首页 > 最新文献

Timing & Time Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Laypeople’s Beliefs Affect their Reports about the Subjective Experience of Time 外行人的信仰影响他们对时间主观体验的报告
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2019-01-11 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181140
Yee Mun Lee, S. Janssen
Because the general population may be familiar with the phenomenon that life appears to speed up as people become older, participants’ preconceptions may affect how they answer questionnaires about the subjective experience of time. To be able to account for these preconceptions in future research, we assessed laypeople’s beliefs about the phenomenon. Participants (N = 313) were asked whether they were familiar with the phenomenon, whether they experienced the phenomenon themselves, and what they thought that the cause or causes of the phenomenon might be. More than 80% of the participants had read or heard about the phenomenon prior to the study, suggesting that the phenomenon is well known among the general population. Furthermore, although most participants experienced the phenomenon themselves, familiarity with the phenomenon affected whether they felt that life appeared to be speeding up and whether time passed fast for them. Familiarity also affected whether participants attributed the phenomenon to changes in objective or subjective time but not the endorsement of the phenomenon’s causes. Finally, participants also had preconceptions about what time periods represent ‘the present’ and ‘the past’. Whereas nearly all participants considered the past to have lasted more than one year, two-third of the participants felt that the present represented a period less than one year.
由于普通人群可能熟悉随着年龄的增长,生活似乎会加快的现象,参与者的先入为主的观念可能会影响他们如何回答关于时间主观体验的问卷。为了能够在未来的研究中解释这些先入为主的观点,我们评估了外行对这一现象的看法。参与者(N=313)被问及他们是否熟悉这一现象,他们自己是否经历过这一现象以及他们认为这一现象的原因可能是什么。超过80%的参与者在研究之前阅读或听说过这种现象,这表明这种现象在普通人群中是众所周知的。此外,尽管大多数参与者自己也经历过这种现象,但对这种现象的熟悉程度会影响他们是否觉得生活似乎在加速,以及时间对他们来说是否过得很快。熟悉程度也会影响参与者是否将这种现象归因于客观或主观时间的变化,而不是对现象原因的认可。最后,参与者对什么时间段代表“现在”和“过去”也有先入为主的看法。尽管几乎所有参与者都认为过去持续了一年以上,但三分之二的参与者认为现在的时间不到一年。
{"title":"Laypeople’s Beliefs Affect their Reports about the Subjective Experience of Time","authors":"Yee Mun Lee, S. Janssen","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181140","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181140","url":null,"abstract":"Because the general population may be familiar with the phenomenon that life appears to speed up as people become older, participants’ preconceptions may affect how they answer questionnaires about the subjective experience of time. To be able to account for these preconceptions in future research, we assessed laypeople’s beliefs about the phenomenon. Participants (N = 313) were asked whether they were familiar with the phenomenon, whether they experienced the phenomenon themselves, and what they thought that the cause or causes of the phenomenon might be. More than 80% of the participants had read or heard about the phenomenon prior to the study, suggesting that the phenomenon is well known among the general population. Furthermore, although most participants experienced the phenomenon themselves, familiarity with the phenomenon affected whether they felt that life appeared to be speeding up and whether time passed fast for them. Familiarity also affected whether participants attributed the phenomenon to changes in objective or subjective time but not the endorsement of the phenomenon’s causes. Finally, participants also had preconceptions about what time periods represent ‘the present’ and ‘the past’. Whereas nearly all participants considered the past to have lasted more than one year, two-third of the participants felt that the present represented a period less than one year.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2019-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43254227","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Age-Related Differences in Time-Based Event Expectancies 基于时间的事件预期的年龄相关差异
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2019-01-11 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181123
M. Kunchulia, K. Parkosadze, R. Thomaschke
The ability to form time-based event expectancies is one of the most important determinants of anticipative behavior. The aim of the present study was to determine whether healthy aging influences the formation of time-based event expectancies. Ten older adults with ages ranging between 60 and 73 years and ten younger adults with ages ranging between 20 and 32 years participated. We employed a binary choice response task mimicking a computer game, in which two target stimuli and two pre-target intervals appeared overall equally often. One of the targets was paired with the short interval and the other target with the long interval in 80% of the trials. Our results showed that younger adults responded more rapidly to frequent interval–target combinations than to infrequent combinations, suggesting that the young participants formed time-based event expectancies. In contrast, the ability to form time-based event expectancies was reduced for older participants. The formation of time-based event expectancies seems to change during healthy aging. We propose that this age-related difference is due to age-related expectation deficits or a reduction of attentional capacities, rather than to deficits in timing abilities.
