首页 > 最新文献

Open Mind最新文献

英文 中文
Preliminary Evidence for Global Properties in Human Listeners During Natural Auditory Scene Perception. 人类听者在自然听觉场景感知过程中全局属性的初步证据
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00131
Margaret A McMullin, Rohit Kumar, Nathan C Higgins, Brian Gygi, Mounya Elhilali, Joel S Snyder

Theories of auditory and visual scene analysis suggest the perception of scenes relies on the identification and segregation of objects within it, resembling a detail-oriented processing style. However, a more global process may occur while analyzing scenes, which has been evidenced in the visual domain. It is our understanding that a similar line of research has not been explored in the auditory domain; therefore, we evaluated the contributions of high-level global and low-level acoustic information to auditory scene perception. An additional aim was to increase the field's ecological validity by using and making available a new collection of high-quality auditory scenes. Participants rated scenes on 8 global properties (e.g., open vs. enclosed) and an acoustic analysis evaluated which low-level features predicted the ratings. We submitted the acoustic measures and average ratings of the global properties to separate exploratory factor analyses (EFAs). The EFA of the acoustic measures revealed a seven-factor structure explaining 57% of the variance in the data, while the EFA of the global property measures revealed a two-factor structure explaining 64% of the variance in the data. Regression analyses revealed each global property was predicted by at least one acoustic variable (R2 = 0.33-0.87). These findings were extended using deep neural network models where we examined correlations between human ratings of global properties and deep embeddings of two computational models: an object-based model and a scene-based model. The results support that participants' ratings are more strongly explained by a global analysis of the scene setting, though the relationship between scene perception and auditory perception is multifaceted, with differing correlation patterns evident between the two models. Taken together, our results provide evidence for the ability to perceive auditory scenes from a global perspective. Some of the acoustic measures predicted ratings of global scene perception, suggesting representations of auditory objects may be transformed through many stages of processing in the ventral auditory stream, similar to what has been proposed in the ventral visual stream. These findings and the open availability of our scene collection will make future studies on perception, attention, and memory for natural auditory scenes possible.

听觉和视觉场景分析理论认为,对场景的感知依赖于识别和分离场景中的物体,这类似于一种以细节为导向的处理方式。然而,在分析场景时可能会出现更全面的过程,这在视觉领域已得到证实。据我们了解,听觉领域还没有类似的研究;因此,我们评估了高层次的全局信息和低层次的声音信息对听觉场景感知的贡献。另一个目的是通过使用和提供新的高质量听觉场景集来提高该领域的生态有效性。参与者对场景的 8 个全局属性(如开放与封闭)进行评分,声学分析评估了哪些低层次特征可以预测评分。我们将声学测量结果和总体属性的平均评分分别进行了探索性因子分析(EFAs)。声学测量的 EFA 发现了一个七因子结构,可以解释数据中 57% 的方差,而总体属性测量的 EFA 发现了一个双因子结构,可以解释数据中 64% 的方差。回归分析表明,每个全局属性至少可以由一个声学变量预测(R2 = 0.33-0.87)。我们使用深度神经网络模型对这些发现进行了扩展,检验了人类对全局属性的评分与两个计算模型(基于物体的模型和基于场景的模型)的深度嵌入之间的相关性。尽管场景感知与听觉感知之间的关系是多方面的,两种模型之间的相关模式也不尽相同,但我们的结果表明,对场景设置的全局分析能更有力地解释参与者的评分。总之,我们的研究结果为从全局角度感知听觉场景的能力提供了证据。一些声学测量结果预测了对全局场景感知的评分,这表明听觉对象的表征可能会在腹侧听觉流的多个处理阶段中发生转变,这与在腹侧视觉流中提出的观点类似。这些发现以及我们场景收集的开放性将使未来对自然听觉场景的感知、注意力和记忆的研究成为可能。
{"title":"Preliminary Evidence for Global Properties in Human Listeners During Natural Auditory Scene Perception.","authors":"Margaret A McMullin, Rohit Kumar, Nathan C Higgins, Brian Gygi, Mounya Elhilali, Joel S Snyder","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00131","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00131","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories of auditory and visual scene analysis suggest the perception of scenes relies on the identification and segregation of objects within it, resembling a detail-oriented processing style. However, a more global process may occur while analyzing scenes, which has been evidenced in the visual domain. It is our understanding that a similar line of research has not been explored in the auditory domain; therefore, we evaluated the contributions of high-level global and low-level acoustic information to auditory scene perception. An additional aim was to increase the field's ecological validity by using and making available a new collection of high-quality auditory scenes. Participants rated scenes on 8 global properties (e.g., open vs. enclosed) and an acoustic analysis evaluated which low-level features predicted the ratings. We submitted the acoustic measures and average ratings of the global properties to separate exploratory factor analyses (EFAs). The EFA of the acoustic measures revealed a seven-factor structure explaining 57% of the variance in the data, while the EFA of the global property measures revealed a two-factor structure explaining 64% of the variance in the data. Regression analyses revealed each global property was predicted by at least one acoustic variable (R<sup>2</sup> = 0.33-0.87). These findings were extended using deep neural network models where we examined correlations between human ratings of global properties and deep embeddings of two computational models: an object-based model and a scene-based model. The results support that participants' ratings are more strongly explained by a global analysis of the scene setting, though the relationship between scene perception and auditory perception is multifaceted, with differing correlation patterns evident between the two models. Taken together, our results provide evidence for the ability to perceive auditory scenes from a global perspective. Some of the acoustic measures predicted ratings of global scene perception, suggesting representations of auditory objects may be transformed through many stages of processing in the ventral auditory stream, similar to what has been proposed in the ventral visual stream. These findings and the open availability of our scene collection will make future studies on perception, attention, and memory for natural auditory scenes possible.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"333-365"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10990578/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140872348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multiple Object Tracking Without Pre-attentive Indexing. 无需预注意力索引的多目标跟踪
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00128
Shubhamkar Ayare, Nisheeth Srivastava

