首页 > 最新文献

Open Mind最新文献

英文 中文
What's in the Box? Preschoolers Consider Ambiguity, Expected Value, and Information for Future Decisions in Explore-Exploit Tasks. 盒子里有什么?学龄前儿童在探索开发任务中考虑歧义、期望值和未来决策信息。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-27 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00110
Elizabeth Lapidow, Elizabeth Bonawitz

Self-directed exploration in childhood appears driven by a desire to resolve uncertainties in order to learn more about the world. However, in adult decision-making, the choice to explore new information rather than exploit what is already known takes many factors beyond uncertainty (such as expected utilities and costs) into account. The evidence for whether young children are sensitive to complex, contextual factors in making exploration decisions is limited and mixed. Here, we investigate whether modifying uncertain options influences explore-exploit behavior in preschool-aged children (48-68 months). Over the course of three experiments, we manipulate uncertain options' ambiguity, expected value, and potential to improve epistemic state for future exploration in a novel forced-choice design. We find evidence that young children are influenced by each of these factors, suggesting that early, self-directed exploration involves sophisticated, context-sensitive decision-making under uncertainty.

童年时期的自我探索似乎是由解决不确定性以了解更多世界的愿望驱动的。然而,在成人决策中,探索新信息而不是利用已知信息的选择考虑了许多不确定性之外的因素(如预期效用和成本)。关于幼儿在做出探索决定时是否对复杂的背景因素敏感的证据是有限的,而且是混合的。在这里,我们调查了修改不确定选项是否会影响学龄前儿童(48-68个月)的探索性剥削行为。在三个实验的过程中,我们在一个新颖的强迫选择设计中操纵了不确定选项的模糊性、期望值和潜力,以改善未来探索的认识状态。我们发现有证据表明,幼儿受到这些因素的影响,这表明早期的自我导向探索涉及到在不确定性下做出复杂、情境敏感的决策。
{"title":"What's in the Box? Preschoolers Consider Ambiguity, Expected Value, and Information for Future Decisions in Explore-Exploit Tasks.","authors":"Elizabeth Lapidow, Elizabeth Bonawitz","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00110","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00110","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-directed exploration in childhood appears driven by a desire to resolve uncertainties in order to learn more about the world. However, in adult decision-making, the choice to explore new information rather than exploit what is already known takes many factors beyond uncertainty (such as expected utilities and costs) into account. The evidence for whether young children are sensitive to complex, contextual factors in making exploration decisions is limited and mixed. Here, we investigate whether modifying uncertain options influences explore-exploit behavior in preschool-aged children (48-68 months). Over the course of three experiments, we manipulate uncertain options' ambiguity, expected value, and potential to improve epistemic state for future exploration in a novel forced-choice design. We find evidence that young children are influenced by each of these factors, suggesting that early, self-directed exploration involves sophisticated, context-sensitive decision-making under uncertainty.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"855-878"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631797/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Number Agreement Attraction in Czech Comprehension: Negligible Facilitation Effects. 捷克语理解中的数字协定吸引:可忽略的促进效应。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-27 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00107
Jan Chromý, Radim Lacina, Jakub Dotlačil

Number agreement attraction in comprehension has been extensively studied in various languages and it has been claimed that attraction effects are generally present across languages. In this paper, four experiments on Czech are presented, each examining a different structure. The Bayesian hierarchical models and Bayes factor analysis pointed towards no agreement attraction effects in three of the experiments. Only in one experiment an effect interpretable as signaling agreement attraction was observed. Its size, however, was so small that it did not translate into a clear preference for models with agreement attraction. The data from the four experiments were further compared to available data from several other languages (English, Armenian, Arabic, and Spanish). The emerging picture is that in Czech, agreement attraction effects are negligible in size if they appear at all. This presents a serious challenge to current theoretical explanations of agreement attraction effects.