形成基于时间的事件预期的能力是预期行为最重要的决定因素之一。本研究的目的是确定健康老龄化是否影响基于时间的事件预期的形成。10名年龄在60至73岁之间的老年人和10名年龄在20至32岁之间的年轻人参与了这项研究。我们采用了一个模拟电脑游戏的二元选择反应任务,在这个任务中,两个目标刺激和两个预目标间隔出现的频率大致相同。在80%的试验中,一个目标与短间隔配对,另一个目标与长间隔配对。我们的研究结果表明,年轻的成年人对频繁的间隔目标组合比不频繁的组合反应更快,这表明年轻的参与者形成了基于时间的事件预期。相比之下,年龄较大的参与者形成基于时间的事件预期的能力有所下降。在健康老龄化过程中,基于时间的事件预期的形成似乎发生了变化。我们认为,这种与年龄相关的差异是由于与年龄相关的期望缺陷或注意能力的降低,而不是由于时间能力的缺陷。
{"title":"Age-Related Differences in Time-Based Event Expectancies","authors":"M. Kunchulia, K. Parkosadze, R. Thomaschke","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181123","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181123","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to form time-based event expectancies is one of the most important determinants of anticipative behavior. The aim of the present study was to determine whether healthy aging influences the formation of time-based event expectancies. Ten older adults with ages ranging between 60 and 73 years and ten younger adults with ages ranging between 20 and 32 years participated. We employed a binary choice response task mimicking a computer game, in which two target stimuli and two pre-target intervals appeared overall equally often. One of the targets was paired with the short interval and the other target with the long interval in 80% of the trials. Our results showed that younger adults responded more rapidly to frequent interval–target combinations than to infrequent combinations, suggesting that the young participants formed time-based event expectancies. In contrast, the ability to form time-based event expectancies was reduced for older participants. The formation of time-based event expectancies seems to change during healthy aging. We propose that this age-related difference is due to age-related expectation deficits or a reduction of attentional capacities, rather than to deficits in timing abilities.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2019-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44148263","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
TRF1: It Was the Best of Time(s)… TRF1:那是最好的时光……
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-12-08 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-00603001
A. Giersch, J. Coull
{"title":"TRF1: It Was the Best of Time(s)…","authors":"A. Giersch, J. Coull","doi":"10.1163/22134468-00603001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00603001","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42032092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Impatience in Timing Decisions: Effects and Moderation 时间决策中的急躁:影响与节制
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-07-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181118
M. Ghafurian, David T. Reitter
Decisions on when to act are critical in many health care, safety and security situations, where acting too early or too late can both lead to huge costs or losses. In this paper, impatience is investigated as a bias affecting timing decisions, and is successfully manipulated and moderated. Experiment 1 (N = 123) shows that in different tasks with the same duration, participants perform better when acting early is advantageous, as compared to when acting late is. Experiment 2 (N = 701) manipulates impatience and shows that impatience induced by delays (a) affects timing decisions in the subsequent tasks, (b) increases a tendency to receive information faster, only for a few seconds, with cost and no gain, and (c) reduces satisfaction in the subsequent task. Furthermore, impatience is significantly moderated by showing fast countdowns during the delays. Experiment 3 (N = 304) shows that the mechanism behind this impatience moderation is altered time perception and presents trade-offs between duration perception and duration recall.
