首页 > 最新文献

Timing & Time Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Front matter 前页
Pub Date : 2023-04-10 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-11010000
{"title":"Front matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1163/22134468-11010000","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-11010000","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":"74 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135593551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Timing & Time Perception 时间和时间感知
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-12-07 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-10010001
{"title":"Timing & Time Perception","authors":"","doi":"10.1163/22134468-10010001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-10010001","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-12-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48812236","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Critical Repetition Rates for Perceptual Segregation of Time-Varying Auditory, Visual and Vibrotactile Stimulation 时变听觉、视觉和振动刺激感知分离的临界重复率
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-08-13 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-bja10043
Carlos Jurado, Marcelo Larrea, David Rosero, Juan Vizuete, T. Marquardt
What sound quality has led to exclude infrasound from sound in the conventional hearing range? We examined whether temporal segregation of pressure pulses is a distinctive property and evaluated this perceptual limit via an adaptive psychophysical procedure for pure tones and carriers of different envelopes. Further, to examine across-domain similarity and individual covariation of this limit, here called the critical segregation rate (CSR), it was also measured for various periodic visual and vibrotactile stimuli. Results showed that sequential auditory or vibrotactile stimuli separated by at least ~80‒90 ms (~11‒12-Hz repetition rates), will be perceived as perceptually segregated from one another. While this limit did not statistically differ between these two modalities, it was significantly lower than the ~150 ms necessary to perceptually segregate successive visual stimuli. For the three sensory modalities, stimulus periodicity was the main factor determining the CSR, which apparently reflects neural recovery times of the different sensory systems. Among all experimental conditions, significant within- and across-modality individual CSR correlations were observed, despite the visual CSR (mean: 6.8 Hz) being significantly lower than that of both other modalities. The auditory CSR was found to be significantly lower than the frequency above which sinusoids start to elicit a tonal quality (19 Hz; recently published for the same subjects). Returning to our initial question, the latter suggests that the cessation of tonal quality — not the segregation of pressure fluctuations — is the perceptual quality that has led to exclude infrasound (sound with frequencies < 20 Hz) from the conventional hearing range.
在传统听力范围内,是什么音质导致次声被排除在声音之外?我们研究了压力脉冲的时间分离是否是一种独特的性质,并通过对纯音和不同包络的载波的自适应心理物理学程序来评估这种感知极限。此外,为了检查该极限的跨域相似性和个体协变,这里称为临界分离率(CSR),还对各种周期性视觉和振动触觉刺激进行了测量。结果表明,连续的听觉或振动触觉刺激至少间隔~80-90 ms(~11-12Hz的重复率),将被感知为在感知上彼此分离。虽然这两种模式之间的这一极限在统计学上没有差异,但它明显低于~150 ms是在感知上分离连续视觉刺激所必需的。对于三种感觉模式,刺激周期性是决定CSR的主要因素,CSR明显反映了不同感觉系统的神经恢复时间。在所有实验条件中,尽管视觉CSR(平均值:6.8Hz)显著低于其他两种模式,但观察到了显著的模态内和模态间个体CSR相关性。听觉CSR被发现显著低于正弦曲线开始引发音调质量的频率(19Hz;最近针对相同受试者发表)。回到我们最初的问题,后者表明,音调质量的停止——而不是压力波动的分离——是导致将次声(频率<20 Hz的声音)排除在传统听力范围之外的感知质量。
{"title":"Critical Repetition Rates for Perceptual Segregation of Time-Varying Auditory, Visual and Vibrotactile Stimulation","authors":"Carlos Jurado, Marcelo Larrea, David Rosero, Juan Vizuete, T. Marquardt","doi":"10.1163/22134468-bja10043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10043","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000What sound quality has led to exclude infrasound from sound in the conventional hearing range? We examined whether temporal segregation of pressure pulses is a distinctive property and evaluated this perceptual limit via an adaptive psychophysical procedure for pure tones and carriers of different envelopes. Further, to examine across-domain similarity and individual covariation of this limit, here called the critical segregation rate (CSR), it was also measured for various periodic visual and vibrotactile stimuli. Results showed that sequential auditory or vibrotactile stimuli separated by at least ~80‒90 ms (~11‒12-Hz repetition rates), will be perceived as perceptually segregated from one another. While this limit did not statistically differ between these two modalities, it was significantly lower than the ~150 ms necessary to perceptually segregate successive visual stimuli. For the three sensory modalities, stimulus periodicity was the main factor determining the CSR, which apparently reflects neural recovery times of the different sensory systems. Among all experimental conditions, significant within- and across-modality individual CSR correlations were observed, despite the visual CSR (mean: 6.8 Hz) being significantly lower than that of both other modalities. The auditory CSR was found to be significantly lower than the frequency above which sinusoids start to elicit a tonal quality (19 Hz; recently published for the same subjects). Returning to our initial question, the latter suggests that the cessation of tonal quality — not the segregation of pressure fluctuations — is the perceptual quality that has led to exclude infrasound (sound with frequencies < 20 Hz) from the conventional hearing range.