首页 > 最新文献

Psychological review最新文献

英文 中文
Differentiating mental models of self and others: A hierarchical framework for knowledge assessment. 区分自我和他人的心智模式:知识评估的分层框架
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000443
Aakriti Kumar, Padhraic Smyth, Mark Steyvers

Developing an accurate model of another agent's knowledge is central to communication and cooperation between agents. In this article, we propose a hierarchical framework of knowledge assessment that explains how people construct mental models of their own knowledge and the knowledge of others. Our framework posits that people integrate information about their own and others' knowledge via Bayesian inference. To evaluate this claim, we conduct an experiment in which participants repeatedly assess their own performance (a metacognitive task) and the performance of another person (a type of theory of mind task) on the same image classification tasks. We contrast the hierarchical framework with simpler alternatives that assume different degrees of differentiation between mental models of self and others. Our model accurately captures participants' assessment of their own performance and the performance of others in the task: Initially, people rely on their own self-assessment process to reason about the other person's performance, leading to similar self- and other-performance predictions. As more information about the other person's ability becomes available, the mental model for the other person becomes increasingly distinct from the mental model of self. Simulation studies also confirm that our framework explains a wide range of findings about human knowledge assessment of themselves and others. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

建立另一个代理的准确知识模型是代理之间进行交流与合作的核心。在本文中,我们提出了一个知识评估的分层框架,解释了人们如何构建自己和他人知识的心智模型。我们的框架认为,人们通过贝叶斯推理整合自己和他人的知识信息。为了评估这一观点,我们进行了一项实验,让参与者在相同的图像分类任务中反复评估自己的表现(元认知任务)和他人的表现(一种心智理论任务)。我们将分层框架与假设自我和他人心智模型之间存在不同程度差异的更简单的替代方法进行了对比。我们的模型准确地捕捉到了参与者在任务中对自己和他人表现的评估:最初,人们依靠自己的自我评估过程来推理他人的表现,从而得出相似的自我和他人表现预测。随着有关他人能力的信息越来越多,他人的心智模型与自我的心智模型就会越来越不同。模拟研究还证实,我们的框架可以解释人类对自己和他人的知识评估的一系列发现。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Differentiating mental models of self and others: A hierarchical framework for knowledge assessment.","authors":"Aakriti Kumar, Padhraic Smyth, Mark Steyvers","doi":"10.1037/rev0000443","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000443","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Developing an accurate model of another agent's knowledge is central to communication and cooperation between agents. In this article, we propose a hierarchical framework of knowledge assessment that explains how people construct mental models of their own knowledge and the knowledge of others. Our framework posits that people integrate information about their own and others' knowledge via Bayesian inference. To evaluate this claim, we conduct an experiment in which participants repeatedly assess their own performance (a metacognitive task) and the performance of another person (a type of theory of mind task) on the same image classification tasks. We contrast the hierarchical framework with simpler alternatives that assume different degrees of differentiation between mental models of self and others. Our model accurately captures participants' assessment of their own performance and the performance of others in the task: Initially, people rely on their own self-assessment process to reason about the other person's performance, leading to similar self- and other-performance predictions. As more information about the other person's ability becomes available, the mental model for the other person becomes increasingly distinct from the mental model of self. Simulation studies also confirm that our framework explains a wide range of findings about human knowledge assessment of themselves and others. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1566-1591"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10014347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Discounting and the portfolio of desires. 折扣和欲望组合。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000447
Peter R Killeen

The additive utility theory of discounting is extended to probability and commodity discounting. Because the utility of a good and the disutility of its delay combine additively, increases in the utility of a good offset the disutility of its delay: Increasing the former slows the apparent discount even with the latter, time-disutility, remaining invariant, giving the magnitude effect. Conjoint measurement showed the subjective value of money to be a logarithmic function of its amount, and subjective probability-the probability weighting function-to be Prelec's (1998). This general theory of discounting (GTD) explains why large amounts are probability discounted more quickly, giving the negative magnitude effect. Whatever enhances the value of a delayed asset, such as its ability to satisfy diverse desires, offsets its delay and reduces discounting. Money's liquidity permits optimization of the portfolio of desired goods, providing added value that accounts for its shallow temporal discount gradient. GTD predicts diversification effects for delay but none for probability discounting. Operations such as episodic future thinking that increase the larder of potential expenditures-the portfolio of desirable goods-increase the value of the asset, flattening the discount gradient. States that decrease the larder, such as stress, depression, and overweening focus on a single substance like a drug, constrict the portfolio, decreasing its utility and thereby steepening the gradient. GTD provides a unified account of delay, probability, and cross-commodity discounting. It explains the effects of motivational states, dispositions, and cognitive manipulations on discount gradients. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