理解中的数字一致吸引在各种语言中得到了广泛的研究,并声称吸引效应通常存在于各种语言中。本文在捷克语上进行了四个实验,每个实验都考察了不同的结构。贝叶斯层次模型和贝叶斯因子分析指出,在三个实验中没有一致的吸引效应。仅在一个实验中观察到可解释为信号一致吸引的效应。然而,它的规模太小,以至于没有转化为对具有协议吸引力的车型的明显偏好。将四个实验的数据与其他几种语言(英语、亚美尼亚语、阿拉伯语和西班牙语)的可用数据进行了进一步比较。新出现的情况是,在捷克,如果协议吸引效应出现的话,其规模可以忽略不计。这对目前关于协议吸引效应的理论解释提出了严峻的挑战。
{"title":"Number Agreement Attraction in Czech Comprehension: Negligible Facilitation Effects.","authors":"Jan Chromý, Radim Lacina, Jakub Dotlačil","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00107","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00107","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Number agreement attraction in comprehension has been extensively studied in various languages and it has been claimed that attraction effects are generally present across languages. In this paper, four experiments on Czech are presented, each examining a different structure. The Bayesian hierarchical models and Bayes factor analysis pointed towards no agreement attraction effects in three of the experiments. Only in one experiment an effect interpretable as signaling agreement attraction was observed. Its size, however, was so small that it did not translate into a clear preference for models with agreement attraction. The data from the four experiments were further compared to available data from several other languages (English, Armenian, Arabic, and Spanish). The emerging picture is that in Czech, agreement attraction effects are negligible in size if they appear at all. This presents a serious challenge to current theoretical explanations of agreement attraction effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"802-836"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631795/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015562","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's Reasoning About Empathy and Social Relationships. 儿童关于移情和社会关系的推理。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-27 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00109
Alexis S Smith-Flores, Gabriel J Bonamy, Lindsey J Powell

Across the lifespan, empathic and counter-empathic emotions are shaped by social relationships. Here we test the hypothesis that this connection is encoded in children's intuitive theory of psychology, allowing them to predict when others will feel empathy versus counter-empathy and to use vicarious emotion information to infer relationships. We asked 4- to 7-year-old children (N = 79) to make emotion predictions or relationship inferences in response to stories featuring two characters, an experiencer and an observer, and either a positive or negative outcome for the experiencer. In the context of positive outcomes, we found that children engaged in robust joint reasoning about relationships and vicarious emotions. When given information about the characters' relationship, children predicted empathy from a friendly observer and counter-empathy from a rival observer. When given information about the observer's response to the experiencer, children inferred positive and negative relationships from empathic and counter-empathic responses, respectively. In the context of negative outcomes, children predicted that both friendly and rival observers would feel empathy toward the experiencer, but they still used information about empathic versus counter-empathic responses to infer relationship status. Our results suggest that young children in the US have a blanket expectation of empathic concern in response to negative outcomes, but otherwise expect and infer that vicarious emotions are connected to social relationships. Future research should investigate if children use this understanding to select social partners, evaluate their own relationships, or decide when to express empathy toward others.