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43559294","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Music Tempo and Perception of Time: Musically Trained vs Nontrained Individuals 音乐节奏与时间感知:受过音乐训练与未受过音乐训练的个体
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-07-26 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-BJA10042
Miria N Plastira, M. Avraamides
In this experiment we explored the effect of music tempo on the perception of time. Musically trained and nontrained participants carried out a reproduction task with music clips of various durations and tempos. Results revealed that the reproduced durations were longer for fast-tempo music clips than for slow-tempo music clips of equal duration. In addition, short clips were more accurately reproduced compared to longer stimuli. Notably, the error in reproducing the duration of a stimulus was overall lower for musically trained than nontrained participants, but more so for short than long clips. Finally, the accuracy in estimating the duration of the music clips correlated positively with years of musical training, further suggesting that musical training is a critical variable for time estimation.
在这个实验中,我们探讨了音乐节奏对时间感知的影响。受过音乐训练和未受过训练的参与者用不同持续时间和节奏的音乐片段进行了复制任务。结果显示,快节奏音乐片段的再现持续时间比等持续时间的慢节奏音乐片段更长。此外,与较长的刺激相比,短片的再现更准确。值得注意的是,受过音乐训练的参与者在再现刺激持续时间方面的误差总体上低于未受过训练的参与者,但短片段的误差更大。最后,估计音乐片段持续时间的准确性与音乐训练的年限呈正相关,这进一步表明音乐训练是时间估计的关键变量。
{"title":"Music Tempo and Perception of Time: Musically Trained vs Nontrained Individuals","authors":"Miria N Plastira, M. Avraamides","doi":"10.1163/22134468-BJA10042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-BJA10042","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In this experiment we explored the effect of music tempo on the perception of time. Musically trained and nontrained participants carried out a reproduction task with music clips of various durations and tempos. Results revealed that the reproduced durations were longer for fast-tempo music clips than for slow-tempo music clips of equal duration. In addition, short clips were more accurately reproduced compared to longer stimuli. Notably, the error in reproducing the duration of a stimulus was overall lower for musically trained than nontrained participants, but more so for short than long clips. Finally, the accuracy in estimating the duration of the music clips correlated positively with years of musical training, further suggesting that musical training is a critical variable for time estimation.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45373943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Effects of Stimulus Intensity and Frequency on the Force and Timing of Sensorimotor Synchronisation 刺激强度和频率对感觉运动同步力和时间的影响
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-06-21 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-bja10041
N. P. Todd, P. Keller, S. Govender, J. Colebatch
We report an experiment to investigate possible vestibular effects on finger tapping to an auditory anapaest rhythm. In a sample of 10 subjects, index finger acceleration and tapping force were recorded along with extensor/flexor activity and the associated electroencephalographic activity measured at central and cerebellar surface electrodes. In a prior session with a standard short air-conducted 500-Hz pip, vestibular evoked myogenic potential thresholds were measured and subsequently used to set the acoustic intensity. During the main experiment subjects were asked to synchronise tapping to the pips arranged in the anapaest at two different frequencies, 500 Hz vs 5 kHz, so that only the low-frequency high-intensity condition was a vestibular, as well as an auditory stimulus. We hypothesised that a vestibular effect would manifest in an interaction between the frequency and intensity factors for a range of dependent measures of tapping performance. No clear evidence was found for vestibular effects, but this was likely due to the confounding effects of an independent effect of intensity and the relative weakness of the acoustic vestibular stimulus. However, the data did show novel evidence for two distinct timing processes for the flexion and extension stages of a tap cycle and two distinct timing strategies, which we refer to as ‘staccato’ and ‘legato’, characterised by different profiles of force and extension.
