首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
The interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation. 小组创意构思中的人际神经耦合。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000524
Kelong Lu, Ning Hao

Group creative ideation, the capacity of group to produce novel and useful ideas, is essential for navigating challenges and embracing opportunities. Despite its significance, research to decode its neurocognitive underpinnings utilizing interpersonal neuroscience paradigm has just commenced, linking group creative ideation to interpersonal neural coupling. In this perspective, we propose an interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation framework, which suggests that group creative ideation is supported by interpersonal neural coupling within three interrelated systems: cognitive, affective, and physical alignments. The cognitive alignment system is considered as the core system that determines the outcome of group creative ideation. Variations in cognitive alignment spanning shared intention, joint attention, shared comprehension, and idea convergence interact with an individual decision making in selecting any of three creative ideation pathways, including flexibility, persistence, and convergence, which collectively determine the final creative performance. The interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation framework enhances our understanding of the neurocognitive underpinnings of group creative ideation and outlines promising avenues for future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

群体创造性构思是群体产生新颖和有用想法的能力,对于应对挑战和抓住机遇至关重要。尽管其意义重大,但利用人际神经科学范式解码其神经认知基础的研究刚刚起步,将群体创意构思与人际神经耦合联系起来。在这一视角下,我们提出了群体创意构思中的人际神经耦合框架,认为群体创意构思是由三个相互关联的系统中的人际神经耦合支持的:认知、情感和物理排列。认知排列系统被认为是决定群体创意构思结果的核心系统。共同意向、共同关注、共同理解和想法趋同等认知一致性的变化与个人决策相互作用,使其选择灵活性、持久性和趋同性等三种创意构思途径中的任何一种,从而共同决定最终的创意表现。群体创意构思中的人际神经耦合框架增强了我们对群体创意构思的神经认知基础的理解,并为未来的研究勾勒出了前景广阔的途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation.","authors":"Kelong Lu, Ning Hao","doi":"10.1037/rev0000524","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000524","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Group creative ideation, the capacity of group to produce novel and useful ideas, is essential for navigating challenges and embracing opportunities. Despite its significance, research to decode its neurocognitive underpinnings utilizing interpersonal neuroscience paradigm has just commenced, linking group creative ideation to interpersonal neural coupling. In this perspective, we propose an interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation framework, which suggests that group creative ideation is supported by interpersonal neural coupling within three interrelated systems: cognitive, affective, and physical alignments. The cognitive alignment system is considered as the core system that determines the outcome of group creative ideation. Variations in cognitive alignment spanning shared intention, joint attention, shared comprehension, and idea convergence interact with an individual decision making in selecting any of three creative ideation pathways, including flexibility, persistence, and convergence, which collectively determine the final creative performance. The interpersonal neural coupling in group creative ideation framework enhances our understanding of the neurocognitive underpinnings of group creative ideation and outlines promising avenues for future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142818983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From interoception to control over the internal body: The ideomotor hypothesis of voluntary interoaction. 从内感知到对身体内部的控制:自愿互动的意念运动假说。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000528
Sam Verschooren, Michael Gaebler, Marcel Brass