Multiple object tracking (MOT) involves simultaneous tracking of a certain number of target objects amongst a larger set of objects as they all move unpredictably over time. The prevalent explanation for successful target tracking by humans in MOT involving visually identical objects is based on the Visual Indexing Theory. This assumes that each target is indexed by a pointer using a non-conceptual mechanism to maintain an object's identity even as its properties change over time. Thus, successful tracking requires successful indexing and the absence of identification errors. Identity maintenance and successful tracking are measured in terms of identification (ID) and tracking accuracy respectively, with higher accuracy indicating better identity maintenance or better tracking. Existing evidence suggests that humans have high tracking accuracy despite poor identification accuracy, suggesting that it might be possible to perform MOT without indexing. Our work adds to existing evidence for this position through two experiments, and presents a computational model of multiple object tracking that does not require indexes. Our empirical results show that identification accuracy is aligned with tracking accuracy in humans for tracking up to three, but is lower when tracking more objects. Our computational model of MOT without indexing accounts for several empirical tracking accuracy patterns shown in earlier studies, reproduces the dissociation between tracking and identification accuracy produced earlier in the literature as well as in our experiments, and makes several novel predictions.

多目标跟踪(MOT)是指在一组较大的物体中同时跟踪一定数量的目标物体,因为这些物体都会随着时间发生不可预知的移动。在涉及视觉相同物体的多目标追踪中,人类成功追踪目标的普遍解释是基于视觉索引理论。该理论假定,每个目标都由一个指针进行索引,该指针使用一种非概念机制来保持物体的身份,即使其属性随时间发生变化。因此,成功的追踪需要成功的索引和无识别错误。身份维护和成功追踪分别以识别(ID)和追踪准确度来衡量,准确度越高,表明身份维护越好或追踪越好。现有证据表明,尽管人类的识别准确率较低,但其追踪准确率却很高,这表明人类有可能在不进行索引的情况下完成 MOT。我们的研究通过两个实验为这一观点提供了更多证据,并提出了一个不需要索引的多目标跟踪计算模型。我们的实证结果表明,在追踪最多三个物体时,识别准确率与人类的追踪准确率一致,但当追踪更多物体时,识别准确率就会降低。我们的无索引 MOT 计算模型解释了早期研究中显示的几种经验追踪准确性模式,再现了早期文献和我们的实验中出现的追踪和识别准确性之间的分离,并做出了几项新的预测。
{"title":"Multiple Object Tracking Without Pre-attentive Indexing.","authors":"Shubhamkar Ayare, Nisheeth Srivastava","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00128","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00128","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multiple object tracking (MOT) involves simultaneous tracking of a certain number of target objects amongst a larger set of objects as they all move unpredictably over time. The prevalent explanation for successful target tracking by humans in MOT involving visually identical objects is based on the Visual Indexing Theory. This assumes that each target is indexed by a pointer using a non-conceptual mechanism to maintain an object's identity even as its properties change over time. Thus, successful tracking requires successful indexing and the absence of identification errors. Identity maintenance and successful tracking are measured in terms of identification (ID) and tracking accuracy respectively, with higher accuracy indicating better identity maintenance or better tracking. Existing evidence suggests that humans have high tracking accuracy despite poor identification accuracy, suggesting that it might be possible to perform MOT without indexing. Our work adds to existing evidence for this position through two experiments, and presents a computational model of multiple object tracking that does not require indexes. Our empirical results show that identification accuracy is aligned with tracking accuracy in humans for tracking up to three, but is lower when tracking more objects. Our computational model of MOT without indexing accounts for several empirical tracking accuracy patterns shown in earlier studies, reproduces the dissociation between tracking and identification accuracy produced earlier in the literature as well as in our experiments, and makes several novel predictions.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"278-308"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10990572/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Response to Difficulty Drives Variation in IQ Test Performance. 对难度的反应导致智商测试成绩的差异。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00127
Samuel J Cheyette, Steven T Piantadosi