在整个生命周期中,移情和反移情情绪都是由社会关系塑造的。在这里,我们检验了这样一种假设,即这种联系被编码在儿童的直觉心理学理论中,使他们能够预测他人何时会感受到同理心与反同理心,并使用替代情绪信息来推断关系。我们要求4至7岁的儿童(N=79)对以两个角色(一个体验者和一个观察者)为特征的故事做出情绪预测或关系推断,以及体验者的积极或消极结果。在积极结果的背景下,我们发现孩子们对关系和替代情绪进行了强有力的联合推理。当被告知角色关系的信息时,孩子们预测来自友好观察者的同理心和来自敌对观察者的反同理心。当被告知观察者对体验者的反应时,孩子们分别从移情和反移情反应中推断出积极和消极的关系。在负面结果的背景下,孩子们预测友好的和敌对的观察者都会对体验者产生同理心,但他们仍然使用移情反应和反移情反应的信息来推断关系状态。我们的研究结果表明,美国的幼儿对负面结果有着全面的移情关注期望,但在其他方面,他们期望并推断替代情绪与社会关系有关。未来的研究应该调查孩子们是否利用这种理解来选择社会伴侣,评估他们自己的关系,或者决定何时对他人表达同理心。
{"title":"Children's Reasoning About Empathy and Social Relationships.","authors":"Alexis S Smith-Flores, Gabriel J Bonamy, Lindsey J Powell","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00109","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00109","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across the lifespan, empathic and counter-empathic emotions are shaped by social relationships. Here we test the hypothesis that this connection is encoded in children's intuitive theory of psychology, allowing them to predict when others will feel empathy versus counter-empathy and to use vicarious emotion information to infer relationships. We asked 4- to 7-year-old children (N = 79) to make emotion predictions or relationship inferences in response to stories featuring two characters, an experiencer and an observer, and either a positive or negative outcome for the experiencer. In the context of positive outcomes, we found that children engaged in robust joint reasoning about relationships and vicarious emotions. When given information about the characters' relationship, children predicted empathy from a friendly observer and counter-empathy from a rival observer. When given information about the observer's response to the experiencer, children inferred positive and negative relationships from empathic and counter-empathic responses, respectively. In the context of negative outcomes, children predicted that both friendly and rival observers would feel empathy toward the experiencer, but they still used information about empathic versus counter-empathic responses to infer relationship status. Our results suggest that young children in the US have a blanket expectation of empathic concern in response to negative outcomes, but otherwise expect and infer that vicarious emotions are connected to social relationships. Future research should investigate if children use this understanding to select social partners, evaluate their own relationships, or decide when to express empathy toward others.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"837-854"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631796/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72211056","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Calculated Feelings: How Children Use Probability to Infer Emotions. 计算的感觉:孩子如何利用概率推断情绪。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-27 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00111
Tiffany Doan, Ori Friedman, Stephanie Denison

Developing the ability to accurately infer others' emotions is crucial for children's cognitive development. Here, we offer a new theoretical perspective on how children develop this ability. We first review recent work showing that with age, children increasingly use probability to infer emotions. We discuss how these findings do not fit with prominent accounts of how children understand emotions, namely the script account and the theory of mind account. We then outline a theory of how probability allows children to infer others' emotions. Specifically, we suggest that probability provides children with information about how much weight to put on alternative outcomes, allowing them to infer emotions by comparing outcomes to counterfactual alternatives.

培养准确推断他人情绪的能力对儿童的认知发展至关重要。在这里,我们为儿童如何发展这种能力提供了一个新的理论视角。我们首先回顾了最近的研究表明,随着年龄的增长,儿童越来越多地使用概率来推断情绪。我们讨论了这些发现与儿童如何理解情绪的突出描述不符,即脚本描述和心理描述理论。然后,我们概述了一个关于概率如何让孩子推断他人情绪的理论。具体而言,我们认为概率为儿童提供了关于对替代结果施加多大权重的信息,使他们能够通过将结果与反事实的替代结果进行比较来推断情绪。
{"title":"Calculated Feelings: How Children Use Probability to Infer Emotions.","authors":"Tiffany Doan, Ori Friedman, Stephanie Denison","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00111","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00111","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Developing the ability to accurately infer others' emotions is crucial for children's cognitive development. Here, we offer a new theoretical perspective on how children develop this ability. We first review recent work showing that with age, children increasingly use probability to infer emotions. We discuss how these findings do not fit with prominent accounts of how children understand emotions, namely the script account and the theory of mind account. We then outline a theory of how probability allows children to infer others' emotions. Specifically, we suggest that probability provides children with information about how much weight to put on alternative outcomes, allowing them to infer emotions by comparing outcomes to counterfactual alternatives.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"879-893"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631798/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Shared Intuitive (Mis)understanding of Psychophysical Law Leads Both Novices and Educated Students to Believe in a Just Noticeable Difference (JND). 对心理物理定律的共同直觉(错误)理解导致新手和受过教育的学生都相信一个明显的差异(JND)。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-20 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00108
Emily M Sanford, Justin Halberda