我们报告了一项实验,以研究前庭对手指敲击听觉刺激节律的可能影响。在10名受试者的样本中,记录食指加速度和敲击力,以及在中央和小脑表面电极测量的伸肌/屈肌活动和相关脑电图活动。在使用标准短空气传导500Hz pip的先前会话中,测量前庭诱发的肌源性电位阈值,随后用于设置声学强度。在主要实验过程中,受试者被要求在两个不同的频率(500 Hz和5 kHz,所以只有低频高强度的情况是前庭,以及听觉刺激。我们假设前庭效应将表现为一系列依赖性敲击性能测量的频率和强度因素之间的相互作用。没有发现前庭效应的明确证据,但这可能是由于强度的独立效应和前庭声刺激的相对较弱的混杂效应。然而,数据确实显示了敲击周期的屈曲和伸展阶段有两个不同的计时过程和两种不同的计时策略的新证据,我们称之为“断奏”和“连奏”,其特征是力和伸展的不同轮廓。
{"title":"Effects of Stimulus Intensity and Frequency on the Force and Timing of Sensorimotor Synchronisation","authors":"N. P. Todd, P. Keller, S. Govender, J. Colebatch","doi":"10.1163/22134468-bja10041","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10041","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000We report an experiment to investigate possible vestibular effects on finger tapping to an auditory anapaest rhythm. In a sample of 10 subjects, index finger acceleration and tapping force were recorded along with extensor/flexor activity and the associated electroencephalographic activity measured at central and cerebellar surface electrodes. In a prior session with a standard short air-conducted 500-Hz pip, vestibular evoked myogenic potential thresholds were measured and subsequently used to set the acoustic intensity. During the main experiment subjects were asked to synchronise tapping to the pips arranged in the anapaest at two different frequencies, 500 Hz vs 5 kHz, so that only the low-frequency high-intensity condition was a vestibular, as well as an auditory stimulus. We hypothesised that a vestibular effect would manifest in an interaction between the frequency and intensity factors for a range of dependent measures of tapping performance. No clear evidence was found for vestibular effects, but this was likely due to the confounding effects of an independent effect of intensity and the relative weakness of the acoustic vestibular stimulus. However, the data did show novel evidence for two distinct timing processes for the flexion and extension stages of a tap cycle and two distinct timing strategies, which we refer to as ‘staccato’ and ‘legato’, characterised by different profiles of force and extension.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43502420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Modelling Feedback Effects on the Production of Short Time Intervals 短时间间隔生产的反馈效应建模
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-05-31 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-BJA10038
J. Wearden, Jordan J. Wehrman
People produced time intervals of 500 to 1250 ms, with accurate feedback in ms provided after each production. The mean times produced tracked the target times closely, and the coefficient of variation (standard deviation/mean) declined with increasing target time. The mean absolute change from one trial to another, and its standard deviation, measures of trial-by-trial change, also increased with target time. A model of feedback was fitted to all four measures. It assumed that the time produced resulted from a combination of a scalar timing process and a non-timing process. Although the non-timing process was on average invariant with target time, the timing process was assumed to be sensitive to feedback, in two different ways. If the previous production was close to the target the model repeated it (a repeat process), but if it was further away the next production was adjusted by an amount related to the discrepancy between the previous production and the target (an adjust process). The balance between the two was governed by a threshold, which was on average constant, and it was further assumed that the relative variability of the repeat process was lower than that of the adjust process. The model produced output which fitted three of the four measures well (average deviation of 3 or 4%) but fitted the standard deviation of change less well. Reducing the magnitude of the non-timing process produced output which conformed approximately to scalar timing, and the model could also mimic data resulting from the provision of inaccurate feedback.