When it comes to body movements in external space, people are experts in learning fine-grained voluntary control, for example, when manipulating tiny objects. Voluntarily controlling actions in the internal body (e.g., decreasing heart rate), however, is far more difficult and requires dedicated training, for example, in meditation or yoga. Not much is currently known about the learning mechanism underlying the acquisition of voluntary control over internal visceromotor actions (i.e., interoaction) or why it is so difficult compared to controlling our external somatomotor actions (i.e., exteroaction). We propose the ideomotor hypothesis of voluntary interoaction in this article, which asserts that voluntary exteroactions and interoactions are governed by the same general principle, namely, the anticipation of sensory feedback. We apply this hypothesis to two techniques that can be used to acquire voluntary control over interoactions, that is, autogenic training and biofeedback training. As the afferent signal we receive from interoaction (i.e., interoceptive signals from the internal body) is of lower sensory quality than the afferent signal that we receive from exteroaction (i.e., exteroceptive signals from the external environment), this hypothesis explains why learning to control interoactions is more difficult. We propose ways in which to test predictions from this hypothesis and show its theoretical value by comparing it to other frameworks in the literature. We hope that this work motivates future empirical studies directly examining voluntary interoaction and its clinical applications, such as autogenic and biofeedback training, and mind-body practices more generally. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当涉及到外部空间的身体运动时,人们是学习精细自主控制的专家,例如在操纵微小物体时。然而,自愿控制身体内部的活动(例如降低心率)要困难得多,需要专门的训练,例如冥想或瑜伽。目前对于获得对内部内脏运动行为(即相互作用)的自主控制的学习机制知之甚少,或者为什么与控制我们的外部躯体运动行为(即外动作)相比,它是如此困难。在本文中,我们提出了自愿互动的观念运动假说,该假说认为自愿的外在行为和互动受相同的一般原则支配,即对感觉反馈的预期。我们将这一假设应用于两种可用于获得对相互作用的自愿控制的技术,即自体训练和生物反馈训练。由于我们从相互作用中接收的传入信号(即来自内部身体的内感受信号)的感官质量低于我们从外部行为中接收的传入信号(即来自外部环境的外感受信号),这一假设解释了为什么学习控制相互作用更困难。我们提出了一些方法来测试这一假设的预测,并通过将其与文献中的其他框架进行比较来显示其理论价值。我们希望这项工作能激发未来的实证研究,直接检查自愿互动及其临床应用,如自体和生物反馈训练,以及更普遍的身心练习。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"From interoception to control over the internal body: The ideomotor hypothesis of voluntary interoaction.","authors":"Sam Verschooren, Michael Gaebler, Marcel Brass","doi":"10.1037/rev0000528","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000528","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When it comes to body movements in external space, people are experts in learning fine-grained voluntary control, for example, when manipulating tiny objects. Voluntarily controlling actions in the internal body (e.g., decreasing heart rate), however, is far more difficult and requires dedicated training, for example, in meditation or yoga. Not much is currently known about the learning mechanism underlying the acquisition of voluntary control over internal visceromotor actions (i.e., interoaction) or why it is so difficult compared to controlling our external somatomotor actions (i.e., exteroaction). We propose the <i>ideomotor hypothesis of voluntary interoaction</i> in this article, which asserts that voluntary exteroactions and interoactions are governed by the same general principle, namely, the anticipation of sensory feedback. We apply this hypothesis to two techniques that can be used to acquire voluntary control over interoactions, that is, autogenic training and biofeedback training. As the afferent signal we receive from interoaction (i.e., interoceptive signals from the internal body) is of lower sensory quality than the afferent signal that we receive from exteroaction (i.e., exteroceptive signals from the external environment), this hypothesis explains why learning to control interoactions is more difficult. We propose ways in which to test predictions from this hypothesis and show its theoretical value by comparing it to other frameworks in the literature. We hope that this work motivates future empirical studies directly examining voluntary interoaction and its clinical applications, such as autogenic and biofeedback training, and mind-body practices more generally. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142818814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How does depressive cognition develop? A state-dependent network model of predictive processing. 抑郁认知是如何形成的?预测处理的状态依赖网络模型
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000512
Nathaniel Hutchinson-Wong, Paul Glue, Divya Adhia, Dirk de Ridder

Depression is vastly heterogeneous in its symptoms, neuroimaging data, and treatment responses. As such, describing how it develops at the network level has been notoriously difficult. In an attempt to overcome this issue, a theoretical "negative prediction mechanism" is proposed. Here, eight key brain regions are connected in a transient, state-dependent, core network of pathological communication that could facilitate the development of depressive cognition. In the context of predictive processing, it is suggested that this mechanism is activated as a response to negative/adverse stimuli in the external and/or internal environment that exceed a vulnerable individual's capacity for cognitive appraisal. Specifically, repeated activation across this network is proposed to update an individual's brain so that it increasingly predicts and reinforces negative experiences over time-pushing an individual at-risk for or suffering from depression deeper into mental illness. Within this, the negative prediction mechanism is poised to explain various aspects of prognostic outcome, describing how depression might ebb and flow over multiple timescales in a dynamically changing, complex environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