Humans are both the scientists who discover psychological laws and the thinkers who behave according to those laws. Oftentimes, when our natural behavior is in accord with those laws, this dual role serves us well: our intuitions about our own behavior can serve to inform our discovery of new laws. But, in cases where the laws that we discover through science do not agree with the intuitions and biases we carry into the lab, we may find it harder to believe in and adopt those laws. Here, we explore one such case. Since the founding of psychophysics, the notion of a Just Noticeable Difference (JND) in perceptual discrimination has been ubiquitous in experimental psychology-even in spite of theoretical advances since the 1950's that argue that there can be no such thing as a threshold in perceiving difference. We find that both novices and psychologically educated students alike misunderstand the JND to mean that, below a certain threshold, humans will be unable to tell which of two quantities is greater (e.g., that humans will be completely at chance when trying to judge which is heavier, a bag with 3000 grains of sand or 3001). This belief in chance performance below a threshold is inconsistent with psychophysical law. We argue that belief in a JND is part of our intuitive theory of psychology and is therefore very difficult to dispel.

人类既是发现心理规律的科学家,也是根据这些规律行事的思想家。通常,当我们的自然行为符合这些规律时,这种双重作用对我们很有用:我们对自己行为的直觉可以为我们发现新的规律提供信息。但是,如果我们通过科学发现的定律与我们带入实验室的直觉和偏见不一致,我们可能会发现更难相信和采用这些定律。在这里,我们探讨一个这样的案例。自心理物理学成立以来,感知辨别中的可察觉差异(JND)概念在实验心理学中无处不在,尽管自20世纪50年代以来的理论进步认为感知差异不可能存在阈值。我们发现,新手和受过心理教育的学生都误解了JND的意思,即在低于一定阈值的情况下,人类将无法判断两个量中的哪一个更大(例如,当试图判断哪个更重时,人类将完全有机会,一个装有3000粒沙子的袋子或3001粒沙子)。这种认为机会表现低于阈值的信念与心理物理定律不一致。我们认为,对JND的信念是我们直觉心理学理论的一部分,因此很难消除。
{"title":"A Shared Intuitive (Mis)understanding of Psychophysical Law Leads Both Novices and Educated Students to Believe in a Just Noticeable Difference (JND).","authors":"Emily M Sanford, Justin Halberda","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00108","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00108","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans are both the scientists who discover psychological laws and the thinkers who behave according to those laws. Oftentimes, when our natural behavior is in accord with those laws, this dual role serves us well: our intuitions about our own behavior can serve to inform our discovery of new laws. But, in cases where the laws that we discover through science do not agree with the intuitions and biases we carry into the lab, we may find it harder to believe in and adopt those laws. Here, we explore one such case. Since the founding of psychophysics, the notion of a Just Noticeable Difference (JND) in perceptual discrimination has been ubiquitous in experimental psychology-even in spite of theoretical advances since the 1950's that argue that there can be no such thing as a threshold in perceiving difference. We find that both novices and psychologically educated students alike misunderstand the JND to mean that, below a certain threshold, humans will be <i>unable</i> to tell which of two quantities is greater (e.g., that humans will be completely at chance when trying to judge which is heavier, a bag with 3000 grains of sand or 3001). This belief in chance performance below a threshold is inconsistent with psychophysical law. We argue that belief in a JND is part of our intuitive theory of psychology and is therefore very difficult to dispel.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"785-801"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631794/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015560","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Erratum. 勘误表。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-01 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_e_00106

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00047.].

[这更正了文章DOI:10.1162/opmi_a_0047。]。
{"title":"Erratum.","authors":"","doi":"10.1162/opmi_e_00106","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_e_00106","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00047.].</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"784"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10575549/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239323","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical Processing Strongly Affects Reading Times But Not Skipping During Natural Reading. 自然阅读过程中词汇加工对阅读时间有强烈影响,但不会跳过。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-10-01 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00099
Micha Heilbron, Jorie van Haren, Peter Hagoort, Floris P de Lange