人们产生了500到1250的时间间隔 ms,在每次生产后提供精确的ms反馈。产生的平均时间与目标时间密切相关,变异系数(标准差/平均值)随着目标时间的增加而下降。从一个试验到另一个试验的平均绝对变化及其标准差,即逐个试验变化的衡量标准,也随着目标时间的增加而增加。反馈模型适用于所有四项指标。它假设产生的时间是由标量计时过程和非计时过程的组合产生的。尽管非定时过程平均而言与目标时间无关,但定时过程被认为以两种不同的方式对反馈敏感。如果上一次生产接近目标,模型会重复它(重复过程),但如果离目标更远,则下一次生产会根据上一次产量与目标之间的差异进行调整(调整过程)。两者之间的平衡由一个阈值决定,该阈值平均恒定,并且进一步假设重复过程的相对可变性低于调整过程的相对变异性。该模型产生的输出很好地拟合了四个测量中的三个(平均偏差为3%或4%),但拟合变化的标准偏差不太好。减少非定时过程的幅度产生了近似符合标量定时的输出,并且该模型还可以模拟由于提供不准确的反馈而产生的数据。
{"title":"Modelling Feedback Effects on the Production of Short Time Intervals","authors":"J. Wearden, Jordan J. Wehrman","doi":"10.1163/22134468-BJA10038","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-BJA10038","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000People produced time intervals of 500 to 1250 ms, with accurate feedback in ms provided after each production. The mean times produced tracked the target times closely, and the coefficient of variation (standard deviation/mean) declined with increasing target time. The mean absolute change from one trial to another, and its standard deviation, measures of trial-by-trial change, also increased with target time. A model of feedback was fitted to all four measures. It assumed that the time produced resulted from a combination of a scalar timing process and a non-timing process. Although the non-timing process was on average invariant with target time, the timing process was assumed to be sensitive to feedback, in two different ways. If the previous production was close to the target the model repeated it (a repeat process), but if it was further away the next production was adjusted by an amount related to the discrepancy between the previous production and the target (an adjust process). The balance between the two was governed by a threshold, which was on average constant, and it was further assumed that the relative variability of the repeat process was lower than that of the adjust process. The model produced output which fitted three of the four measures well (average deviation of 3 or 4%) but fitted the standard deviation of change less well. Reducing the magnitude of the non-timing process produced output which conformed approximately to scalar timing, and the model could also mimic data resulting from the provision of inaccurate feedback.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41976400","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Running Fast and Standing Slow: a Study on the Effect of Gesture Posing on Time Perception 快跑慢站:手势姿势对时间感知的影响研究
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-05-31 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-BJA10037
L. Lo, H. M. Tam, W. O. Li
Everyone has 24 hours in a day, but this does not feel the same for everyone. Time perception can be subjective and is affected by a wide range of factors. Studies have shown that presenting pictures with actions can bias observers’ performance in a duration reproduction task. Instead of watching pictures of actions, this research explored the effect of gesture posing on such a time perception bias. Sixty participants were assigned into either a watching or posing group. Compared with the corresponding baseline performance, participants overestimated time duration when viewing or posing pictures suggesting action. A corresponding underestimation was observed when the pictures or gestures suggested inaction. The role of physical gestures and their potential effect on time perception based on the embodiment account is discussed. Further investigation is also proposed to examine the role of the embodiment effect on subsecond time intervals.