In a large (N = 300), pre-registered experiment and data analysis model, we find that individual variation in overall performance on Raven's Progressive Matrices is substantially driven by differential strategizing in the face of difficulty. Some participants choose to spend more time on hard problems while others choose to spend less and these differences explain about 42% of the variance in overall performance. In a data analysis jointly predicting participants' reaction times and accuracy on each item, we find that the Raven's task captures at most half of participants' variation in time-controlled ability (48%) down to almost none (3%), depending on which notion of ability is assumed. Our results highlight the role that confounding factors such as motivation play in explaining individuals' differential performance in IQ testing.

在一个大型(N = 300)预注册实验和数据分析模型中,我们发现,瑞文渐进矩阵总体成绩的个体差异在很大程度上是由面对困难时的策略差异造成的。一些参与者选择在难题上花费更多时间,而另一些则选择花费较少时间,这些差异解释了总体成绩差异的 42%。在联合预测参与者在每个项目上的反应时间和准确性的数据分析中,我们发现瑞文任务最多能捕捉到参与者在时间控制能力方面的一半差异(48%),而根据假设的能力概念,则几乎捕捉不到任何差异(3%)。我们的研究结果凸显了动机等干扰因素在解释个人在智商测试中的不同表现时所起的作用。
{"title":"Response to Difficulty Drives Variation in IQ Test Performance.","authors":"Samuel J Cheyette, Steven T Piantadosi","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00127","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00127","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a large (<i>N</i> = 300), pre-registered experiment and data analysis model, we find that individual variation in overall performance on Raven's Progressive Matrices is substantially driven by differential strategizing in the face of difficulty. Some participants choose to spend more time on hard problems while others choose to spend less and these differences explain about 42% of the variance in overall performance. In a data analysis jointly predicting participants' reaction times and accuracy on each item, we find that the Raven's task captures at most half of participants' variation in time-controlled ability (48%) down to almost none (3%), depending on which notion of ability is assumed. Our results highlight the role that confounding factors such as motivation play in explaining individuals' differential performance in IQ testing.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"265-277"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10990577/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140858301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It's All in the Interaction: Early Acquired Words Are Both Frequent and Highly Imageable. 一切都在互动中:早期习得的单词既频繁又高度形象化
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00130
Joseph R Coffey, Margarita Zeitlin, Jean Crawford, Jesse Snedeker

Prior studies have found that children are more likely to learn words that are frequent in the input and highly imageable. Many theories of word learning, however, predict that these variables should interact, particularly early in development: frequency of a form is of little use if you cannot infer its meaning, and a concrete word cannot be acquired if you never hear it. The present study explores this interaction, how it changes over time and its relationship to syntactic category effects in children acquiring American English. We analyzed 1461 monolingual English-speaking children aged 1;4-2;6 from the MB-CDI norming study (Fenson et al., 1994). Word frequency was estimated from the CHILDES database, and imageability was measured using adult ratings. There was a strong over-additive interaction between frequency and imageability, such that children were more likely to learn a word if it was both highly imageable and very frequent. This interaction was larger in younger children than in older children. There were reliable differences between syntactic categories independent of frequency and imageability, which did not interact with age. These findings are consistent with theories in which children's early words are acquired by mapping frequent word forms onto concrete, perceptually available referents, such that highly frequent items are only acquired if they are also imageable, and vice versa.

