首页 > 最新文献

Open Mind最新文献

英文 中文
Preschoolers' Comprehension of Functional Metaphors. 学龄前儿童对功能性隐喻的理解。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-07-19 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00152
Rebecca Zhu, Mariel K Goddu, Lily Zihui Zhu, Alison Gopnik

Previous work suggests that preschoolers often misunderstand metaphors. However, some recent studies demonstrate that preschoolers can represent abstract relations, suggesting that the cognitive foundations of metaphor comprehension may develop earlier than previously believed. The present experiments used novel paradigms to explore whether preschoolers (N = 200; 4-5 years; 100 males, 100 females; predominantly White) can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities. In Experiment 1, preschoolers and adults (N = 64; 18-41 years; 25 males, 39 females; predominantly White) rated functional metaphors (e.g., "Roofs are hats"; "Tires are shoes") as "smarter" than nonsense statements (e.g., "Boats are skirts"; "Pennies are sunglasses") in a metalinguistic judgment task (d = .42 in preschoolers; d = 3.06 in adults). In Experiment 2, preschoolers preferred functional explanations (e.g., "Both keep you dry") over perceptual explanations (e.g., "Both have pointy tops") when interpreting functional metaphors (e.g., "Roofs are hats") (d = .99). In Experiment 3, preschoolers preferred functional metaphors (e.g., "Roofs are hats") over nonsense statements (e.g., "Roofs are scissors") when prompted to select the "better" utterance (d = 1.25). Moreover, over a quarter of preschoolers in Experiment 1 and half of preschoolers in Experiment 3 explicitly articulated functional similarities when justifying their responses, and the performance of these subsets of children drove the success of the entire sample in both experiments. These findings demonstrate that preschoolers can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities.

以往的研究表明,学龄前儿童常常误解隐喻。然而,最近的一些研究表明,学龄前儿童可以表现抽象关系,这表明隐喻理解的认知基础可能比以前认为的发展得更早。本实验采用新颖的范式来探讨学龄前儿童(人数=200;4-5 岁;男性 100 人,女性 100 人;以白人为主)能否理解基于抽象功能相似性的隐喻。在实验 1 中,学龄前儿童和成人(人数 = 64;18-41 岁;25 名男性,39 名女性;主要为白人)在金属语言判断任务中将功能性隐喻(如 "屋顶是帽子";"轮胎是鞋子")评为比无意义陈述(如 "船是裙子";"便士是太阳镜")"更聪明"(学龄前儿童的 d = 0.42;成人的 d = 3.06)。在实验 2 中,学龄前儿童在解释功能性隐喻(如 "屋顶是帽子")时,更倾向于功能性解释(如 "都能保持干燥"),而不是知觉性解释(如 "都有尖尖的顶")(d = 0.99)。在实验 3 中,当要求学前儿童选择 "更好 "的语句时,他们更喜欢功能性隐喻(如 "屋顶是帽子"),而不是无意义的语句(如 "屋顶是剪刀")(d = 1.25)。此外,实验 1 中超过四分之一的学龄前儿童和实验 3 中一半的学龄前儿童在解释他们的回答时明确阐述了功能相似性,这些儿童的表现推动了整个样本在这两个实验中的成功。这些研究结果表明,学龄前儿童能够根据抽象的功能相似性理解隐喻。
{"title":"Preschoolers' Comprehension of Functional Metaphors.","authors":"Rebecca Zhu, Mariel K Goddu, Lily Zihui Zhu, Alison Gopnik","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00152","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00152","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous work suggests that preschoolers often misunderstand metaphors. However, some recent studies demonstrate that preschoolers can represent abstract relations, suggesting that the cognitive foundations of metaphor comprehension may develop earlier than previously believed. The present experiments used novel paradigms to explore whether preschoolers (<i>N</i> = 200; 4-5 years; 100 males, 100 females; predominantly White) can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities. In Experiment 1, preschoolers and adults (<i>N</i> = 64; 18-41 years; 25 males, 39 females; predominantly White) rated functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\"; \"Tires are shoes\") as \"smarter\" than nonsense statements (e.g., \"Boats are skirts\"; \"Pennies are sunglasses\") in a metalinguistic judgment task (<i>d</i> = .42 in preschoolers; <i>d</i> = 3.06 in adults). In Experiment 2, preschoolers preferred functional explanations (e.g., \"Both keep you dry\") over perceptual explanations (e.g., \"Both have pointy tops\") when interpreting functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\") (<i>d</i> = .99). In Experiment 3, preschoolers preferred functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\") over nonsense statements (e.g., \"Roofs are scissors\") when prompted to select the \"better\" utterance (<i>d</i> = 1.25). Moreover, over a quarter of preschoolers in Experiment 1 and half of preschoolers in Experiment 3 explicitly articulated functional similarities when justifying their responses, and the performance of these subsets of children drove the success of the entire sample in both experiments. These findings demonstrate that preschoolers can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"924-949"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11285420/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141793704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Wise Mind Balances the Abstract and the Concrete. 智慧的头脑能平衡抽象与具体。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-28 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00149
Igor Grossmann, Johanna Peetz, Anna Dorfman, Amanda Rotella, Roger Buehler