每个人一天都有24小时,但每个人的感觉都不一样。时间感知可能是主观的,并受到多种因素的影响。研究表明,在持续再现任务中,带动作的图片会使观察者的表现产生偏差。这项研究不是看动作的图片,而是探索姿势对这种时间感知偏差的影响。60名参与者被分为观看组和摆姿势组。与相应的基线表现相比,参与者在观看或摆出暗示行动的照片时高估了持续时间。当图片或手势暗示不作为时,观察到相应的低估。讨论了基于实施方式的肢体动作的作用及其对时间感知的潜在影响。我们还提出了进一步的研究,以检验体现效应对亚秒时间间隔的作用。
{"title":"Running Fast and Standing Slow: a Study on the Effect of Gesture Posing on Time Perception","authors":"L. Lo, H. M. Tam, W. O. Li","doi":"10.1163/22134468-BJA10037","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-BJA10037","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Everyone has 24 hours in a day, but this does not feel the same for everyone. Time perception can be subjective and is affected by a wide range of factors. Studies have shown that presenting pictures with actions can bias observers’ performance in a duration reproduction task. Instead of watching pictures of actions, this research explored the effect of gesture posing on such a time perception bias. Sixty participants were assigned into either a watching or posing group. Compared with the corresponding baseline performance, participants overestimated time duration when viewing or posing pictures suggesting action. A corresponding underestimation was observed when the pictures or gestures suggested inaction. The role of physical gestures and their potential effect on time perception based on the embodiment account is discussed. Further investigation is also proposed to examine the role of the embodiment effect on subsecond time intervals.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44518099","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Retrospective and Present Judgment of the Passage of Time in the Elderly 老年人对时间流逝的回顾与当下判断
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-05-17 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-bja10031
S. Droit-Volet, N. Martinelli, M. Dambrun, G. Vallet, Fanny Lorandi
This study examined the judgment of the passage of time in elderly people living in retirement homes, focusing on the passage of time experienced in the present and that judged retrospectively for short periods (last day, week, month) and longer periods of life (last year, now compared with five years ago, as we get older). Participants’ cognitive abilities and feelings of happiness were also assessed among other dimensions. Results showed no significant relationship between these three forms of judgment of the passage of time, except between the judgment of the passage of time for the present and for the day. In addition, the level of happiness was a significant predictor of both the momentary judgment of the passage of time and the retrospective judgment of the passage of time for shorter periods. In contrast, the individual differences in cognitive abilities better explained differences in the retrospective judgment of the passage of time for longer periods. As discussed, the different forms of judgment of the passage of time are therefore based on different cognitive mechanisms.
这项研究考察了生活在养老院的老年人对时间流逝的判断,重点是现在经历的时间流逝,以及对短时间(过去一天、一周、一个月)和较长时间(去年,现在与五年前相比,随着年龄的增长)的回顾性判断。参与者的认知能力和幸福感也在其他方面进行了评估。结果显示,这三种形式对时间流逝的判断之间没有显著的关系,除了对现在和当天时间流逝的判断之间的关系。此外,幸福水平是对时间流逝的瞬间判断和对较短时间流逝的回顾性判断的重要预测指标。相比之下,认知能力的个体差异更好地解释了长时间回顾判断时间流逝的差异。如前所述,对时间流逝的不同形式的判断基于不同的认知机制。
{"title":"The Retrospective and Present Judgment of the Passage of Time in the Elderly","authors":"S. Droit-Volet, N. Martinelli, M. Dambrun, G. Vallet, Fanny Lorandi","doi":"10.1163/22134468-bja10031","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10031","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This study examined the judgment of the passage of time in elderly people living in retirement homes, focusing on the passage of time experienced in the present and that judged retrospectively for short periods (last day, week, month) and longer periods of life (last year, now compared with five years ago, as we get older). Participants’ cognitive abilities and feelings of happiness were also assessed among other dimensions. Results showed no significant relationship between these three forms of judgment of the passage of time, except between the judgment of the passage of time for the present and for the day. In addition, the level of happiness was a significant predictor of both the momentary judgment of the passage of time and the retrospective judgment of the passage of time for shorter periods. In contrast, the individual differences in cognitive abilities better explained differences in the retrospective judgment of the passage of time for longer periods. As discussed, the different forms of judgment of the passage of time are therefore based on different cognitive mechanisms.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48904865","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The Role of Time Perspective in the General Tendency to Disengage and Reengage from Problematic Goal Striving 时间视角在从有问题的目标追求中脱离和重新投入的一般倾向中的作用
