首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
The role of recollection and familiarity in visual working memory: A mixture of threshold and signal detection processes. 回忆和熟悉在视觉工作记忆中的作用:阈值和信号检测过程的混合
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-15 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000432
Andrew P Yonelinas

Whether working memory reflects a thresholded recollection process whereby only a limited number of items are maintained in memory, or a signal detection process in which each studied item is increased in familiarity strength, is a topic of considerable debate. A review of visual working memory studies that have examined receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) across a broad set of materials and test conditions indicates that both signal detection and threshold processes contribute to working memory. In addition, the role that these two processes play varies systematically across conditions, such that a threshold process plays a particularly critical role when binary old/new judgments are required, when changes are relatively discrete, and when the hippocampus does not contribute to performance. In contrast, a signal detection process plays a greater role when confidence judgments are required, when the materials or the changes are global in nature, and when the hippocampus contributes to performance. In addition, the ROC results indicate that in standard single-probe tests of working memory, items that are maintained in an active recollected state support both recall-to-accept and recall-to-reject responses; whereas in complex-probe tests, recollection preferentially supports recall-to-reject; and in item-recognition tests it preferentially supports recall-to-accept. Moreover, there is growing evidence that these threshold and strength-based processes are related to distinct states of conscious awareness whereby they support perceiving- and sensing-based responses, respectively. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

工作记忆究竟是反映了一种阈值化的回忆过程,即记忆中只保留有限数量的项目,还是反映了一种信号检测过程,即每个学习项目的熟悉强度都在增加,这是一个颇有争议的话题。对视觉工作记忆研究的回顾表明,信号检测和阈值过程都有助于工作记忆。此外,这两种过程在不同条件下所起的作用也有系统性的差异,例如,当需要二元新旧判断、变化相对离散以及海马体对表现无贡献时,阈值过程就起着特别关键的作用。与此相反,当需要进行置信度判断、材料或变化具有整体性以及海马体对表现有贡献时,信号检测过程就会发挥更大的作用。此外,ROC 结果表明,在工作记忆的标准单探究测试中,保持积极回忆状态的项目既支持回忆-接受反应,也支持回忆-拒绝反应;而在复杂探究测试中,回忆优先支持回忆-拒绝反应;在项目识别测试中,回忆优先支持回忆-接受反应。此外,越来越多的证据表明,这些以阈值和强度为基础的过程与不同的意识状态有关,它们分别支持以感知和感觉为基础的反应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of recollection and familiarity in visual working memory: A mixture of threshold and signal detection processes.","authors":"Andrew P Yonelinas","doi":"10.1037/rev0000432","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000432","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Whether working memory reflects a thresholded recollection process whereby only a limited number of items are maintained in memory, or a signal detection process in which each studied item is increased in familiarity strength, is a topic of considerable debate. A review of visual working memory studies that have examined receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) across a broad set of materials and test conditions indicates that <i>both</i> signal detection and threshold processes contribute to working memory. In addition, the role that these two processes play varies systematically across conditions, such that a threshold process plays a particularly critical role when binary old/new judgments are required, when changes are relatively discrete, and when the hippocampus does <i>not</i> contribute to performance. In contrast, a signal detection process plays a greater role when confidence judgments are required, when the materials or the changes are global in nature, and when the hippocampus contributes to performance. In addition, the ROC results indicate that in standard single-probe tests of working memory, items that are maintained in an active recollected state support both recall-to-accept and recall-to-reject responses; whereas in complex-probe tests, recollection preferentially supports recall-to-reject; and in item-recognition tests it preferentially supports recall-to-accept. Moreover, there is growing evidence that these threshold and strength-based processes are related to distinct states of conscious awareness whereby they support perceiving- and sensing-based responses, respectively. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11089539/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9637167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When working memory may be just working, not memory. 工作记忆可能只是工作,而不是记忆。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-13 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000448
Andre Beukers, Maia Hamin, Kenneth A Norman, Jonathan D Cohen

The N-back task is often considered to be a canonical example of a task that relies on working memory (WM), requiring both maintenance of representations of previously presented stimuli and also processing of these representations. In particular, the set-size effect in this task (e.g., poorer performance on three-back than two-back judgments), as in others, is often interpreted as indicating that the task relies on retention and processing of information in a limited-capacity WM system. Here, we consider an alternative possibility: that retention in episodic memory (EM) rather than WM can account for both set-size and lure effects in the N-back task. Accordingly, performance in the N-back task may reflect engagement of the processing ("working") function of WM but not necessarily limits in either that processing ability nor in retention ("memory"). To demonstrate this point, we constructed a neural network model that was augmented with an EM component, but lacked any capacity to retain information across trials in WM, and trained it to perform the N-back task. We show that this model can account for the set-size and lure effects obtained in an N-back study by M. J. Kane et al. (2007), and that it does so as a result of the well-understood effects of temporal distinctiveness on EM retrieval, and the processing of this information in WM. These findings help illuminate the ways in which WM may interact with EM in the service of cognitive function and add to a growing body of evidence that tasks commonly assumed to rely on WM may alternatively (or additionally) rely on EM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