将折扣的加性效用理论推广到概率和商品折扣。由于商品的效用和其延迟的不效用相加,商品效用的增加抵消了其延迟的非效用:增加前者会减缓表观折扣,即使后者,时间不效用保持不变,也会产生幅度效应。联合测量表明,货币的主观价值是其金额的对数函数,主观概率是Prelec(1998)的概率加权函数。这种一般的贴现理论(GTD)解释了为什么大额贴现的概率更快,从而产生负幅度效应。任何能提高延迟资产价值的东西,比如它满足各种欲望的能力,都会抵消其延迟并减少折扣。货币的流动性允许对所需商品的投资组合进行优化,从而提供附加值,从而解释其短暂的贴现梯度。GTD预测了延迟的分散效应,但没有预测概率贴现的分散效应。情景式未来思维等操作增加了潜在支出的储藏室——理想商品的投资组合增加了资产的价值,使贴现梯度变平。减少储藏室的州,如压力、抑郁和过度关注药物等单一物质,会限制投资组合,降低其效用,从而使梯度变陡。GTD提供了延迟、概率和跨商品折扣的统一说明。它解释了动机状态、倾向和认知操作对折扣梯度的影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Discounting and the portfolio of desires.","authors":"Peter R Killeen","doi":"10.1037/rev0000447","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000447","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The additive utility theory of discounting is extended to probability and commodity discounting. Because the utility of a good and the disutility of its delay combine additively, increases in the utility of a good offset the disutility of its delay: Increasing the former slows the apparent discount even with the latter, time-disutility, remaining invariant, giving the magnitude effect. Conjoint measurement showed the subjective value of money to be a logarithmic function of its amount, and subjective probability-the probability weighting function-to be Prelec's (1998). This general theory of discounting (GTD) explains why large amounts are probability discounted more quickly, giving the negative magnitude effect. Whatever enhances the value of a delayed asset, such as its ability to satisfy diverse desires, offsets its delay and reduces discounting. Money's liquidity permits optimization of the portfolio of desired goods, providing added value that accounts for its shallow temporal discount gradient. GTD predicts diversification effects for delay but none for probability discounting. Operations such as episodic future thinking that increase the larder of potential expenditures-the portfolio of desirable goods-increase the value of the asset, flattening the discount gradient. States that decrease the larder, such as stress, depression, and overweening focus on a single substance like a drug, constrict the portfolio, decreasing its utility and thereby steepening the gradient. GTD provides a unified account of delay, probability, and cross-commodity discounting. It explains the effects of motivational states, dispositions, and cognitive manipulations on discount gradients. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1310-1325"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10236092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Perception and simulation during concept learning. 概念学习过程中的感知和模拟。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-13 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000433
Erik Weitnauer, Robert L Goldstone, Helge Ritter
A key component of humans' striking creativity in solving problems is our ability to construct novel descriptions to help us characterize novel concepts. Bongard problems (BPs), which challenge the problem solver to come up with a rule for distinguishing visual scenes that fall into two categories, provide an elegant test of this ability. BPs are challenging for both human and machine category learners because only a handful of example scenes are presented for each category, and they often require the open-ended creation of new descriptions. A new type of BP called physical Bongard problems (PBPs) is introduced, which requires solvers to perceive and predict the physical spatial dynamics implicit in the depicted scenes. The perceiving and testing hypotheses on structures (PATHS) computational model, which can solve many PBPs, is presented and compared to human performance on the same problems. PATHS and humans are similarly affected by the ordering of scenes within a PBP. Spatially