We explored how individuals' mental representations of complex and uncertain situations impact their ability to reason wisely. To this end, we introduce situated methods to capture abstract and concrete mental representations and the switching between them when reflecting on social challenges. Using these methods, we evaluated the alignment of abstractness and concreteness with four integral facets of wisdom: intellectual humility, open-mindedness, perspective-taking, and compromise-seeking. Data from North American and UK participants (N = 1,151) revealed that both abstract and concrete construals significantly contribute to wise reasoning, even when controlling for a host of relevant covariates and potential response bias. Natural language processing of unstructured texts among high (top 25%) and low (bottom 25%) wisdom participants corroborated these results: semantic networks of the high wisdom group reveal greater use of both abstract and concrete themes compared to the low wisdom group. Finally, employing a repeated strategy-choice method as an additional measure, our findings demonstrated that individuals who showed a greater balance and switching between these construal types exhibited higher wisdom. Our findings advance understanding of individual differences in mental representations and how construals shape reasoning across contexts in everyday life.

我们探讨了个人对复杂和不确定情况的心理表征如何影响其明智推理的能力。为此,我们引入了情景方法来捕捉抽象和具体的心理表征,以及在反思社会挑战时它们之间的切换。利用这些方法,我们评估了抽象性和具体性与智慧四个不可或缺的方面的一致性,这四个方面是:智慧的谦逊、开放的心态、视角的把握和寻求妥协。来自北美和英国参与者(人数=1,151)的数据显示,即使控制了一系列相关的协变量和潜在的反应偏差,抽象和具体的构想都对智慧推理有显著的促进作用。对高智商(前25%)和低智商(后25%)参与者的非结构化文本进行的自然语言处理证实了这些结果:高智商组的语义网络显示,与低智商组相比,高智商组更多地使用了抽象和具体主题。最后,我们采用了重复策略选择法作为额外的测量方法,结果表明,在这些构解类型之间表现出更大平衡和切换的个体表现出更高的智慧。我们的研究结果加深了人们对心理表征的个体差异以及构解如何影响日常生活中跨情境推理的理解。
{"title":"The Wise Mind Balances the Abstract and the Concrete.","authors":"Igor Grossmann, Johanna Peetz, Anna Dorfman, Amanda Rotella, Roger Buehler","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00149","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00149","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We explored how individuals' mental representations of complex and uncertain situations impact their ability to reason wisely. To this end, we introduce situated methods to capture abstract and concrete mental representations and the switching between them when reflecting on social challenges. Using these methods, we evaluated the alignment of abstractness and concreteness with four integral facets of wisdom: intellectual humility, open-mindedness, perspective-taking, and compromise-seeking. Data from North American and UK participants (<i>N</i> = 1,151) revealed that both abstract and concrete construals significantly contribute to wise reasoning, even when controlling for a host of relevant covariates and potential response bias. Natural language processing of unstructured texts among high (top 25%) and low (bottom 25%) wisdom participants corroborated these results: semantic networks of the high wisdom group reveal greater use of both abstract and concrete themes compared to the low wisdom group. Finally, employing a repeated strategy-choice method as an additional measure, our findings demonstrated that individuals who showed a greater balance and switching between these construal types exhibited higher wisdom. Our findings advance understanding of individual differences in mental representations and how construals shape reasoning across contexts in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"826-858"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11226238/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141555550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Mathematical Relationship Between Contextual Probability and N400 Amplitude. 语境概率与 N400 振幅之间的数学关系。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-28 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00150
James A Michaelov, Benjamin K Bergen

Accounts of human language comprehension propose different mathematical relationships between the contextual probability of a word and how difficult it is to process, including linear, logarithmic, and super-logarithmic ones. However, the empirical evidence favoring any of these over the others is mixed, appearing to vary depending on the index of processing difficulty used and the approach taken to calculate contextual probability. To help disentangle these results, we focus on the mathematical relationship between corpus-derived contextual probability and the N400, a neural index of processing difficulty. Specifically, we use 37 contemporary transformer language models to calculate the contextual probability of stimuli from 6 experimental studies of the N400, and test whether N400 amplitude is best predicted by a linear, logarithmic, super-logarithmic, or sub-logarithmic transformation of the probabilities calculated using these language models, as well as combinations of these transformed metrics. We replicate the finding that on some datasets, a combination of linearly and logarithmically-transformed probability can predict N400 amplitude better than either metric alone. In addition, we find that overall, the best single predictor of N400 amplitude is sub-logarithmically-transformed probability, which for almost all language models and datasets explains all the variance in N400 amplitude otherwise explained by the linear and logarithmic transformations. This is a novel finding that is not predicted by any current theoretical accounts, and thus one that we argue is likely to play an important role in increasing our understanding of how the statistical regularities of language impact language comprehension.