抑郁症在症状、神经影像学数据和治疗反应方面存在巨大差异。因此,从网络层面描述抑郁症如何发展一直是众所周知的难题。为了克服这一问题,我们提出了一种理论上的 "负预测机制"。在这里,八个关键脑区被连接在一个瞬时的、依赖于状态的病理通信核心网络中,该网络可促进抑郁认知的发展。在预测处理方面,该机制被认为是对外部和/或内部环境中超出脆弱个体认知评估能力的负面/不良刺激的一种反应。具体来说,这一网络的反复激活被认为会更新个体的大脑,使其随着时间的推移越来越多地预测和强化负面体验,从而将有抑郁症风险或患有抑郁症的个体推向精神疾病的深渊。在此基础上,负面预测机制可以解释预后结果的各个方面,描述抑郁症如何在动态变化的复杂环境中,在多个时间尺度上起伏不定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How does depressive cognition develop? A state-dependent network model of predictive processing.","authors":"Nathaniel Hutchinson-Wong, Paul Glue, Divya Adhia, Dirk de Ridder","doi":"10.1037/rev0000512","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000512","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Depression is vastly heterogeneous in its symptoms, neuroimaging data, and treatment responses. As such, describing how it develops at the network level has been notoriously difficult. In an attempt to overcome this issue, a theoretical \"negative prediction mechanism\" is proposed. Here, eight key brain regions are connected in a transient, state-dependent, core network of pathological communication that could facilitate the development of depressive cognition. In the context of predictive processing, it is suggested that this mechanism is activated as a response to negative/adverse stimuli in the external and/or internal environment that exceed a vulnerable individual's capacity for cognitive appraisal. Specifically, repeated activation across this network is proposed to update an individual's brain so that it increasingly predicts and reinforces negative experiences over time-pushing an individual at-risk for or suffering from depression deeper into mental illness. Within this, the negative prediction mechanism is poised to explain various aspects of prognostic outcome, describing how depression might ebb and flow over multiple timescales in a dynamically changing, complex environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142627180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The meaning of attention control. 注意力控制的意义
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000514
Klaus Oberauer

Attention control has been proposed as an ability construct that explains individual differences in fluid intelligence. Evaluating this hypothesis is complicated by a lack of clarity in the definition of attention control. Here, I propose a definition of attention control, based on experimental research and computational models of what guides attention, and how cognitive processes are controlled. Attention is the selection of mental representations for prioritized processing, and the ability to control attention is the ability to prioritize those representations that are relevant for the person's current goal, thereby enabling them to think and act in accordance with their intentions. This definition can be used to identify appropriate and less appropriate ways to measure individual differences in attention control. An analysis of various approaches to measurement reveals that the current practice of measuring attention control leaves room for improvement. Aligning our psychometric measurements with a clear, theoretically grounded concept of attention control can lead to more valid measures of that construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有人提出,注意力控制是一种能力结构,可以解释流体智力的个体差异。由于注意力控制的定义不够清晰,评估这一假设变得复杂起来。在此,我根据实验研究和计算模型,提出了注意力控制的定义,即什么会引导注意力,以及认知过程是如何被控制的。注意是选择优先处理的心理表征,而控制注意的能力是优先处理与人当前目标相关的表征的能力,从而使人能够按照自己的意图思考和行动。这一定义可用于确定衡量注意力控制个体差异的合适和不太合适的方法。对各种测量方法的分析表明,目前的注意力控制测量方法还有改进的余地。将我们的心理测量与一个明确的、有理论基础的注意控制概念相统一,可以使我们对该结构的测量更加有效。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The meaning of attention control.","authors":"Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.1037/rev0000514","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000514","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention control has been proposed as an ability construct that explains individual differences in fluid intelligence. Evaluating this hypothesis is complicated by a lack of clarity in the definition of attention control. Here, I propose a definition of attention control, based on experimental research and computational models of what guides attention, and how cognitive processes are controlled. Attention is the selection of mental representations for prioritized processing, and the ability to control attention is the ability to prioritize those representations that are relevant for the person's current goal, thereby enabling them to think and act in accordance with their intentions. This definition can be used to identify appropriate and less appropriate ways to measure individual differences in attention control. An analysis of various approaches to measurement reveals that the current practice of measuring attention control leaves room for improvement. Aligning our psychometric measurements with a clear, theoretically grounded concept of attention control can lead to more valid measures of that construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1509-1526"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142473482","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The dual role of culture for reconstructing early sapiens cognition. 文化对重建早期智人认知的双重作用。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000462
Andrea Bender, Larissa Mendoza Straffon, John B Gatewood, Sieghard Beller