N-back任务通常被认为是依赖于工作记忆(WM)的任务的典型例子,它既需要维持先前呈现的刺激的表征,也需要处理这些表征。特别是,这个任务中的集合大小效应(例如,三回判断的表现比两回判断差),和其他任务一样,经常被解释为表明该任务依赖于有限容量WM系统中信息的保留和处理。在这里,我们考虑了另一种可能性:情景记忆(EM)中的保留而不是情景记忆(WM)可以解释N-back任务中的集大小和诱饵效应。因此,在N-back任务中的表现可能反映了WM的处理(“工作”)功能的参与,但不一定限制了处理能力或保留(“记忆”)。为了证明这一点,我们构建了一个神经网络模型,该模型增强了EM组件,但缺乏在WM中跨试验保留信息的能力,并训练它执行N-back任务。我们的研究表明,该模型可以解释M. J. Kane等人(2007)在N-back研究中获得的集大小和诱饵效应,这是由于我们很好地理解了时间独特性对EM检索的影响,以及在WM中对这些信息的处理。这些发现有助于阐明WM在认知功能服务中与EM相互作用的方式,并增加了越来越多的证据,表明通常被认为依赖于WM的任务可能替代(或额外)依赖于EM。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA,所有权利保留)。
{"title":"When working memory may be just working, not memory.","authors":"Andre Beukers, Maia Hamin, Kenneth A Norman, Jonathan D Cohen","doi":"10.1037/rev0000448","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000448","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The <i>N</i>-back task is often considered to be a canonical example of a task that relies on working memory (WM), requiring both maintenance of representations of previously presented stimuli and also processing of these representations. In particular, the set-size effect in this task (e.g., poorer performance on three-back than two-back judgments), as in others, is often interpreted as indicating that the task relies on retention and processing of information in a limited-capacity WM system. Here, we consider an alternative possibility: that retention in episodic memory (EM) rather than WM can account for both set-size and lure effects in the N-back task. Accordingly, performance in the N-back task may reflect engagement of the processing (\"working\") function of WM but not necessarily limits in either that processing ability nor in retention (\"memory\"). To demonstrate this point, we constructed a neural network model that was augmented with an EM component, but lacked any capacity to retain information across trials in WM, and trained it to perform the N-back task. We show that this model can account for the set-size and lure effects obtained in an N-back study by M. J. Kane et al. (2007), and that it does so as a result of the well-understood effects of temporal distinctiveness on EM retrieval, and the processing of this information in WM. These findings help illuminate the ways in which WM may interact with EM in the service of cognitive function and add to a growing body of evidence that tasks commonly assumed to rely on WM may alternatively (or additionally) rely on EM. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92156313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A probabilistic successor representation for context-dependent learning. 用于上下文相关学习的概率继任表征。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000414
Jesse P Geerts, Samuel J Gershman, Neil Burgess, Kimberly L Stachenfeld

Two of the main impediments to learning complex tasks are that relationships between different stimuli, including rewards, can be uncertain and context-dependent. Reinforcement learning (RL) provides a framework for learning, by predicting total future reward directly (model-free RL), or via predictions of future states (model-based RL). Within this framework, "successor representation" (SR) predicts total future occupancy of all states. A recent theoretical proposal suggests that the hippocampus encodes the SR in order to facilitate prediction of future reward. However, this proposal does not take into account how learning should adapt under uncertainty and switches of context. Here, we introduce a theory of learning SRs using prediction errors which includes optimally balancing uncertainty in new observations versus existing knowledge. We then generalize that approach to a multicontext setting, allowing the model to learn and maintain multiple task-specific SRs and infer which one to use at any moment based on the accuracy of its predictions. Thus, the context used for predictions can be determined by both the contents of the states themselves and the distribution of transitions between them. This probabilistic SR model captures animal behavior in tasks which require contextual memory and generalization, and unifies previous SR theory with hippocampal-dependent contextual decision-making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学习复杂任务的两个主要障碍是,不同刺激(包括奖励)之间的关系可能是不确定的,并且与情境有关。强化学习(RL)通过直接预测未来总奖励(无模型强化学习)或通过预测未来状态(基于模型强化学习)为学习提供了一个框架。在这一框架内,"后继表征"(SR)预测所有状态的未来总占用率。最近的一项理论建议认为,海马编码 SR 是为了促进对未来奖励的预测。然而,这一建议并没有考虑到学习应如何适应不确定性和情境的转换。在这里,我们将介绍一种利用预测误差学习 SR 的理论,其中包括在新的观察结果与现有知识之间实现不确定性的最佳平衡。然后,我们将这种方法推广到多情境设置中,允许模型学习和维护多个特定任务的 SR,并根据预测的准确性推断在任何时刻使用哪一个。因此,预测所使用的情境可以由状态本身的内容和状态之间的转换分布来决定。这种概率SR模型捕捉到了需要情境记忆和概括的任务中的动物行为,并将以前的SR理论与依赖海马的情境决策统一起来。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A probabilistic successor representation for context-dependent learning.","authors":"Jesse P Geerts, Samuel J Gershman, Neil Burgess, Kimberly L Stachenfeld","doi":"10.1037/rev0000414","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000414","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two of the main impediments to learning complex tasks are that relationships between different stimuli, including rewards, can be uncertain and context-dependent. Reinforcement learning (RL) provides a framework for learning, by predicting total future reward directly (model-free RL), or via predictions of future states (model-based RL). Within this framework, \"successor representation\" (SR) predicts total future occupancy of all states. A recent theoretical proposal suggests that the hippocampus encodes the SR in order to facilitate prediction of future reward. However, this proposal does not take into account how learning should adapt under uncertainty and switches of context. Here, we introduce a theory of learning SRs using prediction errors which includes optimally balancing uncertainty in new observations versus existing knowledge. We then generalize that approach to a multicontext setting, allowing the model to learn and maintain multiple task-specific SRs and infer which one to use at any moment based on the accuracy of its predictions. Thus, the context used for predictions can be determined by both the contents of the states themselves and the distribution of transitions between them. This probabilistic SR model captures animal behavior in tasks which require contextual memory and generalization, and unifies previous SR theory with hippocampal-dependent contextual decision-making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9796965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In search of better practice in executive functions assessment: Methodological issues and potential solutions. 寻求更好的执行功能评估方法:方法问题和潜在解决方案。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-24 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000434
Marc Yangüez, Benoit Bediou, Julien Chanal, Daphne Bavelier