or temporally juxtaposing similar (relative to dissimilar) scenes promotes category learning when the scenes belong to different categories but hinders learning when the similar scenes belong to the same category. The core theoretical commitments of PATHS, which we believe to also exemplify open-ended human category learning, are (a) the continual perception of new scene descriptions over the course of category learning; (b) the context-dependent nature of that perceptual process, in which the perceived scenes establish the context for the perception of subsequent scenes; (c) hypothesis construction by combining descriptions into explicit rules; and (d) bidirectional interactions between perceiving new aspects of scenes and constructing hypotheses for the rule that distinguishes categories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
人类在解决问题方面惊人创造力的一个关键组成部分是我们构建新颖描述的能力,以帮助我们描述新颖的概念。Bongard问题(BP)向问题解决者提出了一个区分分为两类视觉场景的规则,它为这种能力提供了一个优雅的测试。BP对人类和机器类别学习者来说都是具有挑战性的,因为每个类别只呈现少数示例场景,而且它们通常需要开放式创建新的描述。介绍了一种称为物理Bongard问题(PBPs)的新型BP,它要求求解者感知和预测所描绘场景中隐含的物理空间动力学。提出了可以解决许多PBP的结构感知和测试假设(PATHS)计算模型,并将其与人类在相同问题上的表现进行了比较。PATHS和人类同样会受到PBP中场景顺序的影响。当场景属于不同类别时,在空间或时间上并置相似(相对于不相似)场景促进了类别学习,但当相似场景属于同一类别时,阻碍了学习。PATHS的核心理论承诺是(a)在类别学习过程中对新场景描述的持续感知;(b) 感知过程的上下文相关性质,其中感知的场景为后续场景的感知建立了上下文;(c) 通过将描述组合成显式规则来构建假设;以及(d)感知场景的新方面和为区分类别的规则构建假设之间的双向交互。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Perception and simulation during concept learning.","authors":"Erik Weitnauer,&nbsp;Robert L Goldstone,&nbsp;Helge Ritter","doi":"10.1037/rev0000433","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000433","url":null,"abstract":"A key component of humans' striking creativity in solving problems is our ability to construct novel descriptions to help us characterize novel concepts. Bongard problems (BPs), which challenge the problem solver to come up with a rule for distinguishing visual scenes that fall into two categories, provide an elegant test of this ability. BPs are challenging for both human and machine category learners because only a handful of example scenes are presented for each category, and they often require the open-ended creation of new descriptions. A new type of BP called physical Bongard problems (PBPs) is introduced, which requires solvers to perceive and predict the physical spatial dynamics implicit in the depicted scenes. The perceiving and testing hypotheses on structures (PATHS) computational model, which can solve many PBPs, is presented and compared to human performance on the same problems. PATHS and humans are similarly affected by the ordering of scenes within a PBP. Spatially or temporally juxtaposing similar (relative to dissimilar) scenes promotes category learning when the scenes belong to different categories but hinders learning when the similar scenes belong to the same category. The core theoretical commitments of PATHS, which we believe to also exemplify open-ended human category learning, are (a) the continual perception of new scene descriptions over the course of category learning; (b) the context-dependent nature of that perceptual process, in which the perceived scenes establish the context for the perception of subsequent scenes; (c) hypothesis construction by combining descriptions into explicit rules; and (d) bidirectional interactions between perceiving new aspects of scenes and constructing hypotheses for the rule that distinguishes categories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1203-1238"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9774634","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Overprecision is a property of thinking systems. 过度背诵是思维系统的一种特性。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-05-05 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000370
Don A Moore