关于人类语言理解能力的研究提出了单词的语境概率与单词处理难度之间的不同数学关系,包括线性关系、对数关系和超对数关系。然而,支持其中任何一种关系的实证证据并不充分,似乎因所使用的处理难度指数和计算上下文概率的方法而异。为了帮助厘清这些结果,我们重点研究了从语料库中得出的语境概率与 N400(一种表示处理难度的神经指数)之间的数学关系。具体来说,我们使用 37 个当代转换器语言模型来计算 6 项 N400 实验研究中刺激的上下文概率,并测试使用这些语言模型计算出的概率的线性、对数、超对数或次对数变换以及这些转换指标的组合是否能最好地预测 N400 振幅。我们重复了这一发现,即在某些数据集上,线性变换和对数变换概率的组合预测 N400 振幅的效果优于单独使用其中一种指标。此外,我们还发现,总体而言,N400 波幅的最佳预测指标是亚对数变换概率,几乎所有语言模型和数据集的 N400 波幅差异都可以用它来解释,而线性变换和对数变换则无法解释。这是一个目前任何理论都无法预测的新发现,因此我们认为,这一发现可能会在加深我们对语言的统计规律性如何影响语言理解的理解方面发挥重要作用。
{"title":"On the Mathematical Relationship Between Contextual Probability and N400 Amplitude.","authors":"James A Michaelov, Benjamin K Bergen","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00150","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00150","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accounts of human language comprehension propose different mathematical relationships between the contextual probability of a word and how difficult it is to process, including linear, logarithmic, and super-logarithmic ones. However, the empirical evidence favoring any of these over the others is mixed, appearing to vary depending on the index of processing difficulty used and the approach taken to calculate contextual probability. To help disentangle these results, we focus on the mathematical relationship between corpus-derived contextual probability and the N400, a neural index of processing difficulty. Specifically, we use 37 contemporary transformer language models to calculate the contextual probability of stimuli from 6 experimental studies of the N400, and test whether N400 amplitude is best predicted by a linear, logarithmic, super-logarithmic, or sub-logarithmic transformation of the probabilities calculated using these language models, as well as combinations of these transformed metrics. We replicate the finding that on some datasets, a combination of linearly and logarithmically-transformed probability can predict N400 amplitude better than either metric alone. In addition, we find that overall, the best single predictor of N400 amplitude is sub-logarithmically-transformed probability, which for almost all language models and datasets explains all the variance in N400 amplitude otherwise explained by the linear and logarithmic transformations. This is a novel finding that is not predicted by any current theoretical accounts, and thus one that we argue is likely to play an important role in increasing our understanding of how the statistical regularities of language impact language comprehension.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"859-897"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11285424/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141793703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Continuity in Logical Development: Domain-General Disjunctive Inference by Toddlers. 逻辑发展的连续性:学步儿童的领域性断章取义推理
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-28 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00148
Nicolò Cesana-Arlotti, Justin Halberda

Children grow up surrounded by opportunities to learn (the language of their community, the movements of their body, other people's preferences and mental lives, games, social norms, etc.). Here, we find that toddlers (N = 36; age range 2.3-3.2 years) rely on a logical reasoning strategy, Disjunctive Inference (i.e., A OR B, A is ruled out, THEREFORE, B), across a variety of situations, all before they have any formal education or extensive experience with words for expressing logical meanings. In learning new words, learning new facts about a person, and finding the winner of a race, toddlers systematically consider and reject competitors before deciding who must be the winner. This suggests that toddlers may have a general-purpose logical reasoning tool that they can use in any situation.

