小模型通过实例化参数先验地预测了结构和能力结果,并定量地解释了数据中观察到的几个关键结果:(a) 增加集合大小会降低灵敏度 (d'),灵敏度的降低与显示项目数量的平方根成正比;(b) 冗余的效果会以变化数量平方根的系数提高性能;(c) 变化可探测性的效果是可分离的,与样本大小成本和冗余效益无关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Unifying approaches to understanding capacity in change detection.","authors":"Lauren C Fong, Anthea G Blunden, Paul M Garrett, Philip L Smith, Daniel R Little","doi":"10.1037/rev0000466","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000466","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To navigate changes within a highly dynamic and complex environment, it is crucial to compare current visual representations of a scene to previously formed representations stored in memory. This process of mental comparison requires integrating information from multiple sources to inform decisions about changes within the environment. In the present article, we combine a novel systems factorial technology change detection task (Blunden et al., 2022) with a set size manipulation. Participants were required to detect 0, 1, or 2 changes of low and high detectability between a memory and probe array of 1-4 spatially separated luminance discs. Analyses using systems factorial technology indicated that the processing architecture was consistent across set sizes but that capacity was always limited and decreased as the number of distractors increased. We developed a novel model of change detection based on the statistical principles of basic sampling theory (Palmer, 1990; Sewell et al., 2014). The sample size model, instantiated parametrically, predicts the architecture and capacity results a priori and quantitatively accounted for several key results observed in the data: (a) increasing set size acted to decrease sensitivity (<i>d</i>') in proportion to the square root of the number of items in the display; (b) the effect of redundancy benefited performance by a factor of the square root of the number of changes; and (c) the effect of change detectability was separable and independent of the sample size costs and redundancy benefits. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1266-1289"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141760648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How do people predict a random walk? Lessons for models of human cognition. 人们如何预测随机行走?人类认知模型的启示
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000493
Jake Spicer, Jian-Qiao Zhu, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn

Repeated forecasts of changing values are common in many everyday tasks, from predicting the weather to financial markets. A particularly simple and informative instance of such fluctuating values are random walks: Sequences in which each point is a random movement from only its preceding value, unaffected by any previous points. Moreover, random walks often yield basic rational forecasting solutions in which predictions of new values should repeat the most recent value, and hence replicate the properties of the original series. In previous experiments, however, we have found that human forecasters do not adhere to this standard, showing systematic deviations from the properties of a random walk such as excessive volatility and extreme movements between subsequent predictions. We suggest that such deviations reflect general statistical signatures of cognition displayed across multiple tasks, offering a window into underlying mechanisms. Using these deviations as new criteria, we here explore several cognitive models of forecasting drawn from various approaches developed in the existing literature, including Bayesian, error-based learning, autoregressive, and sampling mechanisms. These models are contrasted with human data from two experiments to determine which best accounts for the particular statistical features displayed by participants. We find support for sampling models in both aggregate and individual fits, suggesting that these variations are attributable to the use of inherently stochastic prediction systems. We thus argue that variability in predictions is strongly influenced by computational noise within the decision making process, with less influence from "late" noise at the output stage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