Overprecision is the excessive certainty in the accuracy of one's judgment. This article proposes a new theory to explain it. The theory holds that overprecision in judgment results from neglect of all the ways in which one could be wrong. When there are many ways to be wrong, it can be difficult to consider them all. Overprecision is the result of being wrong and not knowing it. This explanation can account for why question formats have such a dramatic influence on the degree of overprecision people report. It also explains the ubiquity of overprecision not only among people but also among artificially intelligent agents. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

过度精确是指一个人判断的准确性过于确定。本文提出了一个新的理论来解释它。该理论认为,判断中的过度复述是由于忽视了所有可能出错的方面。当有很多错误的时候,很难把它们都考虑在内。过度背诵是错误和不知道的结果。这一解释可以解释为什么问题格式对人们报告的过度背诵程度有如此巨大的影响。这也解释了过度背诵不仅在人中普遍存在,而且在人工智能体中也普遍存在。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Overprecision is a property of thinking systems.","authors":"Don A Moore","doi":"10.1037/rev0000370","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Overprecision is the excessive certainty in the accuracy of one's judgment. This article proposes a new theory to explain it. The theory holds that overprecision in judgment results from neglect of all the ways in which one could be wrong. When there are many ways to be wrong, it can be difficult to consider them all. Overprecision is the result of being wrong and not knowing it. This explanation can account for why question formats have such a dramatic influence on the degree of overprecision people report. It also explains the ubiquity of overprecision not only among people but also among artificially intelligent agents. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":"130 5","pages":"1339-1350"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49681699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
The environmental basis of memory. 记忆的环境基础。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-22 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000409
John R Anderson, Shawn Betts, Michael D Byrne, Lael J Schooler, Clayton Stanley

Memory should make more available things that are more likely to be needed. Across multiple environmental domains, it has been shown that such a system would match qualitatively the memory effects involving repetition, delay, and spacing (Schooler & Anderson, 2017). To obtain data of sufficient size to study how detailed patterns of past appearance predict probability of being needed again, we examined the patterns with which words appear in large two data sets: tweets from popular sources and comments on popular subreddits. The two data sets show remarkably similar statistics, which are also consistent with earlier, smaller studies of environmental statistics. None of a candidate set of mathematical models of memory do well at predicting the observed patterns in these environments. A new model of human memory based on the environmental model proposed by Anderson and Milson (1989) did better at predicting the environmental data and a wide range of behavioral studies that measure memory availability by probability of recall and speed of retrieval. A critical variable in this model was range, the span of time over which an item occurs, which was discovered in mining the environmental data. These results suggest that theories of memory can be guided by mining of the statistical structure of the environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆应该使更多的东西变得可用,而这些东西更有可能是需要的。在多个环境领域中,研究表明,这种系统在质量上与涉及重复、延迟和间隔的记忆效应相匹配(Schooler&Anderson,2017)。为了获得足够大的数据来研究过去出现的详细模式如何预测再次被需要的概率,我们研究了单词出现在两个大数据集中的模式:来自流行来源的推文和流行子版块上的评论。这两个数据集显示出非常相似的统计数据,这也与早期较小的环境统计研究一致。没有一组候选的记忆数学模型能很好地预测这些环境中观察到的模式。Anderson和Milson(1989)在环境模型的基础上提出了一种新的人类记忆模型,该模型在预测环境数据和通过回忆概率和检索速度来衡量记忆可用性的广泛行为研究方面做得更好。该模型中的一个关键变量是范围,即项目发生的时间跨度,这是在挖掘环境数据时发现的。这些结果表明,记忆理论可以通过挖掘环境的统计结构来指导。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The environmental basis of memory.","authors":"John R Anderson,&nbsp;Shawn Betts,&nbsp;Michael D Byrne,&nbsp;Lael J Schooler,&nbsp;Clayton Stanley","doi":"10.1037/rev0000409","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000409","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Memory should make more available things that are more likely to be needed. Across multiple environmental domains, it has been shown that such a system would match qualitatively the memory effects involving repetition, delay, and spacing (Schooler & Anderson, 2017). To obtain data of sufficient size to study how detailed patterns of past appearance predict probability of being needed again, we examined the patterns with which words appear in large two data sets: tweets from popular sources and comments on popular subreddits. The two data sets show remarkably similar statistics, which are also consistent with earlier, smaller studies of environmental statistics. None of a candidate set of mathematical models of memory do well at predicting the observed patterns in these environments. A new model of human memory based on the environmental model proposed by Anderson and Milson (1989) did better at predicting the environmental data and a wide range of behavioral studies that measure memory availability by probability of recall and speed of retrieval. A critical variable in this model was range, the span of time over which an item occurs, which was discovered in mining the environmental data. These results suggest that theories of memory can be guided by mining of the statistical structure of the environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1137-1166"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9754966","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A process model of mindsets: Conceptualizing mindsets of ability as dynamic and socially situated. 心态的过程模型:将能力的心态概念化为动态和社会情境。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-06 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000425
Naomi M P de Ruiter, Sander Thomaes