The multicomponent nature of executive functions (EF) has long been recognized, pushing for a better understanding of both the commonalities and the diversity between EF components. Despite the advances made, the operationalization of performance in EF tasks remains rather heterogeneous, and the structure of EF as modeled by confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) is still a topic of debate (Karr et al., 2018). The present work demonstrates these two issues are related, showing how different operationalizations in task-based performance indicators impact the resulting models of EF structure with CFA. Using bootstrapped data from 294 children (8-12 years old) and nine EF tasks (tapping inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility), we first show improved model convergence and acceptance when operationalizing EF through single tasks' scores (e.g., incongruent trials, Flanker task) relative to difference scores (e.g., incongruent minus congruent trials, Flanker task). Furthermore, we show that response times exhibit poor model convergence and acceptance compared not only to accuracy but also drift rate. The latter, a well-known indicator in drift-diffusion models, is found to present the best trade-off between convergence and acceptance to model EF with CFA. Finally, we examine how various operationalizations of performance in EF tasks impact CFA model comparison in the assessment of EF structure and discuss the theoretical foundations for these results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们早已认识到执行功能(EF)的多成分性,这促使人们更好地理解EF成分之间的共性和多样性。尽管取得了进展,但EF任务中表现的操作化仍然相当不统一,而通过确证因子分析(CFA)建模的EF结构仍然是一个争论不休的话题(Karr等人,2018)。本研究证明了这两个问题之间的关联,展示了基于任务的绩效指标的不同操作方法如何影响CFA得出的EF结构模型。利用来自 294 名儿童(8-12 岁)的引导数据和九项 EF 任务(挖掘抑制、工作记忆和认知灵活性),我们首先展示了通过单一任务得分(如不一致试验、Flanker 任务)来操作 EF 时,相对于差异得分(如不一致试验减去一致试验、Flanker 任务),模型的收敛性和接受度有所提高。此外,我们还发现,与准确率和漂移率相比,反应时间表现出较低的模型收敛性和接受性。漂移率是漂移扩散模型中的一个著名指标,我们发现它在收敛性和接受性之间做出了最佳权衡,从而可以用 CFA 建立 EF 模型。最后,我们研究了在评估 EF 结构时,EF 任务中各种绩效操作对 CFA 模型比较的影响,并讨论了这些结果的理论基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"In search of better practice in executive functions assessment: Methodological issues and potential solutions.","authors":"Marc Yangüez, Benoit Bediou, Julien Chanal, Daphne Bavelier","doi":"10.1037/rev0000434","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000434","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The multicomponent nature of executive functions (EF) has long been recognized, pushing for a better understanding of both the commonalities and the diversity between EF components. Despite the advances made, the operationalization of performance in EF tasks remains rather heterogeneous, and the structure of EF as modeled by confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) is still a topic of debate (Karr et al., 2018). The present work demonstrates these two issues are related, showing how different operationalizations in task-based performance indicators impact the resulting models of EF structure with CFA. Using bootstrapped data from 294 children (8-12 years old) and nine EF tasks (tapping inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility), we first show improved model convergence and acceptance when operationalizing EF through single tasks' scores (e.g., incongruent trials, Flanker task) relative to difference scores (e.g., incongruent minus congruent trials, Flanker task). Furthermore, we show that response times exhibit poor model convergence and acceptance compared not only to accuracy but also drift rate. The latter, a well-known indicator in drift-diffusion models, is found to present the best trade-off between convergence and acceptance to model EF with CFA. Finally, we examine how various operationalizations of performance in EF tasks impact CFA model comparison in the assessment of EF structure and discuss the theoretical foundations for these results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10416084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humans reconfigure target and distractor processing to address distinct task demands. 人类重新配置目标和干扰处理,以满足不同的任务需求。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000442
Harrison Ritz, Amitai Shenhav