先前的研究发现,儿童更有可能学习输入频率高、形象性强的单词。然而,许多单词学习理论预测,这些变量应该是相互作用的,尤其是在发展的早期:如果不能推断词义,那么词形的频率就没有什么用处;如果从未听说过一个具体的单词,那么这个单词就无法习得。本研究探讨了儿童在学习美式英语过程中的这种相互作用、这种相互作用随时间的变化及其与句法类别效应之间的关系。我们分析了来自 MB-CDI 标准研究(Fenson 等人,1994 年)的 1461 名 1;4-2;6 岁单语英语儿童。单词频率是通过 CHILDES 数据库估算的,形象性则是通过成人评分来测量的。词频和形象性之间存在很强的超加交互作用,因此,如果一个词的形象性很高而且词频很高,那么儿童就更有可能学会这个词。这种交互作用在年龄较小的儿童身上比在年龄较大的儿童身上更大。句法类别之间存在着独立于频率和形象性的可靠差异,这种差异与年龄无关。这些研究结果与以下理论相一致:儿童早期词汇的习得是通过将频繁出现的词形映射到具体的、可感知的参照物上实现的,因此高频词只有在可形象化的情况下才能习得,反之亦然。
{"title":"It's All in the Interaction: Early Acquired Words Are Both Frequent and Highly Imageable.","authors":"Joseph R Coffey, Margarita Zeitlin, Jean Crawford, Jesse Snedeker","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00130","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00130","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior studies have found that children are more likely to learn words that are frequent in the input and highly imageable. Many theories of word learning, however, predict that these variables should interact, particularly early in development: frequency of a form is of little use if you cannot infer its meaning, and a concrete word cannot be acquired if you never hear it. The present study explores this interaction, how it changes over time and its relationship to syntactic category effects in children acquiring American English. We analyzed 1461 monolingual English-speaking children aged 1;4-2;6 from the MB-CDI norming study (Fenson et al., 1994). Word frequency was estimated from the CHILDES database, and imageability was measured using adult ratings. There was a strong over-additive interaction between frequency and imageability, such that children were more likely to learn a word if it was both highly imageable and very frequent. This interaction was larger in younger children than in older children. There were reliable differences between syntactic categories independent of frequency and imageability, which did not interact with age. These findings are consistent with theories in which children's early words are acquired by mapping frequent word forms onto concrete, perceptually available referents, such that highly frequent items are only acquired if they are also imageable, and vice versa.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"309-332"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10990573/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140868360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What Does That Mean? Complementizers and Epistemic Authority. 这意味着什么?补充者与认识论权威》。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00135
Rebecca Tollan, Bilge Palaz

A core goal of research in language is to understand the factors that guide choice of linguistic form where more than one option is syntactically well-formed. We discuss one case of optionality that has generated longstanding discussion: the choice of either using or dropping the English complementizer that in sentences like I think (that) the cat followed the dog. Existing psycholinguistic analyses tie that-usage to production pressures associated with sentence planning (Ferreira & Dell, 2000), avoidance of ambiguity (Hawkins, 2004), and relative information density (Jaeger, 2010). Building on observations from cross-linguistic fieldwork, we present a novel proposal in which English that can serve to mark a speaker's "epistemic authority" over the information packaged within the embedded clause; that is, it indicates that the speaker has more knowledge of the embedded proposition compared with their addressee and thus has a perspective that they believe their addressee doesn't share. Testing this proposal with a forced-choice task and a series of corpus surveys, we find that English that is keyed to the use of embedded speaker (first-person) subject pronouns and occurs in sentences containing newsworthy information. Our account of that-optionality takes into account why that is associated with both (i) a dense information signal and (ii) semantic-pragmatic content, as well as extending to cases of non-optionality in subject/sentence-initial clauses (e.g., *(That) the cat is following the dog, I already know) and fragment answers (e.g., What do you already know? *(That) the cat is following the dog), where that is required.