Mindsets of ability (i.e., "fixed" and "growth" mindsets) play a pivotal role in students' academic trajectories. However, relatively little is known about the mechanisms underlying mindset development. Identifying these mechanisms is vital for understanding, and potentially influencing, how mindsets emerge and change over time. In this article, we formulate a comprehensive theoretical model that purports to account for the emergence and development of ability mindsets: the process model of mindsets (PMM). The PMM is rooted in complex dynamic systems and enactive perspectives, which allow for conceptualizing psychological phenomena as dynamic and socially situated. The PMM accounts for how mindset-related behaviors, action tendencies, beliefs, and social interactions can become codependent and robust over time. We discuss how the model helps to further our understanding of the efficacy of mindset interventions and the heterogeneity thereof. The PMM has a broad explanatory scope, is generative, and paves the way for future process studies of mindsets and mindset interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

能力心态(即“固定”和“成长”心态)在学生的学术轨迹中发挥着关键作用。然而,人们对心态发展的基本机制知之甚少。识别这些机制对于理解并潜在地影响心态如何随着时间的推移而出现和变化至关重要。在这篇文章中,我们建立了一个全面的理论模型,旨在解释能力心态的产生和发展:心态的过程模型(PMM)。PMM植根于复杂的动态系统和行为视角,允许将心理现象概念化为动态和社会情境。PMM解释了与心态相关的行为、行动倾向、信念和社交互动如何随着时间的推移变得相互依赖和强大。我们讨论了该模型如何帮助我们进一步理解心态干预的有效性及其异质性。PMM具有广泛的解释范围,具有生成性,为未来心态和心态干预的过程研究铺平了道路。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A process model of mindsets: Conceptualizing mindsets of ability as dynamic and socially situated.","authors":"Naomi M P de Ruiter,&nbsp;Sander Thomaes","doi":"10.1037/rev0000425","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000425","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mindsets of ability (i.e., \"fixed\" and \"growth\" mindsets) play a pivotal role in students' academic trajectories. However, relatively little is known about the mechanisms underlying mindset development. Identifying these mechanisms is vital for understanding, and potentially influencing, how mindsets emerge and change over time. In this article, we formulate a comprehensive theoretical model that purports to account for the emergence and development of ability mindsets: the process model of mindsets (PMM). The PMM is rooted in complex dynamic systems and enactive perspectives, which allow for conceptualizing psychological phenomena as dynamic and socially situated. The PMM accounts for how mindset-related behaviors, action tendencies, beliefs, and social interactions can become codependent and robust over time. We discuss how the model helps to further our understanding of the efficacy of mindset interventions and the heterogeneity thereof. The PMM has a broad explanatory scope, is generative, and paves the way for future process studies of mindsets and mindset interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1326-1338"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10821257","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word meaning is both categorical and continuous. 词义既是范畴的又是连续的。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-09 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000420
Sean Trott, Benjamin Bergen

Most words have multiple meanings, but there are foundationally distinct accounts for this. Categorical theories posit that humans maintain discrete entries for distinct word meanings, as in a dictionary. Continuous ones eschew discrete sense representations, arguing that word meanings are best characterized as trajectories through a continuous state space. Both kinds of approach face empirical challenges. In response, we introduce two novel "hybrid" theories, which reconcile discrete sense representations with a continuous view of word meaning. We then report on two behavioral experiments, pairing them with an analytical approach relying on neural language models to test these competing accounts. The experimental results are best explained by one of the novel hybrid accounts, which posits both distinct sense representations and a continuous meaning space. This hybrid account accommodates both the dynamic, context-dependent nature of word meaning, as well as the behavioral evidence for category-like structure in human lexical knowledge. We further develop and quantify the predictive power of several computational implementations of this hybrid account. These results raise questions for future research on lexical ambiguity, such as why and when discrete sense representations might emerge in the first place. They also connect to more general questions about the role of discrete versus gradient representations in cognitive processes and suggest that at least in this case, the best explanation is one that integrates both factors: Word meaning is both categorical and continuous. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