When faced with distraction, we can focus more on goal-relevant information (targets) or focus less on goal-conflicting information (distractors). How people use cognitive control to distribute attention across targets and distractors remains unclear. We address this question by developing a novel Parametric Attentional Control Task that can "tag" participants' sensitivity to target and distractor information. We use these precise measures of attention to develop a novel process model that can explain how participants control attention toward targets and distractors. Across three experiments, we find that participants met the demands of this task by independently controlling their processing of target and distractor information, exhibiting distinct adaptations to manipulations of incentives and conflict. Whereas incentives preferentially led to target enhancement, conflict in the previous trial preferentially led to distractor suppression. These distinct drivers of control altered sensitivity to targets and distractors early in the trial, promptly followed by reactive reconfiguration toward task-appropriate feature sensitivity. To provide a process-level account of these empirical findings, we develop a novel neural network model of evidence accumulation with attractor dynamics over feature weights that reconfigure target and distractor processing. These results provide a computational account of control reconfiguration that provides new insights into how multivariate attentional signals are optimized to achieve task goals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当面对分心时,我们可以更多地关注与目标相关的信息(目标),或者更少地关注与目的冲突的信息(分心物)。人们如何利用认知控制来将注意力分散在目标和干扰物之间,目前尚不清楚。我们通过开发一种新的参数注意力控制任务来解决这个问题,该任务可以“标记”参与者对目标和干扰信息的敏感性。我们使用这些精确的注意力测量来开发一个新的过程模型,该模型可以解释参与者如何控制对目标和分心物的注意力。在三个实验中,我们发现参与者通过独立控制他们对目标和干扰信息的处理来满足这项任务的要求,对激励和冲突的操纵表现出不同的适应能力。尽管激励优先导致目标增强,但先前试验中的冲突优先导致干扰抑制。在试验的早期,这些不同的控制驱动因素改变了对目标和干扰物的敏感性,随后迅速进行反应性重新配置,以达到任务适当的特征敏感性。为了提供这些经验发现的过程级说明,我们开发了一种新的证据积累神经网络模型,该模型具有特征权重上的吸引子动力学,可以重新配置目标和干扰物处理。这些结果提供了控制重构的计算说明,为如何优化多变量注意力信号以实现任务目标提供了新的见解。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Humans reconfigure target and distractor processing to address distinct task demands.","authors":"Harrison Ritz, Amitai Shenhav","doi":"10.1037/rev0000442","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When faced with distraction, we can focus more on goal-relevant information (targets) or focus less on goal-conflicting information (distractors). How people use cognitive control to distribute attention across targets and distractors remains unclear. We address this question by developing a novel Parametric Attentional Control Task that can \"tag\" participants' sensitivity to target and distractor information. We use these precise measures of attention to develop a novel process model that can explain how participants control attention toward targets and distractors. Across three experiments, we find that participants met the demands of this task by independently controlling their processing of target and distractor information, exhibiting distinct adaptations to manipulations of incentives and conflict. Whereas incentives preferentially led to target enhancement, conflict in the previous trial preferentially led to distractor suppression. These distinct drivers of control altered sensitivity to targets and distractors early in the trial, promptly followed by reactive reconfiguration toward task-appropriate feature sensitivity. To provide a process-level account of these empirical findings, we develop a novel neural network model of evidence accumulation with attractor dynamics over feature weights that reconfigure target and distractor processing. These results provide a computational account of control reconfiguration that provides new insights into how multivariate attentional signals are optimized to achieve task goals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11193598/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10152529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The autocorrelated Bayesian sampler: A rational process for probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choices, confidence judgments, and response times. 自相关贝叶斯采样器:概率判断、估计、置信区间、选择、置信判断和响应时间的合理过程。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000427
Jian-Qiao Zhu, Joakim Sundh, Jake Spicer, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn

Normative models of decision-making that optimally transform noisy (sensory) information into categorical decisions qualitatively mismatch human behavior. Indeed, leading computational models have only achieved high empirical corroboration by adding task-specific assumptions that deviate from normative principles. In response, we offer a Bayesian approach that implicitly produces a posterior distribution of possible answers (hypotheses) in response to sensory information. But we assume that the brain has no direct access to this posterior, but can only sample hypotheses according to their posterior probabilities. Accordingly, we argue that the primary problem of normative concern in decision-making is integrating stochastic hypotheses, rather than stochastic sensory information, to make categorical decisions. This implies that human response variability arises mainly from posterior sampling rather than sensory noise. Because human hypothesis generation is serially correlated, hypothesis samples will be autocorrelated. Guided by this new problem formulation, we develop a new process, the Autocorrelated Bayesian Sampler (ABS), which grounds autocorrelated hypothesis generation in a sophisticated sampling algorithm. The ABS provides a single mechanism that qualitatively explains many empirical effects of probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choice, confidence judgments, response times, and their relationships. Our analysis demonstrates the unifying power of a perspective shift in the exploration of normative models. It also exemplifies the proposal that the "Bayesian brain" operates using samples not probabilities, and that variability in human behavior may primarily reflect computational rather than sensory noise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