语言研究的一个核心目标是了解在句法上有不止一种选择的情况下,指导语言形式选择的因素。我们将讨论一个引起长期讨论的选择性案例:在 I think (that) the cat followed the dog 这样的句子中选择使用或放弃英语补语 that。现有的心理语言学分析将that的使用与句子规划(Ferreira & Dell, 2000)、避免歧义(Hawkins, 2004)和相对信息密度(Jaeger, 2010)相关的生产压力联系起来。基于跨语言实地调查的观察结果,我们提出了一个新颖的建议,即英语中的 "that "可以用来标记说话者对嵌入式分句中的信息的 "认识权威";也就是说,它表明说话者比其收信人对嵌入式命题有更多的了解,因此拥有他们认为收信人所不具备的观点。通过强迫选择任务和一系列语料库调查对这一提议进行检验,我们发现,英语中的that-optionality与嵌入式说话人(第一人称)主语代词的使用有关,并且出现在包含有新闻价值信息的句子中。我们对that-optionality的解释考虑到了为什么that与(i)密集的信息信号和(ii)语义-语用内容相关联,并扩展到主语/句子首句中的非optionality情况(例如:*(That) the cat is following the dog, I already know)和片段回答(例如:What do you already know?*(That) the cat is following the dog)中的主语/句首句和片段回答(如:What do you already know?
{"title":"What Does <i>That</i> Mean? Complementizers and Epistemic Authority.","authors":"Rebecca Tollan, Bilge Palaz","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00135","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00135","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A core goal of research in language is to understand the factors that guide choice of linguistic form where more than one option is syntactically well-formed. We discuss one case of optionality that has generated longstanding discussion: the choice of either using or dropping the English complementizer <i>that</i> in sentences like <i>I think (that) the cat followed the dog</i>. Existing psycholinguistic analyses tie <i>that</i>-usage to production pressures associated with sentence planning (Ferreira & Dell, 2000), avoidance of ambiguity (Hawkins, 2004), and relative information density (Jaeger, 2010). Building on observations from cross-linguistic fieldwork, we present a novel proposal in which English <i>that</i> can serve to mark a speaker's \"epistemic authority\" over the information packaged within the embedded clause; that is, it indicates that the speaker has more knowledge of the embedded proposition compared with their addressee and thus has a perspective that they believe their addressee doesn't share. Testing this proposal with a forced-choice task and a series of corpus surveys, we find that English <i>that</i> is keyed to the use of embedded speaker (first-person) subject pronouns and occurs in sentences containing newsworthy information. Our account of <i>that</i>-optionality takes into account why <i>that</i> is associated with both (i) a dense information signal and (ii) semantic-pragmatic content, as well as extending to cases of non-optionality in subject/sentence-initial clauses (e.g., *<i>(That) the cat is following the dog, I already know</i>) and fragment answers (e.g., <i>What do you already know?</i> *<i>(That) the cat is following the dog</i>), where <i>that</i> is required.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"366-394"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10990574/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140863179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants Infer Social Relationships Between Individuals Who Engage in Imitative Social Interactions. 婴儿推断参与模仿性社会互动的个体之间的社会关系。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-05 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00124
Vanessa Kudrnova, Elizabeth S Spelke, Ashley J Thomas

Infants are born into rich social networks and are faced with the challenge of learning about them. When infants observe social interactions, they make predictions about future behavior, but it is not clear whether these predictions are based on social dispositions, social relationships, or both. The current studies (N = 188, N = 90 males) address this question in 12-month-old infants and 16- to 18-month-old toddlers who observe social interactions involving imitation. In Studies 1 and 3, infants and toddlers expected that imitators, compared to non-imitators, would respond to their social partners' distress. Likewise, they expected the targets of imitation, compared to non-targets, to respond to their partner's distress. In Study 2, these expectations did not generalize to interactions with a new partner, providing evidence that infants learned about the relationships between individuals as opposed to their dispositions. In Study 3, infants did not make predictions about responses to laughter, suggesting that infants see imitation as indicative of a specific kind of social relationship. Together, these results provide evidence that imitative interactions support infants' and toddlers' learning about the social relationships connecting unknown individuals.

婴儿一出生就生活在丰富的社会网络中,他们面临着了解这些网络的挑战。当婴儿观察社会互动时,他们会对未来的行为做出预测,但目前还不清楚这些预测是基于社会倾向、社会关系还是两者兼而有之。目前的研究(N = 188,N = 90 名男性)针对 12 个月大的婴儿和 16 至 18 个月大的学步儿童观察涉及模仿的社会互动来探讨这个问题。在研究 1 和研究 3 中,婴儿和学步儿童预期模仿者与非模仿者相比,会对其社会伙伴的痛苦做出反应。同样,与非模仿对象相比,他们也预期模仿对象会对同伴的痛苦做出反应。在研究 2 中,这些预期并没有推广到与新伙伴的互动中,这证明婴儿学习的是个体之间的关系,而不是他们的倾向。在研究 3 中,婴儿没有对笑声的反应做出预测,这表明婴儿将模仿视为一种特定社会关系的标志。这些结果共同证明,模仿性互动有助于婴幼儿学习连接未知个体的社会关系。
{"title":"Infants Infer Social Relationships Between Individuals Who Engage in Imitative Social Interactions.","authors":"Vanessa Kudrnova, Elizabeth S Spelke, Ashley J Thomas","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00124","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00124","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Infants are born into rich social networks and are faced with the challenge of learning about them. When infants observe social interactions, they make predictions about future behavior, but it is not clear whether these predictions are based on social dispositions, social relationships, or both. The current studies (N = 188, N = 90 males) address this question in 12-month-old infants and 16- to 18-month-old toddlers who observe social interactions involving imitation. In Studies 1 and 3, infants and toddlers expected that imitators, compared to non-imitators, would respond to their social partners' distress. Likewise, they expected the targets of imitation, compared to non-targets, to respond to their partner's distress. In Study 2, these expectations did not generalize to interactions with a new partner, providing evidence that infants learned about the relationships between individuals as opposed to their dispositions. In Study 3, infants did not make predictions about responses to laughter, suggesting that infants see imitation as indicative of a specific kind of social relationship. Together, these results provide evidence that imitative interactions support infants' and toddlers' learning about the social relationships connecting unknown individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"202-216"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10932586/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140112614","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word Frequency and Predictability Dissociate in Naturalistic Reading. 自然阅读中的词频与可预测性脱节。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-05 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00119
Cory Shain