大多数单词都有多种含义,但对此有一些基本上不同的解释。分类理论认为,人类为不同的单词含义保留离散的条目,就像在字典中一样。连续的概念避开了离散的意义表示,认为单词含义最好表征为通过连续状态空间的轨迹。这两种方法都面临着经验上的挑战。作为回应,我们介绍了两种新颖的“混合”理论,它们调和了离散的意义表征和对词义的连续看法。然后,我们报告了两个行为实验,将它们与依赖神经语言模型的分析方法配对,以测试这些相互竞争的账户。实验结果最好用一种新颖的混合叙述来解释,该叙述假设了不同的意义表征和连续的意义空间。这种混合解释既考虑了词义的动态性、上下文依赖性,也考虑了人类词汇知识中类结构的行为证据。我们进一步开发并量化了这种混合账户的几种计算实现的预测能力。这些结果为未来的词汇歧义研究提出了问题,比如为什么以及何时会出现离散的意义表征。它们还涉及到关于离散表征与梯度表征在认知过程中的作用的更一般的问题,并表明至少在这种情况下,最好的解释是综合这两个因素的解释:词义既是范畴的,也是连续的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Word meaning is both categorical and continuous.","authors":"Sean Trott,&nbsp;Benjamin Bergen","doi":"10.1037/rev0000420","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000420","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most words have multiple meanings, but there are foundationally distinct accounts for this. Categorical theories posit that humans maintain discrete entries for distinct word meanings, as in a dictionary. Continuous ones eschew discrete sense representations, arguing that word meanings are best characterized as trajectories through a continuous state space. Both kinds of approach face empirical challenges. In response, we introduce two novel \"hybrid\" theories, which reconcile discrete sense representations with a continuous view of word meaning. We then report on two behavioral experiments, pairing them with an analytical approach relying on neural language models to test these competing accounts. The experimental results are best explained by one of the novel hybrid accounts, which posits both distinct sense representations and a continuous meaning space. This hybrid account accommodates both the dynamic, context-dependent nature of word meaning, as well as the behavioral evidence for category-like structure in human lexical knowledge. We further develop and quantify the predictive power of several computational implementations of this hybrid account. These results raise questions for future research on lexical ambiguity, such as why and when discrete sense representations might emerge in the first place. They also connect to more general questions about the role of discrete versus gradient representations in cognitive processes and suggest that at least in this case, the best explanation is one that integrates both factors: Word meaning is both categorical and continuous. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1239-1261"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10011855","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognition and self-control: An integrative framework. 元认知与自我控制:一个综合框架。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-15 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000406
Marie Hennecke, Sebastian Bürgler

Self-control describes the processes by which individuals control their habits, desires, and impulses in the service of long-term goals. Research has identified important components of self-control and proposed theoretical frameworks integrating these components (e.g., Inzlicht et al., 2021; Kotabe & Hofmann, 2015). In our perspective, these frameworks, however, do not yet fully incorporate important metacognitive aspects of self-control. We therefore introduce a framework explicating the role of metacognition for self-control. This framework extends existing frameworks, primarily from the domains of self-regulated learning and problem-solving (e.g., Schraw & Moshman, 1995; Zimmerman, 2000), and integrates past and contemporary research and theorizing on self-control that involves aspects of metacognition. It considers two groups of metacognitive components, namely, (a) individual metacognitive characteristics, that is a person's declarative, procedural, and conditional metacognitive knowledge about self-control, as well as their self-awareness (or metacognitive awareness), and (b) metacognitive regulatory processes that unfold before a self-control conflict (forethought and prevention), when a self-control conflict is identified, during a self-control conflict (regulation and monitoring), and after a self-control conflict (reflection and evaluation). The proposed framework integrates existing research and will be useful for highlighting new directions for research on the role of metacognition for self-control success and failure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