Questions on early sapiens cognition, the cognitive abilities of our ancestors, are intriguing but notoriously hard to tackle. Leaving no hard traces in the archeological record, these abilities need to be inferred from indirect evidence, informed by our understanding of present-day cognition. Most of such attempts acknowledge the role that culture, as a faculty, has played for human evolution, but they underrate or even disregard the role of distinct cultural traditions and the ensuing diversity, both in present-day humans and as a dimension of past cognition. We argue that culture has exerted a profound impact on human cognition from the start in a dual manner: It scaffolds cognition through both development and evolution, and it thereby continually diversifies the form and content of human thinking. To unveil early sapiens cognition and retrace its evolutionary trajectories, this cognitive diversity must be considered. We present two strategies to achieve this: large-scale extrapolation and phylogenetic comparison. The former aims at filtering out diversity to determine what is basic and universal versus culturally shaped (illustrated for theory of mind abilities). The latter capitalizes on the diversity to reconstruct evolutionary trajectories (illustrated for religious beliefs). The two methods, in combination, advance our understanding of the cognitive abilities of our early sapiens ancestors and of how these abilities emerged and evolved. To conclude, we discuss the implications of this approach for our insights into early cognition itself and its scientific investigation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于早期智人的认知,也就是我们祖先的认知能力,这些问题很有意思,但却很难解决。考古记录中没有留下任何确凿的痕迹,这些能力需要我们根据对当今认知的理解,从间接证据中推断出来。大多数此类尝试都承认文化作为一种能力对人类进化所起的作用,但它们低估甚至无视独特文化传统的作用以及随之而来的多样性,无论是在当今人类身上还是作为过去认知的一个维度。我们认为,文化从一开始就以双重方式对人类认知产生了深远影响:它通过发展和进化为认知提供支架,从而不断丰富人类思维的形式和内容。要揭示早期智人的认知并追溯其进化轨迹,就必须考虑这种认知的多样性。为此,我们提出了两种策略:大规模外推法和系统发育比较法。前者旨在筛选出多样性,以确定哪些是基本的、普遍的,哪些是文化塑造的(以心智理论能力为例)。后者利用多样性重建进化轨迹(以宗教信仰为例)。这两种方法结合起来,有助于我们了解早期智人祖先的认知能力,以及这些能力是如何出现和进化的。最后,我们讨论了这种方法对我们了解早期认知本身及其科学研究的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The dual role of culture for reconstructing early sapiens cognition.","authors":"Andrea Bender, Larissa Mendoza Straffon, John B Gatewood, Sieghard Beller","doi":"10.1037/rev0000462","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000462","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Questions on early sapiens cognition, the cognitive abilities of our ancestors, are intriguing but notoriously hard to tackle. Leaving no hard traces in the archeological record, these abilities need to be inferred from indirect evidence, informed by our understanding of present-day cognition. Most of such attempts acknowledge the role that culture, as a faculty, has played for human evolution, but they underrate or even disregard the role of distinct cultural traditions and the ensuing diversity, both in present-day humans and as a dimension of past cognition. We argue that culture has exerted a profound impact on human cognition from the start in a dual manner: It scaffolds cognition through both development and evolution, and it thereby continually diversifies the form and content of human thinking. To unveil early sapiens cognition and retrace its evolutionary trajectories, this cognitive diversity must be considered. We present two strategies to achieve this: large-scale extrapolation and phylogenetic comparison. The former aims at filtering out diversity to determine what is basic and universal versus culturally shaped (illustrated for theory of mind abilities). The latter capitalizes on the diversity to reconstruct evolutionary trajectories (illustrated for religious beliefs). The two methods, in combination, advance our understanding of the cognitive abilities of our early sapiens ancestors and of how these abilities emerged and evolved. To conclude, we discuss the implications of this approach for our insights into early cognition itself and its scientific investigation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1411-1434"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138807774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Violations of transitive preference: A comparison of compensatory and noncompensatory accounts. 违反反式偏好:补偿性和非补偿性解释的比较。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000502
Rob Ranyard, Henry Montgomery, Ashley Luckman, Emmanouil Konstantinidis

Violations of transitive preference can be accounted for by both the noncompensatory lexicographic semiorder heuristic and the compensatory additive difference model. However, the two have not been directly compared. Here, we fully develop a simplified additive difference (SAD) model, which includes a graphical analysis of precisely which parameter values are consistent with adherence to, or violation of, transitive preference, as specified by weak stochastic transitivity (WST) and triangle inequalities (TI). The model is compatible with compensatory, within-dimension evaluation. We also develop a stochastic difference threshold model that also predicts intransitive preferences and encompasses a stochastic lexicographic semiorder model. We apply frequentist methods to compare the goodness of fit of both of these models to Tversky's (1969) data and four replications and Bayes factor methods to determine the strength of evidence for each model. We find that the two methods of analysis converge and that, for two thirds of the participants for whom predictions can be made, one of these models predicting violations of WST has a good and the best fit and has strong Bayesian support relative to an encompassing model. Furthermore, for about 20% of all participants, the SAD model (consistent with violations of WST or TI) is significantly better-fitting and has stronger Bayesian support than the stochastic difference threshold model. Finally, Bayes factor analysis finds strong evidence against transitive models for most participants for whom the SAD model consistent with violation of WST or TI is strongly supported. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