Many studies of human language processing have shown that readers slow down at less frequent or less predictable words, but there is debate about whether frequency and predictability effects reflect separable cognitive phenomena: are cognitive operations that retrieve words from the mental lexicon based on sensory cues distinct from those that predict upcoming words based on context? Previous evidence for a frequency-predictability dissociation is mostly based on small samples (both for estimating predictability and frequency and for testing their effects on human behavior), artificial materials (e.g., isolated constructed sentences), and implausible modeling assumptions (discrete-time dynamics, linearity, additivity, constant variance, and invariance over time), which raises the question: do frequency and predictability dissociate in ordinary language comprehension, such as story reading? This study leverages recent progress in open data and computational modeling to address this question at scale. A large collection of naturalistic reading data (six datasets, >2.2 M datapoints) is analyzed using nonlinear continuous-time regression, and frequency and predictability are estimated using statistical language models trained on more data than is currently typical in psycholinguistics. Despite the use of naturalistic data, strong predictability estimates, and flexible regression models, results converge with earlier experimental studies in supporting dissociable and additive frequency and predictability effects.

许多关于人类语言处理的研究都表明,读者在阅读频率较低或可预测性较低的单词时会放慢阅读速度,但关于频率和可预测性效应是否反映了可分离的认知现象还存在争议:根据感觉线索从心理词典中检索单词的认知操作与根据上下文预测即将出现的单词的认知操作是否不同?以往关于频率-可预测性分离的证据大多基于小样本(既用于估计可预测性和频率,也用于测试它们对人类行为的影响)、人工材料(如孤立的结构化句子)和难以置信的建模假设(离散时间动态、线性、可加性、恒方差和随时间变化的不变性),这就提出了一个问题:在普通语言理解(如故事阅读)中,频率和可预测性是否分离?本研究利用开放数据和计算建模方面的最新进展来大规模解决这一问题。我们使用非线性连续时间回归分析了大量的自然阅读数据(六个数据集,超过 220 万个数据点),并使用统计语言模型对频率和可预测性进行了估计,这些模型是在比目前心理语言学中典型的更多数据基础上训练而成的。尽管使用了自然数据、较强的可预测性估计和灵活的回归模型,但结果与早期的实验研究一致,支持频率和可预测性效应的可分性和可加性。
{"title":"Word Frequency and Predictability Dissociate in Naturalistic Reading.","authors":"Cory Shain","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00119","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00119","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many studies of human language processing have shown that readers slow down at less frequent or less predictable words, but there is debate about whether frequency and predictability effects reflect separable cognitive phenomena: are cognitive operations that retrieve words from the mental lexicon based on sensory cues distinct from those that predict upcoming words based on context? Previous evidence for a frequency-predictability dissociation is mostly based on small samples (both for estimating predictability and frequency and for testing their effects on human behavior), artificial materials (e.g., isolated constructed sentences), and implausible modeling assumptions (discrete-time dynamics, linearity, additivity, constant variance, and invariance over time), which raises the question: do frequency and predictability dissociate in ordinary language comprehension, such as story reading? This study leverages recent progress in open data and computational modeling to address this question at scale. A large collection of naturalistic reading data (six datasets, >2.2 M datapoints) is analyzed using nonlinear continuous-time regression, and frequency and predictability are estimated using statistical language models trained on more data than is currently typical in psycholinguistics. Despite the use of naturalistic data, strong predictability estimates, and flexible regression models, results converge with earlier experimental studies in supporting dissociable and additive frequency and predictability effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"177-201"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10932590/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140111558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Signal Smoothing and Syntactic Choices: A Critical Reflection on the UID Hypothesis. 信号平滑与句法选择:对 UID 假设的批判性反思。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-05 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00125
Tom S Juzek