自我控制描述了个人为实现长期目标而控制自己的习惯、欲望和冲动的过程。研究已经确定了自我控制的重要组成部分,并提出了整合这些组成部分的理论框架(例如,Inzlicht等人,2021;Kotabe和Hofmann,2015)。然而,在我们看来,这些框架还没有完全纳入自我控制的重要元认知方面。因此,我们引入了一个框架来解释元认知在自我控制中的作用。该框架扩展了现有的框架,主要来自自我调节学习和解决问题的领域(例如,Schraw&Moshman,1995;Zimmerman,2000),并整合了过去和当代关于自我控制的研究和理论,涉及元认知的各个方面。它考虑了两组元认知成分,即(a)个体元认知特征,即一个人关于自我控制的陈述性、程序性和条件性元认知知识,以及他们的自我意识(或元认知意识),以及(b)在自我控制冲突之前展开的元认知调节过程(预先思考和预防),当自我控制冲突被识别时,在自我控制冲突期间(监管和监控),以及在自我控制矛盾之后(反思和评估)。所提出的框架整合了现有的研究,将有助于突出元认知在自我控制成功和失败中的作用研究的新方向。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Metacognition and self-control: An integrative framework.","authors":"Marie Hennecke,&nbsp;Sebastian Bürgler","doi":"10.1037/rev0000406","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000406","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-control describes the processes by which individuals control their habits, desires, and impulses in the service of long-term goals. Research has identified important components of self-control and proposed theoretical frameworks integrating these components (e.g., Inzlicht et al., 2021; Kotabe & Hofmann, 2015). In our perspective, these frameworks, however, do not yet fully incorporate important <i>metacognitive</i> aspects of self-control. We therefore introduce a framework explicating the role of metacognition for self-control. This framework extends existing frameworks, primarily from the domains of self-regulated learning and problem-solving (e.g., Schraw & Moshman, 1995; Zimmerman, 2000), and integrates past and contemporary research and theorizing on self-control that involves aspects of metacognition. It considers two groups of metacognitive components, namely, (a) <i>individual metacognitive characteristics</i>, that is a person's declarative, procedural, and conditional metacognitive knowledge about self-control, as well as their self-awareness (or metacognitive awareness), and (b) <i>metacognitive regulatory processes</i> that unfold before a self-control conflict (forethought and prevention), when a self-control conflict is identified, during a self-control conflict (regulation and monitoring), and after a self-control conflict (reflection and evaluation). The proposed framework integrates existing research and will be useful for highlighting new directions for research on the role of metacognition for self-control success and failure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1262-1288"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10348541","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Further perceptions of probability: In defence of associative models. 概率的进一步认知:为关联模型辩护。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-12 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000410
Mattias Forsgren, Peter Juslin, Ronald van den Berg

Extensive research in the behavioral sciences has addressed people's ability to learn stationary probabilities, which stay constant over time, but only recently have there been attempts to model the cognitive processes whereby people learn-and track-nonstationary probabilities. In this context, the old debate on whether learning occurs by the gradual formation of associations or by occasional shifts between hypotheses representing beliefs about distal states of the world has resurfaced. Gallistel et al. (2014) pitched the two theories against each other in a nonstationary probability learning task. They concluded that various qualitative patterns in their data were incompatible with trial-by-trial associative learning and could only be explained by a hypothesis-testing model. Here, we contest that claim and demonstrate that it was premature. First, we argue that their experimental paradigm consisted of two distinct tasks: probability tracking (an estimation task) and change detection (a decision-making task). Next, we present a model that uses the (associative) delta learning rule for the probability tracking task and bounded evidence accumulation for the change detection task. We find that this combination of two highly established theories accounts well for all qualitative phenomena and outperforms the alternative model proposed by Gallistel et al. (2014) in a quantitative model comparison. In the spirit of cumulative science, we conclude that current experimental data on human learning of nonstationary probabilities can be explained as a combination of associative learning and bounded evidence accumulation and does not require a new model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