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-05-13 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-bja10032
Pavol Kačmár, Merav Beere
Both time and goals are ubiquitous in our everyday lives. The main aim of the present set of studies was to corroborate if time perspective, as conceptualized by Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory, is related to goal disengagement and reengagement during problematic goal striving. Over three studies, with more than 600 participants varying in age and cultural background, it was found that future time perspective is related to goal disengagement from a problematic goal striving. A pilot study with 103 university students showed that the future positive scale was negatively related to the general capacity to disengage. In a second study with 356 high-school students, it was shown that the future positive, past positive and present hedonistic scales were negatively correlated to goal disengagement, while the present fatalistic and future negative scales were positively correlated to goal disengagement. However, when accounting for the Big-five, only the future negative and (possibly) present hedonistic scales remained statistically significant predictors of the capacity to disengage from a goal. In the third study, the role of the future time perspective was replicated among 169 people suffering from chronic pain disease. It was shown that the future time perspective (in its general form) negatively predicted the general capacity to disengage. Moreover, focusing on more process-oriented aspects, this further predicted the action crisis during goal pursuit. This set of studies not only provides novel findings but also encourages further investigation of the time perspective in goal striving.
时间和目标在我们的日常生活中无处不在。本研究的主要目的是证实由津巴多时间视角量表概念化的时间视角是否与有问题的目标追求过程中的目标脱离和再投入相关。在对600多名年龄和文化背景各异的参与者进行的三项研究中,研究人员发现,未来时间观与从有问题的目标追求中脱离出来有关。一项针对103名大学生的初步研究表明,未来积极量表与一般脱离能力呈负相关。对356名高中生进行的第二项研究表明,未来积极、过去积极和现在享乐主义量表与目标脱离呈负相关,而现在宿命论和未来消极量表与目标脱离呈正相关。然而,当考虑到大五项时,只有未来的消极和(可能的)现在的享乐主义量表在统计上仍然是脱离目标能力的显著预测因子。在第三项研究中,169名患有慢性疼痛疾病的患者重复了未来时间观的作用。结果表明,未来时间视角(一般形式)对一般脱离能力有负向预测。此外,这更侧重于过程导向的方面,进一步预测了目标追求过程中的行动危机。这组研究不仅提供了新的发现,而且鼓励了对目标奋斗的时间视角的进一步研究。
{"title":"The Role of Time Perspective in the General Tendency to Disengage and Reengage from Problematic Goal Striving","authors":"Pavol Kačmár, Merav Beere","doi":"10.1163/22134468-bja10032","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10032","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Both time and goals are ubiquitous in our everyday lives. The main aim of the present set of studies was to corroborate if time perspective, as conceptualized by Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory, is related to goal disengagement and reengagement during problematic goal striving. Over three studies, with more than 600 participants varying in age and cultural background, it was found that future time perspective is related to goal disengagement from a problematic goal striving. A pilot study with 103 university students showed that the future positive scale was negatively related to the general capacity to disengage. In a second study with 356 high-school students, it was shown that the future positive, past positive and present hedonistic scales were negatively correlated to goal disengagement, while the present fatalistic and future negative scales were positively correlated to goal disengagement. However, when accounting for the Big-five, only the future negative and (possibly) present hedonistic scales remained statistically significant predictors of the capacity to disengage from a goal. In the third study, the role of the future time perspective was replicated among 169 people suffering from chronic pain disease. It was shown that the future time perspective (in its general form) negatively predicted the general capacity to disengage. Moreover, focusing on more process-oriented aspects, this further predicted the action crisis during goal pursuit. This set of studies not only provides novel findings but also encourages further investigation of the time perspective in goal striving.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48219357","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Perceiving Tempo in Incongruent Audiovisual Presentations of Human Motion: Evidence for a Visual Driving Effect 在令人不快的人类运动视听演示中感知节奏:视觉驱动效应的证据