将嘈杂(感官)信息最优化地转化为分类决策的规范决策模型与人类行为在本质上并不匹配。事实上,领先的计算模型只有通过添加偏离规范原则的特定任务假设,才能获得较高的经验佐证。作为回应,我们提供了一种贝叶斯方法,这种方法会根据感官信息隐含地生成可能答案(假设)的后验分布。但我们假定,大脑无法直接获取这种后验分布,而只能根据后验概率对假设进行抽样。因此,我们认为,决策中规范性关注的首要问题是整合随机假设,而不是随机感官信息,以做出分类决策。这意味着人类反应的变异性主要来自于后验取样,而非感觉噪声。由于人类假设的产生具有序列相关性,因此假设样本将具有自相关性。在这一新问题表述的指导下,我们开发了一种新的程序--自相关贝叶斯采样器(ABS),它将自相关假设的生成置于复杂的采样算法中。自相关贝叶斯取样器提供了一种单一的机制,可以定性地解释概率判断、估计、置信区间、选择、置信判断、响应时间及其关系的许多经验效应。我们的分析展示了在探索规范模型时视角转换的统一力量。它还例证了 "贝叶斯大脑 "使用样本而非概率进行运作的提议,以及人类行为的可变性可能主要反映了计算噪音而非感官噪音。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The autocorrelated Bayesian sampler: A rational process for probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choices, confidence judgments, and response times.","authors":"Jian-Qiao Zhu, Joakim Sundh, Jake Spicer, Nick Chater, Adam N Sanborn","doi":"10.1037/rev0000427","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000427","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Normative models of decision-making that optimally transform noisy (sensory) information into categorical decisions qualitatively mismatch human behavior. Indeed, leading computational models have only achieved high empirical corroboration by adding task-specific assumptions that deviate from normative principles. In response, we offer a Bayesian approach that implicitly produces a posterior distribution of possible answers (hypotheses) in response to sensory information. But we assume that the brain has no direct access to this posterior, but can only <i>sample</i> hypotheses according to their posterior probabilities. Accordingly, we argue that the primary problem of normative concern in decision-making is integrating stochastic <i>hypotheses</i>, rather than stochastic sensory information, to make categorical decisions. This implies that human response variability arises mainly from posterior sampling rather than sensory noise. Because human hypothesis generation is serially correlated, hypothesis samples will be autocorrelated. Guided by this new problem formulation, we develop a new process, the Autocorrelated Bayesian Sampler (ABS), which grounds autocorrelated hypothesis generation in a sophisticated sampling algorithm. The ABS provides a single mechanism that qualitatively explains many empirical effects of probability judgments, estimates, confidence intervals, choice, confidence judgments, response times, and their relationships. Our analysis demonstrates the unifying power of a perspective shift in the exploration of normative models. It also exemplifies the proposal that the \"Bayesian brain\" operates using samples not probabilities, and that variability in human behavior may primarily reflect computational rather than sensory noise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11115360/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9586788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Updating, evidence evaluation, and operator availability: A theoretical framework for understanding belief. 更新、证据评估和操作者可用性:理解信念的理论框架。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000444
Joseph Sommer, Julien Musolino, Pernille Hemmer

Decades of findings in psychology suggest that human belief is thoroughly irrational. At best, beliefs might be formed by heuristic processes that predictably lead to suboptimal outcomes. At worst, they are slaves to motivated reasoning, which allows people to come to whichever conclusions they prefer. In this article, we suggest that belief updating, narrowly construed, may be a rational process that is uniquely sensitive to evidence and cognitively impenetrable to desires or incentives. Before any updating can occur, however, a series of processes mediate between information in the world and subjectively compelling evidence. We distinguish between updating proper and processes of evidence search, acceptance, hypothesis specification, integration of relevant information, and reasoning. We review research highlighting the computational difficulty inherent to each of these problems and conclude that solutions must be heuristic and fallible. Beyond incidental failures, evidence evaluation processes-unlike updating-are penetrable to motivation and as such, may be biased by people's desires and goals. In light of this distinction, we propose a theoretical framework for integrating research on belief which divides the cognitive processes involved in belief into two distinct levels. At Level 1, updating is suggested to be approximately Bayesian and impenetrable to desires and goals. In contrast, Level 2 processes, which search for and evaluate evidence, are cognitively penetrable. In addition, we emphasize that Level 2 processes are necessarily heuristic and exhibit bounded rationality (Simon, 1956) given the difficulty of the problems they have to solve. Finally, we specify an additional set of relatively invariant characteristics, which influence how Level 2 processes are employed by making different methods of information processing available. Our framework offers a more nuanced understanding of belief, permits a granular localization of irrationality, and may help reconcile extant debates in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