非补偿性的词法半阶启发式和补偿性的加法差分模型都可以解释违反传递偏好的情况。但是,这两种方法还没有进行过直接比较。在这里,我们完全建立了一个简化的加法差分(SAD)模型,其中包括对哪些参数值符合或违反弱随机传递性(WST)和三角形不等式(TI)规定的传递偏好的精确图形分析。该模型与补偿性、维度内评价兼容。我们还建立了一个随机差异阈值模型,它也能预测非传递性偏好,并包含一个随机词典半阶梯模型。我们采用频繁主义方法比较这两个模型与 Tversky(1969)的数据和四次复制的拟合程度,并采用贝叶斯因子方法确定每个模型的证据强度。我们发现,这两种分析方法趋于一致,而且对于三分之二可以进行预测的参与者来说,其中一个预测违反 WST 的模型具有良好和最佳的拟合度,并且相对于包含模型来说,具有很强的贝叶斯支持。此外,对于约 20%的参与者来说,SAD 模型(与违反 WST 或 TI 一致)的拟合效果明显优于随机差异阈值模型,并具有更强的贝叶斯支持。最后,贝叶斯因子分析发现,对于大多数参与者来说,与违反 WST 或 TI 相一致的 SAD 模型得到了强有力的支持,这有力地证明了反转模型的存在。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Violations of transitive preference: A comparison of compensatory and noncompensatory accounts.","authors":"Rob Ranyard, Henry Montgomery, Ashley Luckman, Emmanouil Konstantinidis","doi":"10.1037/rev0000502","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000502","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Violations of transitive preference can be accounted for by both the noncompensatory lexicographic semiorder heuristic and the compensatory additive difference model. However, the two have not been directly compared. Here, we fully develop a simplified additive difference (SAD) model, which includes a graphical analysis of precisely which parameter values are consistent with adherence to, or violation of, transitive preference, as specified by weak stochastic transitivity (WST) and triangle inequalities (TI). The model is compatible with compensatory, within-dimension evaluation. We also develop a stochastic difference threshold model that also predicts intransitive preferences and encompasses a stochastic lexicographic semiorder model. We apply frequentist methods to compare the goodness of fit of both of these models to Tversky's (1969) data and four replications and Bayes factor methods to determine the strength of evidence for each model. We find that the two methods of analysis converge and that, for two thirds of the participants for whom predictions can be made, one of these models predicting violations of WST has a good and the best fit and has strong Bayesian support relative to an encompassing model. Furthermore, for about 20% of all participants, the SAD model (consistent with violations of WST or TI) is significantly better-fitting and has stronger Bayesian support than the stochastic difference threshold model. Finally, Bayes factor analysis finds strong evidence against transitive models for most participants for whom the SAD model consistent with violation of WST or TI is strongly supported. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1392-1410"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142294116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inductive reasoning in minds and machines. 头脑和机器中的归纳推理。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000446
Sudeep Bhatia

Induction-the ability to generalize from existing knowledge-is the cornerstone of intelligence. Cognitive models of human induction are largely limited to toy problems and cannot make quantitative predictions for the thousands of different induction arguments that have been studied by researchers, or to the countless induction arguments that could be encountered in everyday life. Leading large language models (LLMs) go beyond toy problems but fail to mimic observed patterns of human induction. In this article, we combine rich knowledge representations obtained from LLMs with theories of human inductive reasoning developed by cognitive psychologists. We show that this integrative approach can capture several benchmark empirical findings on human induction and generate human-like responses to natural language arguments with thousands of common categories and properties. These findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms at play in human induction and show how existing theories in psychology and cognitive science can be integrated with new methods in artificial intelligence, to successfully model high-level human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