在许多医疗保健、安全和安保情况下,何时采取行动的决定至关重要,过早或太迟都可能导致巨大的成本或损失。在本文中,不耐烦被研究为影响时间决策的一种偏见,并被成功地操纵和调节。实验1(N=123)表明,在相同持续时间的不同任务中,与晚作相比,早作有利时参与者表现更好。实验2(N=701)操纵了不耐烦,并表明延迟引起的不耐烦(a)影响后续任务的时间决策,(b)增加了更快地接收信息的趋势,只持续几秒钟,并且(c)降低对后续任务的满意度。此外,通过在延迟期间显示快速倒计时,可以显著缓解不耐烦。实验3(N=304)表明,这种不耐烦调节背后的机制是改变的时间感知,并在持续时间感知和持续时间回忆之间进行权衡。
{"title":"Impatience in Timing Decisions: Effects and Moderation","authors":"M. Ghafurian, David T. Reitter","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181118","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181118","url":null,"abstract":"Decisions on when to act are critical in many health care, safety and security situations, where acting too early or too late can both lead to huge costs or losses. In this paper, impatience is investigated as a bias affecting timing decisions, and is successfully manipulated and moderated. Experiment 1 (N = 123) shows that in different tasks with the same duration, participants perform better when acting early is advantageous, as compared to when acting late is. Experiment 2 (N = 701) manipulates impatience and shows that impatience induced by delays (a) affects timing decisions in the subsequent tasks, (b) increases a tendency to receive information faster, only for a few seconds, with cost and no gain, and (c) reduces satisfaction in the subsequent task. Furthermore, impatience is significantly moderated by showing fast countdowns during the delays. Experiment 3 (N = 304) shows that the mechanism behind this impatience moderation is altered time perception and presents trade-offs between duration perception and duration recall.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-20181118","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47999301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Retrospective Temporal Judgment of the Period Dedicated to Recalling a Recent or an Old Emotional Memory 回忆最近或过去情感记忆时期的回顾性时间判断
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-07-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181128
S. Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, G. Mioni, A. Morin, Félix Désautels, Nicolas Bisson
Sixty-one participants were asked (a) to recall a memory for a period lasting 15 minutes and (b), at the end of this period, to estimate retrospectively the duration of this period. They were assigned to one of four groups: the memory was either joyful or sad, and was recent (within the past two years) or old (when the participant was 7 to 10 years old). The most critical finding is the demonstration that the age of the recalled memory has an impact on the verbal estimation. More specifically, duration is underestimated in the old but not in the recent memory condition. Moreover, in this study, recalling a memory, old or recent, is shown to be an efficient way to generate a joyful or sad emotion. Finally, the results also indicate that there is a significant correlation between the uncertainty related to the duration estimated retrospectively and the score on the present-hedonistic scale of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory.
61名参与者被要求(a)回忆一段持续15分钟的记忆,(b)在这段时间结束时,回顾性地估计这段时间的持续时间。他们被分为四组:记忆是快乐的或悲伤的,是最近的(在过去两年内)或年老的(当参与者7到10岁时)。最关键的发现是,回忆记忆的年龄对言语估计有影响。更具体地说,在过去的记忆条件下,持续时间被低估了,但在最近的记忆条件中却没有。此外,在这项研究中,回忆过去或最近的记忆被证明是产生快乐或悲伤情绪的有效方式。最后,结果还表明,与回顾性估计的持续时间相关的不确定性与津巴多时间透视量表的当前享乐量表得分之间存在显著相关性。
{"title":"Retrospective Temporal Judgment of the Period Dedicated to Recalling a Recent or an Old Emotional Memory","authors":"S. Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, G. Mioni, A. Morin, Félix Désautels, Nicolas Bisson","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181128","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181128","url":null,"abstract":"Sixty-one participants were asked (a) to recall a memory for a period lasting 15 minutes and (b), at the end of this period, to estimate retrospectively the duration of this period. They were assigned to one of four groups: the memory was either joyful or sad, and was recent (within the past two years) or old (when the participant was 7 to 10 years old). The most critical finding is the demonstration that the age of the recalled memory has an impact on the verbal estimation. More specifically, duration is underestimated in the old but not in the recent memory condition. Moreover, in this study, recalling a memory, old or recent, is shown to be an efficient way to generate a joyful or sad emotion. Finally, the results also indicate that there is a significant correlation between the uncertainty related to the duration estimated retrospectively and the score on the present-hedonistic scale of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-20181128","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48365722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Illusory Flow and Passage of Time within Consciousness: A Multidisciplinary Analysis 意识中虚幻的流动和时间的流逝:多学科分析