心理学数十年的研究结果表明,人类的信念是完全非理性的。在最好的情况下,信念可能是由启发式过程形成的,可预见地导致次优结果。在最坏的情况下,它们是动机推理的奴隶,动机推理允许人们得出自己喜欢的结论。在本文中,我们提出,狭义上的信念更新可能是一种理性过程,它对证据具有独特的敏感性,并且在认知上不受欲望或动机的影响。然而,在任何更新发生之前,世界上的信息和主观上令人信服的证据之间需要一系列的过程进行调解。我们将更新本身与证据搜索、接受、假设规范、整合相关信息和推理等过程区分开来。我们回顾了强调这些问题中每个问题内在计算难度的研究,并得出结论:解决方案必须是启发式和易错的。除了偶然的失误之外,证据评估过程与更新不同,可以渗透到动机中,因此可能会受到人们的愿望和目标的影响。根据这一区别,我们提出了一个整合信念研究的理论框架,将信念所涉及的认知过程分为两个不同的层次。在第一层次,更新被认为是近似贝叶斯的,不受欲望和目标的影响。与此相反,第二层次的过程,即搜索和评估证据的过程,在认知上是可以穿透的。此外,我们还强调,第二级过程必然是启发式的,鉴于其所要解决的问题的难度,第二级过程表现出有限理性(Simon,1956 年)。最后,我们还明确了一组相对不变的特征,这些特征通过提供不同的信息处理方法来影响第二级过程的使用方式。我们的框架提供了对信念更细致入微的理解,允许对非理性进行细化定位,并可能有助于调和文献中的现有争论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Updating, evidence evaluation, and operator availability: A theoretical framework for understanding belief.","authors":"Joseph Sommer, Julien Musolino, Pernille Hemmer","doi":"10.1037/rev0000444","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Decades of findings in psychology suggest that human belief is thoroughly irrational. At best, beliefs might be formed by heuristic processes that predictably lead to suboptimal outcomes. At worst, they are slaves to motivated reasoning, which allows people to come to whichever conclusions they prefer. In this article, we suggest that belief updating, narrowly construed, may be a rational process that is uniquely sensitive to evidence and cognitively impenetrable to desires or incentives. Before any updating can occur, however, a series of processes mediate between information in the world and subjectively compelling evidence. We distinguish between updating proper and processes of evidence search, acceptance, hypothesis specification, integration of relevant information, and reasoning. We review research highlighting the computational difficulty inherent to each of these problems and conclude that solutions must be heuristic and fallible. Beyond incidental failures, evidence evaluation processes-unlike updating-are penetrable to motivation and as such, may be biased by people's desires and goals. In light of this distinction, we propose a theoretical framework for integrating research on belief which divides the cognitive processes involved in belief into two distinct levels. At Level 1, updating is suggested to be approximately Bayesian and impenetrable to desires and goals. In contrast, Level 2 processes, which search for and evaluate evidence, are cognitively penetrable. In addition, we emphasize that Level 2 processes are necessarily heuristic and exhibit bounded rationality (Simon, 1956) given the difficulty of the problems they have to solve. Finally, we specify an additional set of relatively invariant characteristics, which influence how Level 2 processes are employed by making different methods of information processing available. Our framework offers a more nuanced understanding of belief, permits a granular localization of irrationality, and may help reconcile extant debates in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10005538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
PONG: A computational model of visual word recognition through bihemispheric activation. PONG:通过双半球激活进行视觉单词识别的计算模型。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000461
Joshua Snell

Orthographic processing is an open problem. Decades of visual word recognition research have fueled the development of various theoretical frameworks. Although these frameworks have had good explanatory power, various recent results cannot be satisfactorily captured in any model. In order to account for old and new phenomena alike, here I present a new theory of how the brain computes letter positions. According to PONG (which describes the Positional Ordering of N-Grams), each hemisphere of the brain comprises a set of mono- and multigram detectors. The crux is that the detectors for a given N-gram are activated to different extents in their respective hemispheres, depending on where in the visual field the N-gram is located. This differential activity allows the brain to estimate the leftness or rightness of that N-gram, whereby word activation is a function of the N-gram's identity plus its laterality relative to that of other activated N-grams. Simulations with PONG suggest that the framework effectively accounts for classic phenomena, as well as newer phenomena and cross-linguistic differences that cannot be explained by other models. I also reflect on the neurophysiological plausibility of the model and avenues for future inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