归纳——从现有知识中归纳的能力是智力的基石。人类归纳的认知模型在很大程度上仅限于玩具问题,无法对研究人员研究的数千种不同的归纳论点或日常生活中可能遇到的无数归纳论点做出定量预测。领先的大型语言模型(LLM)超越了玩具问题,但未能模仿观察到的人类归纳模式。在这篇文章中,我们将从LLM中获得的丰富的知识表示与认知心理学家发展的人类归纳推理理论相结合。我们表明,这种综合方法可以捕捉到关于人类归纳的几个基准经验发现,并对具有数千个常见类别和属性的自然语言论点产生类似人类的反应。这些发现揭示了人类诱导过程中的认知机制,并展示了心理学和认知科学中的现有理论如何与人工智能中的新方法相结合,以成功地模拟人类的高级认知。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Inductive reasoning in minds and machines.","authors":"Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/rev0000446","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000446","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Induction-the ability to generalize from existing knowledge-is the cornerstone of intelligence. Cognitive models of human induction are largely limited to toy problems and cannot make quantitative predictions for the thousands of different induction arguments that have been studied by researchers, or to the countless induction arguments that could be encountered in everyday life. Leading large language models (LLMs) go beyond toy problems but fail to mimic observed patterns of human induction. In this article, we combine rich knowledge representations obtained from LLMs with theories of human inductive reasoning developed by cognitive psychologists. We show that this integrative approach can capture several benchmark empirical findings on human induction and generate human-like responses to natural language arguments with thousands of common categories and properties. These findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms at play in human induction and show how existing theories in psychology and cognitive science can be integrated with new methods in artificial intelligence, to successfully model high-level human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1373-1391"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41165565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Advancing the network theory of mental disorders: A computational model of panic disorder. 推进精神障碍的网络理论:惊恐障碍的计算模型。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000515
Donald J Robinaugh, Jonas M B Haslbeck, Lourens J Waldorp, Jolanda J Kossakowski, Eiko I Fried, Alexander J Millner, Richard J McNally, Oisín Ryan, Jill de Ron, Han L J van der Maas, Egbert H van Nes, Marten Scheffer, Kenneth S Kendler, Denny Borsboom

The network theory of psychopathology posits that mental disorders are systems of mutually reinforcing symptoms. This framework has proven highly generative but does not specify precisely how any specific mental disorder operates as such a system. Cognitive behavioral theories of mental disorders provide considerable insight into how these systems may operate. However, the development of cognitive behavioral theories has itself been stagnant in recent years. In this article, we advance both theoretical frameworks by developing a network theory of panic disorder rooted in cognitive behavioral theory and formalized as a computational model. We use this computational model to evaluate the theory's ability to explain five fundamental panic disorder-related phenomena. Our results demonstrate that the network theory of panic disorder can explain core panic disorder phenomena. In addition, by formalizing this theory as a computational model and using the model to evaluate the theory's implications, we reveal gaps in the empirical literature and shortcomings in theories of panic disorder. We use these limitations to develop a novel, theory-driven agenda for panic disorder research. This agenda departs from current research practices and places its focus on (a) addressing areas in need of more rigorous descriptive research, (b) investigating novel phenomena predicted by the computational model, and (c) ongoing collaborative development of formal theories of panic disorder, with explanation as a central criterion for theory evaluation. We conclude with a discussion of the implications of this work for research investigating mental disorders as complex systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

精神病理学的网络理论认为,精神障碍是症状相互强化的系统。这一框架已被证明是高度生成的,但并没有精确地说明任何特定的精神障碍是如何作为这样一个系统运作的。精神障碍的认知行为理论为这些系统如何运作提供了相当深入的见解。然而,近年来认知行为理论本身的发展却停滞不前。在这篇文章中,我们通过发展一个基于认知行为理论并形式化为计算模型的恐慌症网络理论来推进这两个理论框架。我们使用这个计算模型来评估该理论解释五种与恐慌症相关的基本现象的能力。研究结果表明,惊恐障碍的网络理论可以解释惊恐障碍的核心现象。此外,通过将该理论形式化为计算模型并使用该模型评估该理论的含义,我们揭示了经验文献中的空白和恐慌障碍理论的缺陷。我们利用这些限制为恐慌症研究制定了一个新颖的、理论驱动的议程。该议程偏离了当前的研究实践,并将其重点放在(a)解决需要更严格的描述性研究的领域,(b)调查计算模型预测的新现象,以及(c)正在进行的恐慌障碍正式理论的合作发展,并将解释作为理论评估的中心标准。最后,我们讨论了这项工作对研究精神障碍作为复杂系统的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Advancing the network theory of mental disorders: A computational model of panic disorder.","authors":"Donald J Robinaugh, Jonas M B Haslbeck, Lourens J Waldorp, Jolanda J Kossakowski, Eiko I Fried, Alexander J Millner, Richard J McNally, Oisín Ryan, Jill de Ron, Han L J van der Maas, Egbert H van Nes, Marten Scheffer, Kenneth S Kendler, Denny Borsboom","doi":"10.1037/rev0000515","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000515","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The network theory of psychopathology posits that mental disorders are systems of mutually reinforcing symptoms. This framework has proven highly generative but does not specify precisely how any specific mental disorder operates as such a system. Cognitive behavioral theories of mental disorders provide considerable insight into how these systems may operate. However, the development of cognitive behavioral theories has itself been stagnant in recent years. In this article, we advance both theoretical frameworks by developing a network theory of panic disorder rooted in cognitive behavioral theory and formalized as a computational model. We use this computational model to evaluate the theory's ability to explain five fundamental panic disorder-related phenomena. Our results demonstrate that the network theory of panic disorder can explain core panic disorder phenomena. In addition, by formalizing this theory as a computational model and using the model to evaluate the theory's implications, we reveal gaps in the empirical literature and shortcomings in theories of panic disorder. We use these limitations to develop a novel, theory-driven agenda for panic disorder research. This agenda departs from current research practices and places its focus on (a) addressing areas in need of more rigorous descriptive research, (b) investigating novel phenomena predicted by the computational model, and (c) ongoing collaborative development of formal theories of panic disorder, with explanation as a central criterion for theory evaluation. We conclude with a discussion of the implications of this work for research investigating mental disorders as complex systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":"131 6","pages":"1482-1508"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142882590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamic retrieval of events and associations from memory: An integrated account of item and associative recognition. 从记忆中动态检索事件和联想:项目识别和联想识别的综合说明。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000486
Gregory E Cox