儿童在成长过程中会有很多学习的机会(社区的语言、身体的动作、他人的喜好和精神生活、游戏、社会规范等)。在这里,我们发现学步儿童(N = 36;年龄范围为 2.3-3.2 岁)在各种情况下都依赖于一种逻辑推理策略--分离推理(即:A 或 B,排除 A,因此,B),而这一切都发生在他们接受任何正规教育或对表达逻辑含义的词语有丰富经验之前。在学习新单词、了解一个人的新情况和寻找比赛的获胜者时,学步儿童会系统地考虑和拒绝竞争对手,然后再决定谁一定是获胜者。这表明幼儿可能拥有一种通用的逻辑推理工具,可以在任何情况下使用。
{"title":"A Continuity in Logical Development: Domain-General Disjunctive Inference by Toddlers.","authors":"Nicolò Cesana-Arlotti, Justin Halberda","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00148","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00148","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children grow up surrounded by opportunities to learn (the language of their community, the movements of their body, other people's preferences and mental lives, games, social norms, etc.). Here, we find that toddlers (N = 36; age range 2.3-3.2 years) rely on a logical reasoning strategy, Disjunctive Inference (i.e., A OR B, A is ruled out, THEREFORE, B), across a variety of situations, all before they have any formal education or extensive experience with words for expressing logical meanings. In learning new words, learning new facts about a person, and finding the winner of a race, toddlers systematically consider and reject competitors before deciding who must be the winner. This suggests that toddlers may have a general-purpose logical reasoning tool that they can use in any situation.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"809-825"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11226237/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141556449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conducting Developmental Research Online vs. In-Person: A Meta-Analysis. 在线与亲自开展发展研究:元分析。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-12 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00147
Aaron Chuey, Veronica Boyce, Anjie Cao, Michael C Frank

An increasing number of psychological experiments with children are being conducted using online platforms, in part due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Individual replications have compared the findings of particular experiments online and in-person, but the general effect of data collection method on data collected from children is still unknown. Therefore, the goal of the current meta-analysis is to estimate the average difference in effect size for developmental studies conducted online compared to the same studies conducted in-person. Our pre-registered analysis includes 211 effect sizes calculated from 30 papers with 3282 children, ranging in age from four months to six years. The estimated effect size for studies conducted online was slightly smaller than for their counterparts conducted in-person, a difference of d = -.05, but this difference was not significant, 95% CI = [-.17, .07]. We examined several potential moderators of the effect of online testing, including the role of dependent measure (looking vs verbal), online study method (moderated vs unmoderated), and age, but none of these were significant. The literature to date thus suggests-on average-small differences in results between in-person and online experimentation.

部分由于 COVID-19 的流行,越来越多的儿童心理实验通过网络平台进行。个别复制研究对特定实验的在线和现场结果进行了比较,但数据收集方法对儿童数据收集的总体影响仍是未知数。因此,本次荟萃分析的目的是估算在线进行的发育研究与面对面进行的相同研究在效应大小上的平均差异。我们的预注册分析包括从 30 篇论文中计算出的 211 个效应大小,涉及 3282 名儿童,年龄从 4 个月到 6 岁不等。在线研究的估计效应大小略小于面对面研究,差异为 d = -.05,但这一差异并不显著,95% CI = [-.17, .07]。我们研究了在线测试影响的几个潜在调节因素,包括依赖测量的作用(观察与口头)、在线研究方法(有调节与无调节)和年龄,但这些因素都不显著。因此,迄今为止的文献表明,面对面实验和在线实验的结果平均差异很小。
{"title":"Conducting Developmental Research Online vs. In-Person: A Meta-Analysis.","authors":"Aaron Chuey, Veronica Boyce, Anjie Cao, Michael C Frank","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00147","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00147","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An increasing number of psychological experiments with children are being conducted using online platforms, in part due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Individual replications have compared the findings of particular experiments online and in-person, but the general effect of data collection method on data collected from children is still unknown. Therefore, the goal of the current meta-analysis is to estimate the average difference in effect size for developmental studies conducted online compared to the same studies conducted in-person. Our pre-registered analysis includes 211 effect sizes calculated from 30 papers with 3282 children, ranging in age from four months to six years. The estimated effect size for studies conducted online was slightly smaller than for their counterparts conducted in-person, a difference of <i>d</i> = -.05, but this difference was not significant, 95% CI = [-.17, .07]. We examined several potential moderators of the effect of online testing, including the role of dependent measure (looking vs verbal), online study method (moderated vs unmoderated), and age, but none of these were significant. The literature to date thus suggests-on average-small differences in results between in-person and online experimentation.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"795-808"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11219065/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Phone in a Basket Looks Like a Knife in a Cup: Role-Filler Independence in Visual Processing. 篮中的手机就像杯中的刀:视觉处理中的角色填充独立性
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-12 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00146
Alon Hafri, Michael F Bonner, Barbara Landau, Chaz Firestone