IF 1.4 Pub Date : 2021-05-11 DOI: 10.1163/22134468-BJA10036
Xinyue Wang, Clemens Wöllner, Zhuanghua Shi
Compared to vision, audition has been considered to be the dominant sensory modality for temporal processing. Nevertheless, recent research suggests the opposite, such that the apparent inferiority of visual information in tempo judgements might be due to the lack of ecological validity of experimental stimuli, and reliable visual movements may have the potential to alter the temporal location of perceived auditory inputs. To explore the role of audition and vision in overall time perception, audiovisual stimuli with various degrees of temporal congruence were developed in the current study. We investigated which sensory modality weighs more in holistic tempo judgements with conflicting audiovisual information, and whether biological motion (point-light displays of dancers) rather than auditory cues (rhythmic beats) dominate judgements of tempo. A bisection experiment found that participants relied more on visual tempo compared to auditory tempo in overall tempo judgements. For fast tempi (150 to 180 BPM), participants judged ‘fast’ significantly more often with visual cues regardless of the auditory tempo, whereas for slow tempi (60 to 90 BPM), they did so significantly less often. Our results support the notion that visual stimuli with higher ecological validity have the potential to drive up or down the holistic perception of tempo.
与视觉相比,听觉被认为是时间处理的主要感觉模式。然而,最近的研究表明情况恰恰相反,视觉信息在节奏判断中的明显劣势可能是由于实验刺激缺乏生态有效性,而可靠的视觉运动可能有可能改变感知听觉输入的时间位置。为了探索听觉和视觉在整体时间感知中的作用,本研究开发了具有不同程度时间一致性的视听刺激。我们研究了在视听信息冲突的情况下,哪种感觉模式在整体节奏判断中更重要,以及生物运动(舞者的点光显示)而不是听觉线索(节奏节拍)是否主导了节奏判断。一项平分实验发现,在整体节奏判断中,参与者更依赖视觉节奏,而不是听觉节奏。对于快节奏(150至180 BPM),无论听觉节奏如何,参与者都会更频繁地用视觉线索判断“快”,而对于慢节奏(60至90 BPM),他们这样做的频率明显更低。我们的研究结果支持这样一种观点,即具有更高生态有效性的视觉刺激有可能提高或降低对节奏的整体感知。
{"title":"Perceiving Tempo in Incongruent Audiovisual Presentations of Human Motion: Evidence for a Visual Driving Effect","authors":"Xinyue Wang, Clemens Wöllner, Zhuanghua Shi","doi":"10.1163/22134468-BJA10036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-BJA10036","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Compared to vision, audition has been considered to be the dominant sensory modality for temporal processing. Nevertheless, recent research suggests the opposite, such that the apparent inferiority of visual information in tempo judgements might be due to the lack of ecological validity of experimental stimuli, and reliable visual movements may have the potential to alter the temporal location of perceived auditory inputs. To explore the role of audition and vision in overall time perception, audiovisual stimuli with various degrees of temporal congruence were developed in the current study. We investigated which sensory modality weighs more in holistic tempo judgements with conflicting audiovisual information, and whether biological motion (point-light displays of dancers) rather than auditory cues (rhythmic beats) dominate judgements of tempo. A bisection experiment found that participants relied more on visual tempo compared to auditory tempo in overall tempo judgements. For fast tempi (150 to 180 BPM), participants judged ‘fast’ significantly more often with visual cues regardless of the auditory tempo, whereas for slow tempi (60 to 90 BPM), they did so significantly less often. Our results support the notion that visual stimuli with higher ecological validity have the potential to drive up or down the holistic perception of tempo.","PeriodicalId":29927,"journal":{"name":"Timing & Time Perception","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41562060","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Timing & Time Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1