Memory theories distinguish between item and associative information, which are engaged by different tasks: item recognition uses item information to decide whether an event occurred in a particular context; associative recognition uses associative information to decide whether two events occurred together. Associative recognition is slower and less accurate than item recognition, suggesting that item and associative information may be represented in different forms and retrieved using different processes. Instead, I show how a dynamic model (Cox & Criss, 2020; Cox & Shiffrin, 2017) accounts for accuracy and response time distributions in both item and associative recognition with the same set of representations and processes. Item and associative information are both represented as vectors of features. Item and associative recognition both depend on comparing traces in memory with probes of memory in which item and associative features gradually accumulate. Associative features are slower to accumulate, but largely because they emerge from conjunctions of already-accumulated item features. I apply the model to data from 453 participants, each of whom performed an item and performed associative recognition following identical study conditions (Cox et al., 2018). Comparisons among restricted versions of the model show that its account of associative feature formation, coupled with limits on the rate at which features accumulate from multiple items, explains how and why the dynamics of associative recognition differ from those of item recognition even while both tasks rely on the same underlying representations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆理论将项目信息和联想信息区分开来,这两种信息被不同的任务所使用:项目识别使用项目信息来判断一个事件是否在特定环境中发生;联想识别使用联想信息来判断两个事件是否一起发生。与项目识别相比,联想识别的速度更慢,准确性也更低,这表明项目信息和联想信息可能以不同的形式表示,并使用不同的过程进行检索。相反,我展示了一个动态模型(Cox & Criss, 2020; Cox & Shiffrin, 2017)是如何用同一套表征和过程来解释项目识别和联想识别的准确率和反应时间分布的。项目信息和联想信息都表示为特征向量。项目识别和联想识别都依赖于将记忆中的痕迹与记忆探针进行比较,在记忆探针中,项目特征和联想特征会逐渐积累。联想特征的积累速度较慢,但这主要是因为它们是由已经积累的项目特征组合而成的。我将该模型应用于来自 453 名参与者的数据,他们每人都在相同的研究条件下进行了项目识别和联想识别(Cox 等人,2018 年)。该模型的限制性版本之间的比较表明,该模型对联想特征形成的解释,加上对多个项目特征积累速度的限制,解释了联想识别的动态如何以及为什么与项目识别的动态不同,即使这两种任务都依赖于相同的底层表征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Dynamic retrieval of events and associations from memory: An integrated account of item and associative recognition.","authors":"Gregory E Cox","doi":"10.1037/rev0000486","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000486","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Memory theories distinguish between item and associative information, which are engaged by different tasks: item recognition uses item information to decide whether an event occurred in a particular context; associative recognition uses associative information to decide whether two events occurred together. Associative recognition is slower and less accurate than item recognition, suggesting that item and associative information may be represented in different forms and retrieved using different processes. Instead, I show how a dynamic model (Cox & Criss, 2020; Cox & Shiffrin, 2017) accounts for accuracy and response time distributions in both item and associative recognition with the same set of representations and processes. Item and associative information are both represented as vectors of features. Item and associative recognition both depend on comparing traces in memory with probes of memory in which item and associative features gradually accumulate. Associative features are slower to accumulate, but largely because they emerge from conjunctions of already-accumulated item features. I apply the model to data from 453 participants, each of whom performed an item and performed associative recognition following identical study conditions (Cox et al., 2018). Comparisons among restricted versions of the model show that its account of associative feature formation, coupled with limits on the rate at which features accumulate from multiple items, explains how and why the dynamics of associative recognition differ from those of item recognition even while both tasks rely on the same underlying representations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1297-1336"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141760645","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A flexible threshold theory of change perception in self, others, and the world. 关于自我、他人和世界变化感知的灵活阈值理论。
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000490
Ed O'Brien