When a piece of fruit is in a bowl, and the bowl is on a table, we appreciate not only the individual objects and their features, but also the relations containment and support, which abstract away from the particular objects involved. Independent representation of roles (e.g., containers vs. supporters) and "fillers" of those roles (e.g., bowls vs. cups, tables vs. chairs) is a core principle of language and higher-level reasoning. But does such role-filler independence also arise in automatic visual processing? Here, we show that it does, by exploring a surprising error that such independence can produce. In four experiments, participants saw a stream of images containing different objects arranged in force-dynamic relations-e.g., a phone contained in a basket, a marker resting on a garbage can, or a knife sitting in a cup. Participants had to respond to a single target image (e.g., a phone in a basket) within a stream of distractors presented under time constraints. Surprisingly, even though participants completed this task quickly and accurately, they false-alarmed more often to images matching the target's relational category than to those that did not-even when those images involved completely different objects. In other words, participants searching for a phone in a basket were more likely to mistakenly respond to a knife in a cup than to a marker on a garbage can. Follow-up experiments ruled out strategic responses and also controlled for various confounding image features. We suggest that visual processing represents relations abstractly, in ways that separate roles from fillers.

当一块水果放在一个碗里,而碗放在一张桌子上时,我们欣赏的不仅是单个物体及其特征,还有从特定物体中抽象出来的 "容纳 "和 "支持 "关系。独立表示角色(如容器与支撑物)和这些角色的 "填充物"(如碗与杯子、桌子与椅子)是语言和高级推理的核心原则。但是,这种角色-填充物的独立性是否也会出现在自动视觉处理过程中呢?在这里,我们通过探索这种独立性可能产生的惊人错误,证明了它确实存在。在四项实验中,参与者看到的图像流包含了以力-动态关系排列的不同物体--例如,装在篮子里的手机、放在垃圾桶上的记号笔或放在杯子里的小刀。受试者必须在时间限制下,对一组干扰图像中的单个目标图像(如篮子里的手机)做出反应。令人惊讶的是,尽管受试者快速准确地完成了这项任务,但他们对与目标图像的关系类别相匹配的图像发出误报的频率却高于那些不匹配的图像--即使这些图像涉及的是完全不同的物体。换句话说,在篮子里寻找手机的受试者更有可能对杯子里的小刀做出错误反应,而不是对垃圾桶上的记号笔做出错误反应。后续实验排除了策略性反应,并控制了各种干扰图像特征。我们认为,视觉处理以抽象的方式表示关系,将角色与填充物区分开来。
{"title":"A Phone in a Basket Looks Like a Knife in a Cup: Role-Filler Independence in Visual Processing.","authors":"Alon Hafri, Michael F Bonner, Barbara Landau, Chaz Firestone","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00146","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00146","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When a piece of fruit is in a bowl, and the bowl is on a table, we appreciate not only the individual objects and their features, but also the relations <i>containment</i> and <i>support</i>, which abstract away from the particular objects involved. Independent representation of roles (e.g., containers vs. supporters) and \"fillers\" of those roles (e.g., bowls vs. cups, tables vs. chairs) is a core principle of language and higher-level reasoning. But does such role-filler independence also arise in automatic visual processing? Here, we show that it does, by exploring a surprising error that such independence can produce. In four experiments, participants saw a stream of images containing different objects arranged in force-dynamic relations-e.g., a phone contained in a basket, a marker resting on a garbage can, or a knife sitting in a cup. Participants had to respond to a single target image (e.g., a phone in a basket) within a stream of distractors presented under time constraints. Surprisingly, even though participants completed this task quickly and accurately, they false-alarmed more often to images matching the target's relational category than to those that did not-even when those images involved completely different objects. In other words, participants searching for a phone in a basket were more likely to mistakenly respond to a knife in a cup than to a marker on a garbage can. Follow-up experiments ruled out strategic responses and also controlled for various confounding image features. We suggest that visual processing represents relations abstractly, in ways that separate roles from fillers.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"766-794"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11219067/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unconscious Perception of Vernier Offsets. 对游标偏移的无意识感知。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-04 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00145
Pietro Amerio, Matthias Michel, Stephan Goerttler, Megan A K Peters, Axel Cleeremans

The comparison between conscious and unconscious perception is a cornerstone of consciousness science. However, most studies reporting above-chance discrimination of unseen stimuli do not control for criterion biases when assessing awareness. We tested whether observers can discriminate subjectively invisible offsets of Vernier stimuli when visibility is probed using a bias-free task. To reduce visibility, stimuli were either backward masked or presented for very brief durations (1-3 milliseconds) using a modern-day Tachistoscope. We found some behavioral indicators of perception without awareness, and yet, no conclusive evidence thereof. To seek more decisive proof, we simulated a series of Bayesian observer models, including some that produce visibility judgements alongside type-1 judgements. Our data are best accounted for by observers with slightly suboptimal conscious access to sensory evidence. Overall, the stimuli and visibility manipulations employed here induced mild instances of blindsight-like behavior, making them attractive candidates for future investigation of this phenomenon.