从预测天气到金融市场,重复预测不断变化的数值在许多日常工作中都很常见。随机漫步就是这种数值波动的一个特别简单且信息丰富的例子:序列中的每个点都是在其前一个值的基础上随机移动的,不受任何前一个点的影响。此外,随机漫步通常会产生基本的理性预测方案,其中对新值的预测应重复最近的值,从而复制原始序列的特性。然而,在之前的实验中,我们发现人类预测者并没有遵守这一标准,而是系统性地偏离了随机游走的特性,例如过度波动和后续预测之间的极端变动。我们认为,这种偏差反映了认知在多个任务中表现出的一般统计特征,为了解潜在机制提供了一个窗口。利用这些偏差作为新的标准,我们在此探讨了几种预测认知模型,这些模型来自现有文献中开发的各种方法,包括贝叶斯、基于误差的学习、自回归和抽样机制。我们将这些模型与两次实验中的人类数据进行对比,以确定哪种模型最能说明参与者所显示的特定统计特征。我们发现抽样模型在总体和个体拟合上都得到了支持,这表明这些变化可归因于使用了固有的随机预测系统。因此,我们认为预测的变异受决策过程中计算噪音的影响较大,而受输出阶段 "后期 "噪音的影响较小。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How do people predict a random walk? Lessons for models of human cognition.","authors":"Jake Spicer, Jian-Qiao Zhu, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn","doi":"10.1037/rev0000493","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000493","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Repeated forecasts of changing values are common in many everyday tasks, from predicting the weather to financial markets. A particularly simple and informative instance of such fluctuating values are <i>random walks</i>: Sequences in which each point is a random movement from only its preceding value, unaffected by any previous points. Moreover, random walks often yield basic rational forecasting solutions in which predictions of new values should repeat the most recent value, and hence replicate the properties of the original series. In previous experiments, however, we have found that human forecasters do not adhere to this standard, showing systematic deviations from the properties of a random walk such as excessive volatility and extreme movements between subsequent predictions. We suggest that such deviations reflect general statistical signatures of cognition displayed across multiple tasks, offering a window into underlying mechanisms. Using these deviations as new criteria, we here explore several cognitive models of forecasting drawn from various approaches developed in the existing literature, including Bayesian, error-based learning, autoregressive, and sampling mechanisms. These models are contrasted with human data from two experiments to determine which best accounts for the particular statistical features displayed by participants. We find support for sampling models in both aggregate and individual fits, suggesting that these variations are attributable to the use of inherently stochastic prediction systems. We thus argue that variability in predictions is strongly influenced by computational noise within the decision making process, with less influence from \"late\" noise at the output stage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1069-1113"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142294112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring the impact of multiple social cues to advance theory in person perception research. 衡量多重社会线索的影响,推进人的感知研究理论。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000503
Samuel A W Klein, Jeffrey W Sherman

Forming impressions of others is a fundamental aspect of social life. These impressions necessitate the integration of many and varied sources of information about other people, including social group memberships, apparent personality traits, inferences from observed behaviors, and so forth. However, methodological limitations have hampered progress in understanding this integration process. In particular, extant approaches have been unable to measure the independent contributions of multiple features to a given impression. In this article, after describing these limitations and their constraints on theory testing and development, we present a multinomial processing tree model as a computational solution to the problem. Specifically, the model distinguishes the contributions of multiple cues to social judgment. We describe an empirical demonstration of how applying the model can resolve long-standing debates among person perception researchers. Finally, we survey a variety of questions to which this approach can be profitably applied. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

形成对他人的印象是社会生活的一个基本方面。这些印象的形成需要整合许多不同来源的有关他人的信息,包括社会群体成员身份、明显的个性特征、观察到的行为推断等。然而,方法上的局限性阻碍了对这一整合过程的理解。特别是,现有的方法无法测量多个特征对特定印象的独立贡献。在本文中,我们在阐述了这些局限性及其对理论测试和发展的制约之后,提出了一种多叉处理树模型作为该问题的计算解决方案。具体来说,该模型区分了多种线索对社会判断的贡献。我们描述了应用该模型如何解决人的感知研究人员之间长期争论的实证演示。最后,我们探讨了这一方法可用于解决的各种问题。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Measuring the impact of multiple social cues to advance theory in person perception research.","authors":"Samuel A W Klein, Jeffrey W Sherman","doi":"10.1037/rev0000503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000503","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Forming impressions of others is a fundamental aspect of social life. These impressions necessitate the integration of many and varied sources of information about other people, including social group memberships, apparent personality traits, inferences from observed behaviors, and so forth. However, methodological limitations have hampered progress in understanding this integration process. In particular, extant approaches have been unable to measure the independent contributions of multiple features to a given impression. In this article, after describing these limitations and their constraints on theory testing and development, we present a multinomial processing tree model as a computational solution to the problem. Specifically, the model distinguishes the contributions of multiple cues to social judgment. We describe an empirical demonstration of how applying the model can resolve long-standing debates among person perception researchers. Finally, we survey a variety of questions to which this approach can be profitably applied. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142294114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Efficient visual representations for learning and decision making. 用于学习和决策的高效视觉表征。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000498
Tyler Malloy, Chris R Sims