行为科学的广泛研究已经解决了人们学习平稳概率的能力,这种概率随着时间的推移保持不变,但直到最近才有人试图对人们学习和跟踪非平稳概率的认知过程进行建模。在这种背景下,关于学习是通过逐渐形成联想发生的,还是通过代表对世界遥远状态的信念的假设之间的偶尔转变发生的,这一古老的争论再次浮出水面。Gallistel等人(2014)在非平稳概率学习任务中将这两种理论对立起来。他们得出结论,他们数据中的各种定性模式与逐个试验的联想学习不兼容,只能通过假设检验模型来解释。在这里,我们对这一说法提出质疑,并证明这还为时过早。首先,我们认为他们的实验范式由两个不同的任务组成:概率跟踪(一种估计任务)和变化检测(一种决策任务)。接下来,我们提出了一个模型,该模型将(关联)delta学习规则用于概率跟踪任务,将有界证据累积用于变化检测任务。我们发现,这两种高度成熟的理论的结合很好地解释了所有定性现象,并在定量模型比较中优于Gallistel等人提出的替代模型。(2014)。本着累积科学的精神,我们得出结论,当前关于人类非平稳概率学习的实验数据可以解释为联想学习和有界证据积累的结合,不需要新的模型。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Further perceptions of probability: In defence of associative models.","authors":"Mattias Forsgren,&nbsp;Peter Juslin,&nbsp;Ronald van den Berg","doi":"10.1037/rev0000410","DOIUrl":"10.1037/rev0000410","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extensive research in the behavioral sciences has addressed people's ability to learn stationary probabilities, which stay constant over time, but only recently have there been attempts to model the cognitive processes whereby people learn-and track-<i>nonstationary</i> probabilities. In this context, the old debate on whether learning occurs by the gradual formation of associations or by occasional shifts between hypotheses representing beliefs about distal states of the world has resurfaced. Gallistel et al. (2014) pitched the two theories against each other in a nonstationary probability learning task. They concluded that various qualitative patterns in their data were incompatible with trial-by-trial associative learning and could only be explained by a hypothesis-testing model. Here, we contest that claim and demonstrate that it was premature. First, we argue that their experimental paradigm consisted of two distinct tasks: probability tracking (an estimation task) and change detection (a decision-making task). Next, we present a model that uses the (associative) delta learning rule for the probability tracking task and bounded evidence accumulation for the change detection task. We find that this combination of two highly established theories accounts well for all qualitative phenomena and outperforms the alternative model proposed by Gallistel et al. (2014) in a quantitative model comparison. In the spirit of cumulative science, we conclude that current experimental data on human learning of nonstationary probabilities can be explained as a <i>combination</i> of associative learning and bounded evidence accumulation and does not require a new model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":"1383-1400"},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10525389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inductive reasoning in minds and machines. 头脑和机器中的归纳推理。
IF 5.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/rev0000446
Sudeep Bhatia

Induction-the ability to generalize from existing knowledge-is the cornerstone of intelligence. Cognitive models of human induction are largely limited to toy problems and cannot make quantitative predictions for the thousands of different induction arguments that have been studied by researchers, or to the countless induction arguments that could be encountered in everyday life. Leading large language models (LLMs) go beyond toy problems but fail to mimic observed patterns of human induction. In this article, we combine rich knowledge representations obtained from LLMs with theories of human inductive reasoning developed by cognitive psychologists. We show that this integrative approach can capture several benchmark empirical findings on human induction and generate human-like responses to natural language arguments with thousands of common categories and properties. These findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms at play in human induction and show how existing theories in psychology and cognitive science can be integrated with new methods in artificial intelligence, to successfully model high-level human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

归纳——从现有知识中归纳的能力是智力的基石。人类归纳的认知模型在很大程度上仅限于玩具问题,无法对研究人员研究的数千种不同的归纳论点或日常生活中可能遇到的无数归纳论点做出定量预测。领先的大型语言模型(LLM)超越了玩具问题,但未能模仿观察到的人类归纳模式。在这篇文章中,我们将从LLM中获得的丰富的知识表示与认知心理学家发展的人类归纳推理理论相结合。我们表明,这种综合方法可以捕捉到关于人类归纳的几个基准经验发现,并对具有数千个常见类别和属性的自然语言论点产生类似人类的反应。这些发现揭示了人类诱导过程中的认知机制,并展示了心理学和认知科学中的现有理论如何与人工智能中的新方法相结合,以成功地模拟人类的高级认知。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Inductive reasoning in minds and machines.","authors":"Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/rev0000446","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000446","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Induction-the ability to generalize from existing knowledge-is the cornerstone of intelligence. Cognitive models of human induction are largely limited to toy problems and cannot make quantitative predictions for the thousands of different induction arguments that have been studied by researchers, or to the countless induction arguments that could be encountered in everyday life. Leading large language models (LLMs) go beyond toy problems but fail to mimic observed patterns of human induction. In this article, we combine rich knowledge representations obtained from LLMs with theories of human inductive reasoning developed by cognitive psychologists. We show that this integrative approach can capture several benchmark empirical findings on human induction and generate human-like responses to natural language arguments with thousands of common categories and properties. These findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms at play in human induction and show how existing theories in psychology and cognitive science can be integrated with new methods in artificial intelligence, to successfully model high-level human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":21016,"journal":{"name":"Psychological review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.4,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41165565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Psychological review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1