有意识感知和无意识感知之间的比较是意识科学的基石。然而,大多数研究在评估意识时,并没有控制标准偏差,而是报告了对未见刺激物的高于概率的辨别能力。我们测试了当使用无偏差任务探测可见性时,观察者是否能分辨出主观上不可见的维尼尔刺激偏移。为了降低可视性,我们使用现代的 Tachistoscope 对刺激物进行了向后遮蔽或非常短暂地呈现(1-3 毫秒)。我们发现了一些无意识感知的行为指标,但还没有确凿的证据。为了寻求更确凿的证据,我们模拟了一系列贝叶斯观察者模型,其中包括一些能在产生第一类判断的同时产生可见性判断的模型。我们的数据最好地解释了观察者有意识地获取感官证据的能力略微欠佳的情况。总之,本文所采用的刺激和可见性操作诱发了轻微的类似盲视的行为,使它们成为未来研究这种现象的有吸引力的候选对象。
{"title":"Unconscious Perception of Vernier Offsets.","authors":"Pietro Amerio, Matthias Michel, Stephan Goerttler, Megan A K Peters, Axel Cleeremans","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00145","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00145","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The comparison between conscious and unconscious perception is a cornerstone of consciousness science. However, most studies reporting above-chance discrimination of unseen stimuli do not control for criterion biases when assessing awareness. We tested whether observers can discriminate subjectively invisible offsets of Vernier stimuli when visibility is probed using a bias-free task. To reduce visibility, stimuli were either backward masked or presented for very brief durations (1-3 milliseconds) using a modern-day Tachistoscope. We found some behavioral indicators of perception without awareness, and yet, no conclusive evidence thereof. To seek more decisive proof, we simulated a series of Bayesian observer models, including some that produce visibility judgements alongside type-1 judgements. Our data are best accounted for by observers with slightly suboptimal conscious access to sensory evidence. Overall, the stimuli and visibility manipulations employed here induced mild instances of blindsight-like behavior, making them attractive candidates for future investigation of this phenomenon.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"739-765"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11185422/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421204","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Large Language Models and the Wisdom of Small Crowds. 大型语言模型与小众智慧。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-20 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00144
Sean Trott

Recent advances in Large Language Models (LLMs) have raised the question of replacing human subjects with LLM-generated data. While some believe that LLMs capture the "wisdom of the crowd"-due to their vast training data-empirical evidence for this hypothesis remains scarce. We present a novel methodological framework to test this: the "number needed to beat" (NNB), which measures how many humans are needed for a sample's quality to rival the quality achieved by GPT-4, a state-of-the-art LLM. In a series of pre-registered experiments, we collect novel human data and demonstrate the utility of this method for four psycholinguistic datasets for English. We find that NNB > 1 for each dataset, but also that NNB varies across tasks (and in some cases is quite small, e.g., 2). We also introduce two "centaur" methods for combining LLM and human data, which outperform both stand-alone LLMs and human samples. Finally, we analyze the trade-offs in data cost and quality for each approach. While clear limitations remain, we suggest that this framework could guide decision-making about whether and how to integrate LLM-generated data into the research pipeline.

大型语言模型(LLM)的最新进展提出了用 LLM 生成的数据取代人类研究对象的问题。虽然有些人认为 LLMs 可以捕捉到 "群众的智慧"--因为它们拥有大量的训练数据--但这一假设的经验证据仍然很少。我们提出了一个新颖的方法论框架来测试这一点:"需要击败的人数"(NNB),它衡量的是一个样本的质量需要多少人类才能与最先进的 LLM GPT-4 所达到的质量相媲美。在一系列预先注册的实验中,我们收集了新的人类数据,并在四个英语心理语言学数据集上证明了这种方法的实用性。我们发现,每个数据集的 NNB 均大于 1,但不同任务的 NNB 也各不相同(有些任务的 NNB 非常小,例如只有 2)。我们还介绍了两种结合 LLM 和人类数据的 "半人马 "方法,这两种方法都优于独立的 LLM 和人类样本。最后,我们分析了每种方法在数据成本和质量方面的权衡。虽然仍然存在明显的局限性,但我们认为这个框架可以指导决策,决定是否以及如何将 LLM 生成的数据整合到研究流水线中。
{"title":"Large Language Models and the Wisdom of Small Crowds.","authors":"Sean Trott","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00144","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00144","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent advances in Large Language Models (LLMs) have raised the question of replacing human subjects with LLM-generated data. While some believe that LLMs capture the \"wisdom of the crowd\"-due to their vast training data-empirical evidence for this hypothesis remains scarce. We present a novel methodological framework to test this: the \"number needed to beat\" (NNB), which measures how many humans are needed for a sample's quality to rival the quality achieved by GPT-4, a state-of-the-art LLM. In a series of pre-registered experiments, we collect novel human data and demonstrate the utility of this method for four psycholinguistic datasets for English. We find that NNB > 1 for each dataset, but also that NNB varies across tasks (and in some cases is quite small, e.g., 2). We also introduce two \"centaur\" methods for combining LLM and human data, which outperform both stand-alone LLMs and human samples. Finally, we analyze the trade-offs in data cost and quality for each approach. While clear limitations remain, we suggest that this framework could guide decision-making about whether and how to integrate LLM-generated data into the research pipeline.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"723-738"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11142632/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Do You Know If You Were Mind Wandering? Dissociating Explicit Memories of Off Task Thought From Subjective Feelings of Inattention. 如何知道自己是否在胡思乱想?从注意力不集中的主观感觉中分离出任务外思维的显性记忆。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-10 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00142
Nathan K Mathews, Umer Bin Faiz, Nicholaus P Brosowsky