The efficient representation of visual information is essential for learning and decision making due to the complexity and uncertainty of the world, as well as inherent constraints on the capacity of cognitive systems. We hypothesize that biological agents learn to efficiently represent visual information in a manner that balances performance across multiple potentially competing objectives. In this article, we examine two such objectives: storing information in a manner that supports accurate recollection (maximizing veridicality) and in a manner that facilitates utility-based decision making (maximizing behavioral utility). That these two objectives may be in conflict is not immediately obvious. Our hypothesis suggests that neither behavior nor representation formation can be fully understood by studying either in isolation, with information processing constraints exerting an overarching influence. Alongside this hypothesis we develop a computational model of representation formation and behavior motivated by recent methods in machine learning and neuroscience. The resulting model explains both the beneficial aspects of human visual learning, such as fast acquisition and high generalization, as well as the biases that result from information constraints. To test this model, we developed two experimental paradigms, in decision making and learning, to evaluate how well the model's predictions match human behavior. A key feature of the proposed model is that it predicts the occurrence of commonly found biases in human decision making, resulting from the desire to form efficient representations of visual information that are useful for behavioral goals in learning and decision making and optimized under an information processing constraint. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

由于世界的复杂性和不确定性,以及认知系统能力的内在限制,视觉信息的有效表征对于学习和决策至关重要。我们假设,生物制剂能学会以一种平衡多个潜在竞争目标的方式有效地表征视觉信息。在本文中,我们将探讨这样两个目标:以支持准确回忆(最大化真实性)和促进基于效用的决策(最大化行为效用)的方式存储信息。这两个目标可能存在冲突,这一点并不明显。我们的假设表明,孤立地研究行为或表征的形成都无法完全理解它们,信息处理的限制因素会对它们产生总体影响。在提出这一假设的同时,我们借鉴机器学习和神经科学的最新方法,建立了表征形成和行为的计算模型。由此产生的模型既能解释人类视觉学习的有利方面,如快速获取和高度泛化,也能解释信息限制导致的偏差。为了检验这一模型,我们开发了决策和学习两个实验范例,以评估模型的预测与人类行为的匹配程度。该模型的一个主要特点是,它能预测人类决策过程中常见偏差的出现,这些偏差是由于人类希望形成有效的视觉信息表征,以实现学习和决策过程中的行为目标,并在信息处理约束条件下进行优化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Efficient visual representations for learning and decision making.","authors":"Tyler Malloy, Chris R Sims","doi":"10.1037/rev0000498","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000498","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The efficient representation of visual information is essential for learning and decision making due to the complexity and uncertainty of the world, as well as inherent constraints on the capacity of cognitive systems. We hypothesize that biological agents learn to efficiently represent visual information in a manner that balances performance across multiple potentially competing objectives. In this article, we examine two such objectives: storing information in a manner that supports accurate recollection (maximizing veridicality) and in a manner that facilitates utility-based decision making (maximizing behavioral utility). That these two objectives may be in conflict is not immediately obvious. Our hypothesis suggests that neither behavior nor representation formation can be fully understood by studying either in isolation, with information processing constraints exerting an overarching influence. Alongside this hypothesis we develop a computational model of representation formation and behavior motivated by recent methods in machine learning and neuroscience. The resulting model explains both the beneficial aspects of human visual learning, such as fast acquisition and high generalization, as well as the biases that result from information constraints. To test this model, we developed two experimental paradigms, in decision making and learning, to evaluate how well the model's predictions match human behavior. A key feature of the proposed model is that it predicts the occurrence of commonly found biases in human decision making, resulting from the desire to form efficient representations of visual information that are useful for behavioral goals in learning and decision making and optimized under an information processing constraint. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142294109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1