The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis explains variations in syllable length as a means to more uniformly distribute information throughout the speech signal. The Uniform Information Density hypothesis seeks to generalize this to choices on all linguistic levels, particularly syntactic choices. While there is some evidence for the Uniform Information Density hypothesis, it faces several challenges, four of which are discussed in this paper. First, it is not clear what exactly counts as uniform. Second, there are syntactic alternations that occur systematically but that can cause notable fluctuations in the information signature. Third, there is an increasing body of negative results. Fourth, there is a lack of large-scale evidence. As to the fourth point, this paper provides a broader array of data-936 sentence pairs for nine syntactic constructions-and analyzes them in a test setup that treats the hypothesis as a classifier. For our data, the Uniform Information Density hypothesis showed little predictive capacity. We explore ways to reconcile our data with theory.

平滑信号冗余假说将音节长度的变化解释为在整个语音信号中更均匀地分配信息的一种手段。统一信息密度假说试图将此推广到所有语言层面的选择,尤其是句法选择。虽然 "统一信息密度 "假说有一些证据,但它也面临着一些挑战,本文将讨论其中的四项。首先,不清楚究竟什么才算统一。其次,有些句法交替是系统发生的,但会导致信息特征的明显波动。第三,负面结果越来越多。第四,缺乏大规模的证据。关于第四点,本文提供了一系列更广泛的数据--九种句法结构的 936 对句子--并在将假设视为分类器的测试设置中对其进行了分析。对于我们的数据,统一信息密度假说几乎没有显示出预测能力。我们探讨了将数据与理论相协调的方法。
{"title":"Signal Smoothing and Syntactic Choices: A Critical Reflection on the UID Hypothesis.","authors":"Tom S Juzek","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00125","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00125","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis explains variations in syllable length as a means to more uniformly distribute information throughout the speech signal. The Uniform Information Density hypothesis seeks to generalize this to choices on all linguistic levels, particularly syntactic choices. While there is some evidence for the Uniform Information Density hypothesis, it faces several challenges, four of which are discussed in this paper. First, it is not clear what exactly counts as uniform. Second, there are syntactic alternations that occur systematically but that can cause notable fluctuations in the information signature. Third, there is an increasing body of negative results. Fourth, there is a lack of large-scale evidence. As to the fourth point, this paper provides a broader array of data-936 sentence pairs for nine syntactic constructions-and analyzes them in a test setup that treats the hypothesis as a classifier. For our data, the Uniform Information Density hypothesis showed little predictive capacity. We explore ways to reconcile our data with theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"217-234"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10932588/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140111557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expecting the Unexpected: Infants Use Others' Surprise to Revise Their Own Expectations. 期待意外:婴儿利用他人的惊讶来修正自己的期望。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-01 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00117
Yang Wu, Megan Merrick, Hyowon Gweon

Human infants show systematic responses to events that violate their expectations. Can they also revise these expectations based on others' expressions of surprise? Here we ask whether infants (N = 156, mean = 15.2 months, range: 12.0-18.0 months) can use an experimenter's expression of surprise to revise their own expectations about statistically probable vs. improbable events. An experimenter sampled a ball from a box of red and white balls and briefly displayed either a surprised or an unsurprised expression at the outcome before revealing it to the infant. Following an unsurprised expression, the results were consistent with prior work; infants looked longer at a statistically improbable outcome than a probable outcome. Following a surprised expression, however, this standard pattern disappeared or was even reversed. These results suggest that even before infants can observe the unexpected events themselves, they can use others' surprise to expect the unexpected. Starting early in life, human learners can leverage social information that signals others' prediction error to update their own predictions.