In a typical text, readers look much longer at some words than at others, even skipping many altogether. Historically, researchers explained this variation via low-level visual or oculomotor factors, but today it is primarily explained via factors determining a word's lexical processing ease, such as how well word identity can be predicted from context or discerned from parafoveal preview. While the existence of these effects is well established in controlled experiments, the relative importance of prediction, preview and low-level factors in natural reading remains unclear. Here, we address this question in three large naturalistic reading corpora (n = 104, 1.5 million words), using deep neural networks and Bayesian ideal observers to model linguistic prediction and parafoveal preview from moment to moment in natural reading. Strikingly, neither prediction nor preview was important for explaining word skipping-the vast majority of explained variation was explained by a simple oculomotor model, using just fixation position and word length. For reading times, by contrast, we found strong but independent contributions of prediction and preview, with effect sizes matching those from controlled experiments. Together, these results challenge dominant models of eye movements in reading, and instead support alternative models that describe skipping (but not reading times) as largely autonomous from word identification, and mostly determined by low-level oculomotor information.

在典型的文本中,读者看某些单词的时间比看其他单词的时间长得多,甚至跳过了许多单词。从历史上看,研究人员通过低水平的视觉或动眼因素来解释这种变化,但今天,它主要通过决定单词词汇处理容易程度的因素来解释,例如从上下文中预测单词身份或从旁凹预览中辨别单词身份的程度。虽然这些影响的存在在对照实验中已经得到了很好的证实,但预测、预览和低水平因素在自然阅读中的相对重要性仍不清楚。在这里,我们在三个大型自然主义阅读语料库(n=104,150万字)中解决了这个问题,使用深度神经网络和贝叶斯理想观测器对自然阅读中的语言预测和旁凹预览进行建模。引人注目的是,预测和预览对于解释单词跳跃都不重要——绝大多数解释的变化都是通过简单的动眼器模型来解释的,只使用固定位置和单词长度。相比之下,在阅读时间方面,我们发现预测和预览有着强大但独立的贡献,效果大小与对照实验的效果大小相匹配。总之,这些结果挑战了阅读中主要的眼动模型,转而支持替代模型,这些模型将跳跃(但不是阅读时间)描述为在很大程度上独立于单词识别,并且主要由低级眼动信息决定。
{"title":"Lexical Processing Strongly Affects Reading Times But Not Skipping During Natural Reading.","authors":"Micha Heilbron, Jorie van Haren, Peter Hagoort, Floris P de Lange","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00099","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00099","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a typical text, readers look much longer at some words than at others, even skipping many altogether. Historically, researchers explained this variation via low-level visual or oculomotor factors, but today it is primarily explained via factors determining a word's lexical processing ease, such as how well word identity can be predicted from context or discerned from parafoveal preview. While the existence of these effects is well established in controlled experiments, the relative importance of prediction, preview and low-level factors in natural reading remains unclear. Here, we address this question in three large naturalistic reading corpora (<i>n</i> = 104, 1.5 million words), using deep neural networks and Bayesian ideal observers to model linguistic prediction and parafoveal preview from moment to moment in natural reading. Strikingly, neither prediction nor preview was important for explaining word skipping-the vast majority of explained variation was explained by a simple oculomotor model, using just fixation position and word length. For reading times, by contrast, we found strong but independent contributions of prediction and preview, with effect sizes matching those from controlled experiments. Together, these results challenge dominant models of eye movements in reading, and instead support alternative models that describe skipping (but not reading times) as largely autonomous from word identification, and mostly determined by low-level oculomotor information.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"757-783"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10575561/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239326","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward 'Computational-Rationality' Approaches to Arbitrating Models of Cognition: A Case Study Using Perceptual Metacognition. 认知仲裁模型的“计算理性”方法:一个使用感知元认知的案例研究。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-20 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00100
Yingqi Rong, Megan A K Peters