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-07-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-2018E001
R. P. Gruber, Ryan P. Smith, R. Block
Flow and passage of time puzzles were analyzed by first clarifying their roles in the current multidisciplinary understanding of time in consciousness. All terms ( flow, passage, happening, becoming) are carefully defined. Flow and passage are defined differently, the former involving the psychological aspects of time and the latter involving the evolving universe and associated new cerebral events. The concept of the flow of time (FOT) is deconstructed into two levels: (a) a lower level ― a perceptual dynamic flux, or happening, or flow of events (not time); and (b) an upper level ― a cognitive view of past/present/future in which the observer seems to move from one to the other. With increasing evidence that all perception is a discrete continuity provided by illusory perceptual completion, the lower-level FOT is essentially the result of perceptual completion. The brain conflates the expression flow (passage, for some) of time with experiences of perceptual completion. However, this is an illusory percept. Converging evidence on the upper-level FOT reveals it as a false cognition that has the illusory percept of object persistence as its prerequisite. To research this argument, an experiment that temporarily removes the experience of the lower-level FOT might be conducted. The claustrum of the brain (arguably the center of consciousness) should be intermittently stimulated to create a scenario of discrete observations (involving all the senses) with long interstimulus intervals of non-consciousness and thereby no perceptual completion. Without perceptual completion, there should be no subjective experience of the lower-level FOT.
通过首先阐明它们在当前意识中对时间的多学科理解中的作用,分析了时间的流动和流逝谜题。所有条款( 流动、通过、发生、成为)都是经过仔细定义的。流动和通过的定义不同,前者涉及时间的心理方面,后者涉及不断进化的宇宙和相关的新大脑事件。时间流(FOT)的概念被解构为两个层次:(a)较低层次——感知的动态流动,或正在发生,或事件的流动(而不是时间);以及(b)更高层次——观察者似乎从一个到另一个的过去/现在/未来的认知观。随着越来越多的证据表明,所有感知都是由虚幻的感知完成提供的离散连续性,较低级别的FOT本质上是感知完成的结果。大脑将时间的表达流(对一些人来说是流逝)与感知完成的体验混为一谈。然而,这是一种虚幻的感觉。在上层FOT上聚合证据表明,它是一种以对象持久性的虚幻感知为前提的错误认知。为了研究这一论点,可以进行一项实验,暂时消除较低级别FOT的经验。大脑的幽闭(可以说是意识的中心)应该被间歇性地刺激,以创造一个离散观察(涉及所有感官)的场景,其中有很长的无意识间隙,因此没有感知完成。没有感性的完成,就不应该有较低层次FOT的主观体验。
{"title":"The Illusory Flow and Passage of Time within Consciousness: A Multidisciplinary Analysis","authors":"R. P. Gruber, Ryan P. Smith, R. Block","doi":"10.1163/22134468-2018E001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-2018E001","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Flow and passage of time puzzles were analyzed by first clarifying their roles in the current multidisciplinary understanding of time in consciousness. All terms ( flow, passage, happening, becoming) are carefully defined. Flow and passage are defined differently, the former involving the psychological aspects of time and the latter involving the evolving universe and associated new cerebral events. The concept of the flow of time (FOT) is deconstructed into two levels: (a) a lower level ― a perceptual dynamic flux, or happening, or flow of events (not time); and (b) an upper level ― a cognitive view of past/present/future in which the observer seems to move from one to the other. With increasing evidence that all perception is a discrete continuity provided by illusory perceptual completion, the lower-level FOT is essentially the result of perceptual completion. The brain conflates the expression flow (passage, for some) of time with experiences of perceptual completion. However, this is an illusory percept. Converging evidence on the upper-level FOT reveals it as a false cognition that has the illusory percept of object persistence as its prerequisite. To research this argument, an experiment that temporarily removes the experience of the lower-level FOT might be conducted. The claustrum of the brain (arguably the center of consciousness) should be intermittently stimulated to create a scenario of discrete observations (involving all the senses) with long interstimulus intervals of non-consciousness and thereby no perceptual completion. Without perceptual completion, there should be no subjective experience of the lower-level FOT.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-2018E001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41776547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Increased Frustration Predicts the Experience of Time Slowing-Down: Evidence from an Experience Sampling Study 增加的挫败感预示着时间减慢的经历:来自经验抽样研究的证据
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-07-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181134