I propose a flexible threshold theory of change perception in self and social judgment. Traditionally, change perception is viewed as a basic cognitive process entailing the act of discriminating informational differences. This article takes a more dynamic view of change perception, highlighting people's motivations in interpreting those differences. Specifically, I propose people's change perceptions depend not only on the salience and quality of the evidence for change but they also depend on the adaptation implications of the change, as people are sensitive to whether their prompted response would be worth it. Variables that exacerbate perceived adaptation implications should thus lead people to contract their change perception thresholds (people should become less open to concluding things have changed and so less likely to act), while variables that alleviate perceived adaptation implications should thus lead people to expand their change perception thresholds (people should become more open to concluding things have changed and so more likely to act), all else equal in the evidence. Moreover, these effects should emerge for perceiving declines and improvements alike so long as change bears on adaptation implications. I review support for these proposals and use the theory to generate novel predictions, contributions, and applications. The theory can explain anew why people respond (or fail to respond) to changing climates and economies, worsening personal health, growing social progress, and many other self and social phenomena. Change perception is more than an act of discriminating differences-it also entails people's threshold judgments of whether and how these differences matter. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我提出了一种关于自我和社会判断中变化感知的灵活阈值理论。传统上,变化感知被视为一种基本的认知过程,包含对信息差异的辨别行为。本文从更动态的角度来看待变化感知,强调人们在解释这些差异时的动机。具体来说,我认为人们的变化感知不仅取决于变化证据的显著性和质量,而且还取决于变化的适应影响,因为人们对自己的提示反应是否值得很敏感。因此,在其他证据相同的情况下,加剧所感知的适应影响的变量应导致人们收缩其变化感知阈值(人们应更不愿意得出事情已经发生变化的结论,因此更不可能采取行动),而减轻所感知的适应影响的变量应导致人们扩大其变化感知阈值(人们应更愿意得出事情已经发生变化的结论,因此更可能采取行动)。此外,只要变化对适应有影响,这些效应就会出现在对下降和改善的感知上。我回顾了对这些建议的支持,并利用该理论提出了新的预测、贡献和应用。该理论可以重新解释为什么人们会对不断变化的气候和经济、不断恶化的个人健康、不断增长的社会进步以及许多其他自我和社会现象做出反应(或不做出反应)。变化感知不仅仅是一种辨别差异的行为--它还包括人们对这些差异是否重要以及如何重要的阈值判断。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A flexible threshold theory of change perception in self, others, and the world.","authors":"Ed O'Brien","doi":"10.1037/rev0000490","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000490","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>I propose a flexible threshold theory of change perception in self and social judgment. Traditionally, change perception is viewed as a basic cognitive process entailing the act of discriminating informational differences. This article takes a more dynamic view of change perception, highlighting people's motivations in interpreting those differences. Specifically, I propose people's change perceptions depend not only on the salience and quality of the evidence for change but they also depend on the adaptation implications of the change, as people are sensitive to whether their prompted response would be worth it. Variables that exacerbate perceived adaptation implications should thus lead people to contract their change perception thresholds (people should become less open to concluding things have changed and so less likely to act), while variables that alleviate perceived adaptation implications should thus lead people to expand their change perception thresholds (people should become more open to concluding things have changed and so more likely to act), all else equal in the evidence. Moreover, these effects should emerge for perceiving declines and improvements alike so long as change bears on adaptation implications. I review support for these proposals and use the theory to generate novel predictions, contributions, and applications. The theory can explain anew why people respond (or fail to respond) to changing climates and economies, worsening personal health, growing social progress, and many other self and social phenomena. Change perception is more than an act of discriminating differences-it also entails people's threshold judgments of whether and how these differences matter. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1435-1458"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142294104","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1