Mind wandering is a common experience in which your attention drifts away from the task at hand and toward task-unrelated thoughts. To measure mind wandering we typically use experience sampling and retrospective self-reports, which require participants to make metacognitive judgments about their immediately preceding attentional states. In the current study, we aimed to better understand how people come to make such judgments by introducing a novel distinction between explicit memories of off task thought and subjective feelings of inattention. Across two preregistered experiments, we found that participants often indicated they were "off task" and yet had no memory of the content of their thoughts-though, they were less common than remembered experiences. Critically, remembered experiences of mind wandering and subjective feelings of inattention differed in their behavioral correlates. In Experiment 1, we found that only the frequency of remembered mind wandering varied with task demands. In contrast, only subjective feelings of inattention were associated with poor performance (Experiments 1 and 2) and individual differences in executive functioning (Experiment 2). These results suggest that the phenomenology of mind wandering may differ depending on how the experiences are brought about (e.g., executive functioning errors versus excess attentional resources), and provide preliminary evidence of the importance of measuring subjective feelings of inattention when assessing mind wandering.

思维游离是一种常见的体验,即注意力从手头的任务转移到与任务无关的想法上。为了测量思维游离,我们通常会使用经验取样和回顾性自我报告,这需要参与者对他们之前的注意力状态进行元认知判断。在当前的研究中,我们引入了一种新的方法来区分任务外思维的明确记忆和注意力不集中的主观感受,旨在更好地了解人们是如何做出这种判断的。在两个预先登记的实验中,我们发现参与者经常表示他们 "偏离了任务",但却对他们的想法内容没有任何记忆--尽管这些记忆比记忆中的经历要少。重要的是,记忆中的思想游离体验和主观上的注意力不集中感觉在行为相关性上有所不同。在实验 1 中,我们发现只有记忆中的思绪游移频率会随任务要求而变化。相反,只有注意力不集中的主观感受与成绩不佳(实验 1 和 2)和执行功能的个体差异(实验 2)相关。这些结果表明,思维游离的现象学可能会因体验的产生方式(如执行功能错误或注意力资源过剩)而有所不同,并初步证明了在评估思维游离时测量注意力不集中的主观感受的重要性。
{"title":"How Do You Know If You Were Mind Wandering? Dissociating Explicit Memories of Off Task Thought From Subjective Feelings of Inattention.","authors":"Nathan K Mathews, Umer Bin Faiz, Nicholaus P Brosowsky","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00142","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00142","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mind wandering is a common experience in which your attention drifts away from the task at hand and toward task-unrelated thoughts. To measure mind wandering we typically use experience sampling and retrospective self-reports, which require participants to make metacognitive judgments about their immediately preceding attentional states. In the current study, we aimed to better understand how people come to make such judgments by introducing a novel distinction between explicit memories of off task thought and subjective feelings of inattention. Across two preregistered experiments, we found that participants often indicated they were \"off task\" and yet had no memory of the content of their thoughts-though, they were less common than remembered experiences. Critically, remembered experiences of mind wandering and subjective feelings of inattention differed in their behavioral correlates. In Experiment 1, we found that only the frequency of remembered mind wandering varied with task demands. In contrast, only subjective feelings of inattention were associated with poor performance (Experiments 1 and 2) and individual differences in executive functioning (Experiment 2). These results suggest that the phenomenology of mind wandering may differ depending on how the experiences are brought about (e.g., executive functioning errors versus excess attentional resources), and provide preliminary evidence of the importance of measuring subjective feelings of inattention when assessing mind wandering.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"666-687"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11142633/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141200924","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Instruction on the Scientific Method Provides (Some) Protection Against Illusions of Causality. 关于科学方法的教学可(在一定程度上)防止因果关系的错觉。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-10 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00141
Julie Y L Chow, Micah B Goldwater, Ben Colagiuri, Evan J Livesey