正字法处理是一个尚未解决的问题。数十年的视觉单词识别研究推动了各种理论框架的发展。尽管这些框架具有很好的解释力,但最近的各种结果却无法在任何模型中得到令人满意的体现。为了解释新旧现象,我在此提出了大脑如何计算字母位置的新理论。根据 PONG(描述 N 字符的位置排序)理论,大脑的每个半球都由一组单字符和多字符检测器组成。问题的关键在于,特定 N 字符的检测器在各自半球的激活程度不同,这取决于 N 字符在视野中的位置。这种不同的活动使大脑能够估算出该 N-gram的左度或右度,因此单词激活是该 N-gram的特性加上其相对于其他激活 N-gram的侧向性的函数。用 PONG 进行的模拟表明,该框架能有效解释经典现象以及其他模型无法解释的新现象和跨语言差异。我还对该模型的神经生理学合理性和未来的研究方向进行了思考。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"PONG: A computational model of visual word recognition through bihemispheric activation.","authors":"Joshua Snell","doi":"10.1037/rev0000461","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000461","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Orthographic processing is an open problem. Decades of visual word recognition research have fueled the development of various theoretical frameworks. Although these frameworks have had good explanatory power, various recent results cannot be satisfactorily captured in any model. In order to account for old and new phenomena alike, here I present a new theory of how the brain computes letter positions. According to <i>PONG</i> (which describes the <i>Positional Ordering of N-Grams</i>), each hemisphere of the brain comprises a set of mono- and multigram detectors. The crux is that the detectors for a given N-gram are activated to different extents in their respective hemispheres, depending on where in the visual field the N-gram is located. This differential activity allows the brain to estimate the leftness or rightness of that N-gram, whereby word activation is a function of the N-gram's identity plus its laterality relative to that of other activated N-grams. Simulations with PONG suggest that the framework effectively accounts for classic phenomena, as well as newer phenomena and cross-linguistic differences that cannot be explained by other models. I also reflect on the neurophysiological plausibility of the model and avenues for future inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139973304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prejudice model 1.0: A predictive model of prejudice. 偏见模型 1.0:偏见预测模型。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000470
Eric Hehman, Rebecca Neel

The present research develops a predictive model of prejudice. For nearly a century, psychology and other fields have sought to scientifically understand and describe the causes of prejudice. Numerous theories of prejudice now exist. Yet these theories are overwhelmingly defined verbally and thus lack the ability to precisely predict when and to what extent prejudice will emerge. The abundance of theory also raises the possibility of undetected overlap between constructs theorized to cause prejudice. Predictive models enable falsification and provide a way for the field to move forward. To this end, here we present 18 studies with ∼5,000 participants in seven phases of model development. After initially identifying major theorized causes of prejudice in the literature, we used a model selection approach to winnow constructs into a parsimonious predictive model of prejudice (Phases I and II). We confirm this model in a preregistered out-of-sample test (Phase III), test variations in operationalizations and boundary conditions (Phases IV and V), and test generalizability on a U.S. representative sample, an Indian sample, and a U.K. sample (Phase VI). Finally, we consulted the predictions of experts in the field to examine how well they align with our results (Phase VII). We believe this initial predictive model is limited and bad, but by developing a model that makes highly specific predictions, drawing on the state of the art, we hope to provide a foundation from which research can build to improve science of prejudice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究建立了一个偏见预测模型。近一个世纪以来,心理学和其他领域一直试图科学地理解和描述偏见的成因。现在已经有了许多关于偏见的理论。然而,这些理论绝大多数都是口头定义的,因此缺乏精确预测偏见何时以及在何种程度上会出现的能力。大量的理论还可能导致未被发现的、被认为会导致偏见的结构之间的重叠。预测模型可以帮助人们进行证伪,并为这一领域的研究提供了前进的方向。为此,我们在此介绍了模型开发七个阶段的18项研究,参与者达5000人。在初步确定了文献中偏见的主要理论成因后,我们使用模型选择法将各种建构物筛选成一个简明的偏见预测模型(第一和第二阶段)。我们在预先登记的样本外测试中确认了这一模型(第三阶段),测试了操作方法和边界条件的变化(第四和第五阶段),并在美国代表性样本、印度样本和英国样本中测试了可推广性(第六阶段)。最后,我们咨询了该领域专家的预测,以检验他们与我们的结果的一致性(第七阶段)。我们相信,这个初始预测模型是有限的,也是糟糕的,但是通过开发一个模型,在借鉴现有技术的基础上做出高度具体的预测,我们希望能够为研究提供一个基础,在此基础上改进偏见科学。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Prejudice model 1.0: A predictive model of prejudice.","authors":"Eric Hehman, Rebecca Neel","doi":"10.1037/rev0000470","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present research develops a predictive model of prejudice. For nearly a century, psychology and other fields have sought to scientifically understand and describe the causes of prejudice. Numerous theories of prejudice now exist. Yet these theories are overwhelmingly defined verbally and thus lack the ability to precisely predict when and to what extent prejudice will emerge. The abundance of theory also raises the possibility of undetected overlap between constructs theorized to cause prejudice. Predictive models enable falsification and provide a way for the field to move forward. To this end, here we present 18 studies with ∼5,000 participants in seven phases of model development. After initially identifying major theorized causes of prejudice in the literature, we used a model selection approach to winnow constructs into a parsimonious predictive model of prejudice (Phases I and II). We confirm this model in a preregistered out-of-sample test (Phase III), test variations in operationalizations and boundary conditions (Phases IV and V), and test generalizability on a U.S. representative sample, an Indian sample, and a U.K. sample (Phase VI). Finally, we consulted the predictions of experts in the field to examine how well they align with our results (Phase VII). We believe this initial predictive model is limited and bad, but by developing a model that makes highly specific predictions, drawing on the state of the art, we hope to provide a foundation from which research can build to improve science of prejudice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139932658","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How getting in sync is curative: Insights gained from research in psychotherapy. 同步是如何治疗的?从心理治疗研究中获得的启示。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000471
Sigal Zilcha-Mano