J. Tipples
Recent experience sampling research supports the idea that our experience of time speeds up when we are happy and slows down when we feel sad. However, this research had only examined a single negative mood state namely, sadness. Here, I extend this research by testing whether the experience of time speeding-up and slowing down is associated with other thoughts and negative mood states. Thirty-nine participants aged from 18 to 29 completed an experience sampling procedure that lasted for five consecutive days. The experience sampling procedure included measures of time experience (passage of time judgements), mood, levels of activity and time orientation. Increased frustration predicted the experience of time slowing down more than sadness and increased activity, thinking about the future and to a lesser extent happiness, predicted time moving more quickly. Implications of the findings are discussed in relation to laboratory-based studies of time perception.
最近的经验抽样研究支持这样一种观点,即当我们快乐时,我们对时间的体验会加快,而当我们感到悲伤时,我们对时间的体验会减慢。然而,这项研究只研究了一种消极情绪状态,即悲伤。在这里,我通过测试时间加速和减慢的体验是否与其他想法和消极情绪状态有关来扩展这项研究。39名年龄在18岁到29岁之间的参与者完成了一个连续五天的经验抽样程序。经验抽样程序包括时间经验(时间判断的流逝)、情绪、活动水平和时间取向的测量。与悲伤相比,沮丧感的增加更能预测时间流逝的速度,而活动的增加、对未来的思考以及幸福感的增加(在较小程度上)更能预测时间流逝的速度。研究结果的含义讨论了相关的实验室为基础的研究的时间感知。
{"title":"Increased Frustration Predicts the Experience of Time Slowing-Down: Evidence from an Experience Sampling Study","authors":"J. Tipples","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181134","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181134","url":null,"abstract":"Recent experience sampling research supports the idea that our experience of time speeds up when we are happy and slows down when we feel sad. However, this research had only examined a single negative mood state namely, sadness. Here, I extend this research by testing whether the experience of time speeding-up and slowing down is associated with other thoughts and negative mood states. Thirty-nine participants aged from 18 to 29 completed an experience sampling procedure that lasted for five consecutive days. The experience sampling procedure included measures of time experience (passage of time judgements), mood, levels of activity and time orientation. Increased frustration predicted the experience of time slowing down more than sadness and increased activity, thinking about the future and to a lesser extent happiness, predicted time moving more quickly. Implications of the findings are discussed in relation to laboratory-based studies of time perception.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-20181134","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46052388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 17
Spatial Representation of Time in Backspace 时间在Backspace中的空间表示
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2018-07-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-20181120
Nicole Schwan, P. Brugger, E. Huberle
Temporal information, numerical magnitude and space extension appear to share common representational mechanisms and be processed similarly in the brain. Evidence comes from the phenomenon of ‘pseudoneglect’, i.e. healthy persons’ orientation asymmetry toward the left side of space. Pseudoneglect is also evident along the mental number line which extends from small numbers on the left to large numbers on the right. In analogy to numbers, time is typically represented on a line extending from the left to the right side. It may thus be no surprise that pseudoneglect has been demonstrated in the temporal domain as well. Besides the perception of the space located anteriorly to our trunk (frontspace), we are able to represent the space behind us, which we cannot visually perceive (backspace). The translational model suggests a mapping of spatially defined information to the ipsilateral side of the egocentric reference frame in front- and backspace, while the rotational concept focuses on a 360° spatial representation around the midsagittal plane of the trunk. At the present stage of investigation, little is known about the representation of temporal information in backspace. In an attempt to fill this gap, we compared duration estimations of auditory stimuli in frontspace and backspace. Healthy right-handers were instructed to judge their duration relative to each other. We found a pseudoneglect-behavior not only in frontspace but also in backspace. The data are discussed in the context of common processing mechanisms for time, numbers and space and favor a translational over a rotational account for the representation of backspace. The results are further discussed with reference to potential consequences for the rehabilitation of hemispatial neglect.