Perceptual confidence results from a metacognitive process which evaluates how likely our percepts are to be correct. Many competing models of perceptual metacognition enjoy strong empirical support. Arbitrating these models traditionally proceeds via researchers conducting experiments and then fitting several models to the data collected. However, such a process often includes conditions or paradigms that may not best arbitrate competing models: Many models make similar predictions under typical experimental conditions. Consequently, many experiments are needed, collectively (sub-optimally) sampling the space of conditions to compare models. Here, instead, we introduce a variant of optimal experimental design which we call a computational-rationality approach to generative models of cognition, using perceptual metacognition as a case study. Instead of designing experiments and post-hoc specifying models, we began with comprehensive model comparison among four competing generative models for perceptual metacognition, drawn from literature. By simulating a simple experiment under each model, we identified conditions where these models made maximally diverging predictions for confidence. We then presented these conditions to human observers, and compared the models' capacity to predict choices and confidence. Results revealed two surprising findings: (1) two models previously reported to differently predict confidence to different degrees, with one predicting better than the other, appeared to predict confidence in a direction opposite to previous findings; and (2) two other models previously reported to equivalently predict confidence showed stark differences in the conditions tested here. Although preliminary with regards to which model is actually 'correct' for perceptual metacognition, our findings reveal the promise of this computational-rationality approach to maximizing experimental utility in model arbitration while minimizing the number of experiments necessary to reveal the winning model, both for perceptual metacognition and in other domains.

感知信心源于元认知过程,该过程评估我们的感知正确的可能性。许多相互竞争的感知元认知模型都得到了强有力的实证支持。传统上,仲裁这些模型是通过研究人员进行实验,然后将几个模型与收集的数据进行拟合来进行的。然而,这样的过程通常包括可能不能最好地仲裁竞争模型的条件或范式:许多模型在典型的实验条件下做出类似的预测。因此,需要进行许多实验,对条件空间进行集体(次优)采样以比较模型。相反,在这里,我们引入了一种优化实验设计的变体,我们称之为认知生成模型的计算理性方法,使用感知元认知作为案例研究。我们没有设计实验和事后指定模型,而是从文献中提取的四个相互竞争的感知元认知生成模型之间的全面模型比较开始。通过在每个模型下模拟一个简单的实验,我们确定了这些模型对置信度进行最大偏差预测的条件。然后,我们向人类观察者展示了这些条件,并比较了模型预测选择和信心的能力。结果揭示了两个令人惊讶的发现:(1)之前报道的两个模型在不同程度上预测置信度不同,其中一个预测得比另一个好,似乎预测置信度的方向与之前的发现相反;以及(2)先前报道的另外两个等效预测置信度的模型在这里测试的条件中显示出明显的差异。尽管对于哪种模型实际上对感知元认知是“正确的”是初步的,但我们的研究结果揭示了这种计算理性方法的前景,即在模型仲裁中最大限度地提高实验效用,同时在感知元认知和其他领域中尽可能减少揭示获胜模型所需的实验数量。
{"title":"Toward 'Computational-Rationality' Approaches to Arbitrating Models of Cognition: A Case Study Using Perceptual Metacognition.","authors":"Yingqi Rong, Megan A K Peters","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00100","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00100","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceptual confidence results from a metacognitive process which evaluates how likely our percepts are to be correct. Many competing models of perceptual metacognition enjoy strong empirical support. Arbitrating these models traditionally proceeds via researchers conducting experiments and then fitting several models to the data collected. However, such a process often includes conditions or paradigms that may not best arbitrate competing models: Many models make similar predictions under typical experimental conditions. Consequently, many experiments are needed, collectively (sub-optimally) sampling the space of conditions to compare models. Here, instead, we introduce a variant of optimal experimental design which we call a <i>computational-rationality</i> approach to generative models of cognition, using perceptual metacognition as a case study. Instead of designing experiments and post-hoc specifying models, we <i>began</i> with comprehensive model comparison among four competing generative models for perceptual metacognition, drawn from literature. By simulating a simple experiment under each model, we identified conditions where these models made <i>maximally diverging predictions</i> for confidence. We then presented these conditions to human observers, and compared the models' capacity to predict choices and confidence. Results revealed two surprising findings: (1) two models previously reported to differently predict confidence to different degrees, with one predicting better than the other, appeared to predict confidence in a direction <i>opposite</i> to previous findings; and (2) two other models previously reported to equivalently predict confidence showed stark differences in the conditions tested here. Although preliminary with regards to which model is actually 'correct' for perceptual metacognition, our findings reveal the promise of this <i>computational-rationality</i> approach to maximizing experimental utility in model arbitration while minimizing the number of experiments necessary to reveal the winning model, both for perceptual metacognition and in other domains.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"652-674"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10575558/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Everything is Infinite: Children's Beliefs About Endless Space, Time, and Number. 一切都是无限的:孩子们对无尽的空间、时间和数字的信念。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-20 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00104
Jessica Sullivan, Sophie Cramer-Benjamin, Joseph Alvarez, David Barner