People tend to overestimate the efficacy of an ineffective treatment when they experience the treatment and its supposed outcome co-occurring frequently. This is referred to as the outcome density effect. Here, we attempted to improve the accuracy of participants' assessments of an ineffective treatment by instructing them about the scientific practice of comparing treatment effects against a relevant base-rate, i.e., when no treatment is delivered. The effect of these instructions was assessed in both a trial-by-trial contingency learning task, where cue administration was either decided by the participant (Experiments 1 & 2) or pre-determined by the experimenter (Experiment 3), as well as in summary format where all information was presented on a single screen (Experiment 4). Overall, we found two means by which base-rate instructions influence efficacy ratings for the ineffective treatment: 1) When information was presented sequentially, the benefit of base-rate instructions on illusory belief was mediated by reduced sampling of cue-present trials, and 2) When information was presented in summary format, we found a direct effect of base-rate instruction on reducing causal illusion. Together, these findings suggest that simple instructions on the scientific method were able to decrease participants' (over-)weighting of cue-outcome coincidences when making causal judgements, as well as decrease their tendency to over-sample cue-present events. However, the effect of base-rate instructions on correcting illusory beliefs was incomplete, and participants still showed illusory causal judgements when the probability of the outcome occurring was high. Thus, simple textual information about assessing causal relationships is partially effective in influencing people's judgements of treatment efficacy, suggesting an important role of scientific instruction in debiasing cognitive errors.

当人们经常经历无效治疗及其假定结果的同时出现时,他们往往会高估无效治疗的疗效。这就是所谓的结果密度效应。在这里,我们试图通过指导参与者了解将治疗效果与相关基数(即未进行治疗时)进行比较的科学实践,来提高参与者对无效治疗评估的准确性。我们在逐次试验的权变学习任务中对这些指导的效果进行了评估,在该任务中,提示给定要么由参与者决定(实验 1 和 2),要么由实验者预先确定(实验 3);此外,我们还以摘要形式评估了这些指导的效果,在该任务中,所有信息都呈现在一个屏幕上(实验 4)。总的来说,我们发现基率指示对无效治疗的疗效评分有两种影响方式:1)当信息按顺序呈现时,基率指示对幻觉信念的益处是通过减少对提示出现的试验的取样来中介的;2)当信息以摘要形式呈现时,我们发现基率指示对减少因果幻觉有直接影响。这些发现共同表明,关于科学方法的简单指导能够降低被试在进行因果判断时对线索-结果巧合的(过度)权重,并降低他们对线索出现事件的过度采样倾向。然而,基率指示对纠正虚幻信念的作用并不完全,当结果发生的概率很高时,参与者仍然会表现出虚幻的因果判断。因此,关于评估因果关系的简单文字信息对影响人们对治疗效果的判断有部分作用,这表明科学指导在消除认知错误方面发挥着重要作用。
{"title":"Instruction on the Scientific Method Provides (Some) Protection Against Illusions of Causality.","authors":"Julie Y L Chow, Micah B Goldwater, Ben Colagiuri, Evan J Livesey","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00141","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00141","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People tend to overestimate the efficacy of an ineffective treatment when they experience the treatment and its supposed outcome co-occurring frequently. This is referred to as the <i>outcome density</i> effect. Here, we attempted to improve the accuracy of participants' assessments of an ineffective treatment by instructing them about the scientific practice of comparing treatment effects against a relevant base-rate, i.e., when no treatment is delivered. The effect of these instructions was assessed in both a trial-by-trial contingency learning task, where cue administration was either decided by the participant (Experiments 1 & 2) or pre-determined by the experimenter (Experiment 3), as well as in summary format where all information was presented on a single screen (Experiment 4). Overall, we found two means by which base-rate instructions influence efficacy ratings for the ineffective treatment: 1) When information was presented sequentially, the benefit of base-rate instructions on illusory belief was mediated by reduced sampling of cue-present trials, and 2) When information was presented in summary format, we found a <i>direct</i> effect of base-rate instruction on reducing causal illusion. Together, these findings suggest that simple instructions on the scientific method were able to decrease participants' (over-)weighting of cue-outcome coincidences when making causal judgements, as well as decrease their tendency to over-sample cue-present events. However, the effect of base-rate instructions on correcting illusory beliefs was incomplete, and participants still showed illusory causal judgements when the probability of the outcome occurring was high. Thus, simple textual information about assessing causal relationships is partially effective in influencing people's judgements of treatment efficacy, suggesting an important role of scientific instruction in debiasing cognitive errors.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"639-665"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11142631/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141200925","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Open Mind
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1