We are all constantly going in and out of sync with the people we meet in our lives: significant others, incidental encounters, and strangers. Synchrony is a ubiquitous phenomenon, considered an evolution-based mechanism of survival. In recent years, technological development has made it possible to collect much data on synchrony across disciplines. The collected data show great potential to shed light on the benefits of this universal phenomenon. At the same time, mixed results emerged, stressing the need for a theory to navigate research inquiries and discoveries. It is proposed here that synchrony serves as an individual-specific mechanism for making relationships curative in all life circumstances, especially therapeutic ones-hence its special relevance for psychotherapy. A synthesis of the majority of the literature across disciplines reveals two implicit assumptions about synchrony, resulting in two separate bodies of knowledge: (a) synchrony is a trait-like signature characterizing individuals; and (b) synchrony is a state-like phenomenon that can be manipulated in the lab. It is proposed here to personalize synchrony research by integrating the two assumptions into a comprehensive theory according to which individuals have a trait-like signature for getting in sync, which determines their physical and mental health, and that this deterministic reality can be subject to state-like manipulation. Individuals can deviate from their trait-like signature. When the deviation is toward normative activation, mental health improves, and the state-like changes are defined as therapeutic. This article calls for research to investigate how trait-like signature of synchrony develops and how it can be therapeutically changed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们经常与生活中遇到的人同步或不同步,包括重要的人、偶遇的人和陌生人。同步是一种无处不在的现象,被认为是一种基于进化的生存机制。近年来,技术的发展使得收集大量跨学科的同步数据成为可能。收集到的数据显示出巨大的潜力,可以揭示这一普遍现象的益处。与此同时,也出现了好坏参半的结果,这就强调了需要一种理论来指导研究调查和发现。我们在此提出,同步性是一种针对个体的机制,它能使人际关系在所有生活环境中,尤其是治疗环境中发挥治疗作用,因此它与心理治疗特别相关。综合各学科的大部分文献,我们发现了关于同步性的两个隐含假设,并由此形成了两个独立的知识体系:(a)同步性是个人的特质特征;(b)同步性是一种可在实验室中操控的状态现象。本文建议将这两个假设整合为一个全面的理论,从而实现同步研究的个性化。根据该理论,个人具有获得同步的类特质特征,这决定了他们的身心健康,而且这种决定性的现实可以受到类状态的操纵。个体可以偏离其特质特征。当这种偏离趋于正常激活时,心理健康就会得到改善,而这种类似状态的变化被定义为治疗性的。这篇文章呼吁开展研究,调查同步性的特质特征是如何形成的,以及如何通过治疗改变这种特征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"How getting in sync is curative: Insights gained from research in psychotherapy.","authors":"Sigal Zilcha-Mano","doi":"10.1037/rev0000471","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000471","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We are all constantly going in and out of sync with the people we meet in our lives: significant others, incidental encounters, and strangers. Synchrony is a ubiquitous phenomenon, considered an evolution-based mechanism of survival. In recent years, technological development has made it possible to collect much data on synchrony across disciplines. The collected data show great potential to shed light on the benefits of this universal phenomenon. At the same time, mixed results emerged, stressing the need for a theory to navigate research inquiries and discoveries. It is proposed here that synchrony serves as an individual-specific mechanism for making relationships curative in all life circumstances, especially therapeutic ones-hence its special relevance for psychotherapy. A synthesis of the majority of the literature across disciplines reveals two implicit assumptions about synchrony, resulting in two separate bodies of knowledge: (a) synchrony is a trait-like signature characterizing individuals; and (b) synchrony is a state-like phenomenon that can be manipulated in the lab. It is proposed here to personalize synchrony research by integrating the two assumptions into a comprehensive theory according to which individuals have a trait-like signature for getting in sync, which determines their physical and mental health, and that this deterministic reality can be subject to state-like manipulation. Individuals can deviate from their trait-like signature. When the deviation is toward normative activation, mental health improves, and the state-like changes are defined as therapeutic. This article calls for research to investigate how trait-like signature of synchrony develops and how it can be therapeutically changed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1