时间信息、数值幅度和空间扩展似乎共享共同的表征机制,并在大脑中进行类似的处理。证据来自“伪忽视”现象,即健康人朝向空间左侧的方向不对称。伪忽略在心理数字线上也很明显,从左边的小数字延伸到右边的大数字。与数字类似,时间通常表示在从左向右延伸的线上。因此,在时域中也证明了伪忽视也就不足为奇了。除了对位于我们躯干前方的空间(前空间)的感知外,我们还能够表示我们身后的空间,而我们无法视觉感知(后空间)。平移模型建议将空间定义的信息映射到前后空间中自我中心参考系的同侧,而旋转概念侧重于躯干中矢面周围的360°空间表示。在目前的研究阶段,人们对后空间中时间信息的表示知之甚少。为了填补这一空白,我们比较了前空间和后空间听觉刺激的持续时间估计。健康的右投手被要求判断他们的持续时间。我们发现了一种伪忽视行为,不仅在前空间,而且在后空间。这些数据是在时间、数字和空间的常见处理机制的背景下讨论的,并且有利于对后空间的表示进行平移而非旋转。该结果将进一步讨论,以参考半空间忽视康复的潜在后果。
{"title":"Spatial Representation of Time in Backspace","authors":"Nicole Schwan, P. Brugger, E. Huberle","doi":"10.1163/22134468-20181120","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20181120","url":null,"abstract":"Temporal information, numerical magnitude and space extension appear to share common representational mechanisms and be processed similarly in the brain. Evidence comes from the phenomenon of ‘pseudoneglect’, i.e. healthy persons’ orientation asymmetry toward the left side of space. Pseudoneglect is also evident along the mental number line which extends from small numbers on the left to large numbers on the right. In analogy to numbers, time is typically represented on a line extending from the left to the right side. It may thus be no surprise that pseudoneglect has been demonstrated in the temporal domain as well. Besides the perception of the space located anteriorly to our trunk (frontspace), we are able to represent the space behind us, which we cannot visually perceive (backspace). The translational model suggests a mapping of spatially defined information to the ipsilateral side of the egocentric reference frame in front- and backspace, while the rotational concept focuses on a 360° spatial representation around the midsagittal plane of the trunk. At the present stage of investigation, little is known about the representation of temporal information in backspace. In an attempt to fill this gap, we compared duration estimations of auditory stimuli in frontspace and backspace. Healthy right-handers were instructed to judge their duration relative to each other. We found a pseudoneglect-behavior not only in frontspace but also in backspace. The data are discussed in the context of common processing mechanisms for time, numbers and space and favor a translational over a rotational account for the representation of backspace. The results are further discussed with reference to potential consequences for the rehabilitation of hemispatial neglect.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2018-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-20181120","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44161668","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Reward Contrast Effects on Impulsive Choice and Timing in Rats. 奖励对比对大鼠冲动选择和时机的影响。
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2016-06-10 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-00002059
Aaron P. Smith, J. R. Peterson, Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick
Despite considerable interest in impulsive choice as a predictor of a variety of maladaptive behaviors, the mechanisms that drive choice behavior are still poorly understood. The present study sought to examine the influence of one understudied variable, reward magnitude contrast, on choice and timing behavior as changes in magnitude commonly occur within choice procedures. In addition, assessments of indirect effects on choice behavior through magnitude-timing interactions were assessed by measuring timing within the choice task. Rats were exposed to choice procedures composed of different pairs of magnitudes of rewards for either the smaller-sooner (SS) or larger-later (LL) option. In Phase 2, the magnitude of reward either increased or decreased by 1 pellet in different groups (LL increase = 1v1→1v2; SS decrease = 2v2 → 1v2; SS increase = 1v2 → 2v2), followed by a return to baseline in Phase 3. Choice behavior was affected by the initial magnitudes experienced in the task, demonstrating a strong anchor effect. The nature of the change in magnitude affected choice behavior as well. Timing behavior was also affected by the reward contrast manipulation albeit to a lesser degree and the timing and choice effects were correlated. The results suggest that models of choice behavior should incorporate reinforcement history, reward contrast elements, and magnitude-timing interactions, but that direct effects of reward contrast on choice should be given more weight than the indirect reward-timing interactions. A better understanding of the factors that contribute to choice behavior could supply key insights into this important individual differences variable.