人类婴儿会对违反其预期的事件做出系统的反应。他们是否也能根据他人的惊讶表达来修正这些预期呢?在此,我们询问婴儿(N = 156,平均 = 15.2 个月,范围:12.0-18.0 个月)能否利用实验者的惊讶表情来修正他们自己对统计上可能发生与不可能发生事件的预期。实验者从一个装有红球和白球的盒子中抽取一个球,并在将结果展示给婴儿之前短暂地表现出惊讶或不惊讶的表情。在婴儿表现出不惊讶的表情时,实验结果与之前的研究结果一致;婴儿看统计上不可能出现的结果的时间比看可能出现的结果的时间长。然而,当婴儿表现出惊讶的表情时,这种标准模式消失了,甚至发生了逆转。这些结果表明,甚至在婴儿自己观察到意外事件之前,他们就能利用他人的惊讶来预期意外。从生命的早期开始,人类学习者就可以利用提示他人预测错误的社会信息来更新自己的预测。
{"title":"Expecting the Unexpected: Infants Use Others' Surprise to Revise Their Own Expectations.","authors":"Yang Wu, Megan Merrick, Hyowon Gweon","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00117","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00117","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human infants show systematic responses to events that violate their expectations. Can they also revise these expectations based on others' expressions of surprise? Here we ask whether infants (<i>N</i> = 156, mean = 15.2 months, range: 12.0-18.0 months) can use an experimenter's expression of surprise to revise their own expectations about statistically probable vs. improbable events. An experimenter sampled a ball from a box of red and white balls and briefly displayed either a surprised or an unsurprised expression at the outcome before revealing it to the infant. Following an unsurprised expression, the results were consistent with prior work; infants looked longer at a statistically improbable outcome than a probable outcome. Following a surprised expression, however, this standard pattern disappeared or was even reversed. These results suggest that even before infants can observe the unexpected events themselves, they can use others' surprise to <i>expect the unexpected</i>. Starting early in life, human learners can leverage social information that signals others' prediction error to update their own predictions.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"67-83"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10898783/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140022738","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Systematic Human Learning and Generalization From a Brief Tutorial With Explanatory Feedback. 人类从带有解释性反馈的简短教程中进行系统学习和归纳。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-01 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00123
Andrew J Nam, James L McClelland

We investigate human adults' ability to learn an abstract reasoning task quickly and to generalize outside of the range of training examples. Using a task based on a solution strategy in Sudoku, we provide Sudoku-naive participants with a brief instructional tutorial with explanatory feedback using a narrow range of training examples. We find that most participants who master the task do so within 10 practice trials and generalize well to puzzles outside of the training range. We also find that most of those who master the task can describe a valid solution strategy, and such participants perform better on transfer puzzles than those whose strategy descriptions are vague or incomplete. Interestingly, fewer than half of our human participants were successful in acquiring a valid solution strategy, and this ability was associated with completion of high school algebra and geometry. We consider the implications of these findings for understanding human systematic reasoning, as well as the challenges these findings pose for building computational models that capture all aspects of our findings, and we point toward a role for learning from instructions and explanations to support rapid learning and generalization.

我们研究了人类成年人快速学习抽象推理任务以及在训练示例范围之外进行泛化的能力。我们使用一个基于数独解题策略的任务,通过使用范围较窄的训练示例,为未接触过数独的参与者提供简短的教学指导和解释性反馈。我们发现,大多数参与者都能在 10 次练习测试内掌握任务,并能很好地泛化到训练范围之外的谜题中。我们还发现,大多数掌握任务的参与者都能描述有效的解题策略,而且这些参与者在转移谜题中的表现要好于那些策略描述模糊或不完整的参与者。有趣的是,只有不到一半的人类参与者能够成功获得有效的解题策略,而这种能力与高中代数和几何的学业成绩有关。我们考虑了这些发现对理解人类系统推理的影响,以及这些发现对建立能捕捉我们发现的所有方面的计算模型所提出的挑战,并指出了从指令和解释中学习以支持快速学习和泛化的作用。
{"title":"Systematic Human Learning and Generalization From a Brief Tutorial With Explanatory Feedback.","authors":"Andrew J Nam, James L McClelland","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00123","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigate human adults' ability to learn an abstract reasoning task quickly and to generalize outside of the range of training examples. Using a task based on a solution strategy in Sudoku, we provide Sudoku-naive participants with a brief instructional tutorial with explanatory feedback using a narrow range of training examples. We find that most participants who master the task do so within 10 practice trials and generalize well to puzzles outside of the training range. We also find that most of those who master the task can describe a valid solution strategy, and such participants perform better on transfer puzzles than those whose strategy descriptions are vague or incomplete. Interestingly, fewer than half of our human participants were successful in acquiring a valid solution strategy, and this ability was associated with completion of high school algebra and geometry. We consider the implications of these findings for understanding human systematic reasoning, as well as the challenges these findings pose for building computational models that capture all aspects of our findings, and we point toward a role for learning from instructions and explanations to support rapid learning and generalization.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"8 ","pages":"148-176"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10898786/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140022740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Open Mind
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1