尽管人们对冲动性选择作为各种适应不良行为的预测因素有相当大的兴趣,但驱动选择行为的机制仍然知之甚少。本研究试图检验一个未被充分研究的变量——奖励大小对比——对选择和选择时机行为的影响,因为大小的变化通常发生在选择过程中。此外,通过测量选择任务内的时间,评估了通过量级-时间相互作用对选择行为的间接影响。大鼠被暴露在由不同量级的奖励对组成的选择程序中,无论是小早(SS)还是大晚(LL)选项。在第二阶段,不同组的奖励大小增加或减少1个颗粒(LL增加= 1v1→1v2;SS减小= 2v2→1v2;SS增加= 1v2→2v2),然后在第3阶段恢复到基线。选择行为受到任务初始值的影响,表现出强烈的锚效应。大小变化的性质也会影响选择行为。时机行为也受到奖励对比操纵的影响,尽管影响程度较小,但时机效应和选择效应是相关的。结果表明,选择行为的模型应该包含强化历史、奖励对比因素和大小-时间相互作用,但奖励对比对选择的直接影响应该比间接的奖励-时间相互作用更重要。更好地了解影响选择行为的因素,可以为这一重要的个体差异变量提供关键见解。
{"title":"Reward Contrast Effects on Impulsive Choice and Timing in Rats.","authors":"Aaron P. Smith, J. R. Peterson, Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick","doi":"10.1163/22134468-00002059","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002059","url":null,"abstract":"Despite considerable interest in impulsive choice as a predictor of a variety of maladaptive behaviors, the mechanisms that drive choice behavior are still poorly understood. The present study sought to examine the influence of one understudied variable, reward magnitude contrast, on choice and timing behavior as changes in magnitude commonly occur within choice procedures. In addition, assessments of indirect effects on choice behavior through magnitude-timing interactions were assessed by measuring timing within the choice task. Rats were exposed to choice procedures composed of different pairs of magnitudes of rewards for either the smaller-sooner (SS) or larger-later (LL) option. In Phase 2, the magnitude of reward either increased or decreased by 1 pellet in different groups (LL increase = 1v1→1v2; SS decrease = 2v2 → 1v2; SS increase = 1v2 → 2v2), followed by a return to baseline in Phase 3. Choice behavior was affected by the initial magnitudes experienced in the task, demonstrating a strong anchor effect. The nature of the change in magnitude affected choice behavior as well. Timing behavior was also affected by the reward contrast manipulation albeit to a lesser degree and the timing and choice effects were correlated. The results suggest that models of choice behavior should incorporate reinforcement history, reward contrast elements, and magnitude-timing interactions, but that direct effects of reward contrast on choice should be given more weight than the indirect reward-timing interactions. A better understanding of the factors that contribute to choice behavior could supply key insights into this important individual differences variable.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":"48 1","pages":"147-166"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2016-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/22134468-00002059","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64576966","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Timing & Time Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1