How do children form beliefs about the infinity of space, time, and number? We asked whether children held similar beliefs about infinity across domains, and whether beliefs in infinity for domains like space and time might be scaffolded upon numerical knowledge (e.g., knowledge successors within the count list). To test these questions, 112 U.S. children (aged 4;0-7;11) completed an interview regarding their beliefs about infinite space, time, and number. We also measured their knowledge of counting, and other factors that might impact performance on linguistic assessments of infinity belief (e.g., working memory, ability to respond to hypothetical questions). We found that beliefs about infinity were very high across all three domains, suggesting that infinity beliefs may arise early in development for space, time, and number. Second, we found that-across all three domains-children were more likely to believe that it is always possible to add a unit than to believe that the domain is endless. Finally, we found that understanding the rules underlying counting predicted children's belief that it is always possible to add 1 to any number, but did not predict any of the other elements of infinity belief.

孩子们是如何形成关于空间、时间和数字的无限性的信念的?我们询问了孩子们是否在各个领域对无穷大持有相似的信念,以及对空间和时间等领域的无穷大信念是否可能建立在数字知识(例如,计数列表中的知识继承者)的基础上。为了测试这些问题,112名美国儿童(4岁;0-7岁;11岁)完成了一项关于他们对无限空间、时间和数字的信念的采访。我们还测量了他们的计数知识,以及其他可能影响无限信念语言评估表现的因素(例如,工作记忆、回答假设问题的能力)。我们发现,在所有三个领域中,关于无穷大的信念都非常高,这表明无穷大信念可能在空间、时间和数字发展的早期就出现了。其次,我们发现,在所有三个领域中,孩子们更可能相信添加一个单元总是可能的,而不是相信这个领域是无穷无尽的。最后,我们发现,理解计数的基本规则可以预测孩子们的信念,即任何数字都有可能加1,但不能预测无穷大信念的任何其他元素。
{"title":"Everything is Infinite: Children's Beliefs About Endless Space, Time, and Number.","authors":"Jessica Sullivan,&nbsp;Sophie Cramer-Benjamin,&nbsp;Joseph Alvarez,&nbsp;David Barner","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00104","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00104","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do children form beliefs about the infinity of space, time, and number? We asked whether children held similar beliefs about infinity across domains, and whether beliefs in infinity for domains like space and time might be scaffolded upon numerical knowledge (e.g., knowledge successors within the count list). To test these questions, 112 U.S. children (aged 4;0-7;11) completed an interview regarding their beliefs about infinite space, time, and number. We also measured their knowledge of counting, and other factors that might impact performance on linguistic assessments of infinity belief (e.g., working memory, ability to respond to hypothetical questions). We found that beliefs about infinity were very high across all three domains, suggesting that infinity beliefs may arise early in development for space, time, and number. Second, we found that-across all three domains-children were more likely to believe that it is always possible to add a unit than to believe that the domain is endless. Finally, we found that understanding the rules underlying counting predicted children's belief that it is always possible to add 1 to any number, but did not predict any of the other elements of infinity belief.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"715-731"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10575555/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239324","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Relation Between Cross-Linguistic Influence, Between-Language Priming and Language Proficiency: Priming of Ungrammatical Adjective Placement in Bilingual Spanish-Dutch and French-Dutch Children. 跨语言影响、语言启动与语言能力之间的关系——西班牙-荷兰和法国-荷兰双语儿童非语法形容词位置的启动。
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-20 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00105
Chantal van Dijk, Sharon Unsworth

After hearing a structure in one language, bilinguals are more likely to produce the same structure in their other language. Such between-language priming is often interpreted as evidence for shared syntactic representations between a bilingual's two languages and is positively related to proficiency. Recently, shared syntactic structures and structural priming have been invoked to explain cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children. This paper examines the relation between cross-linguistic influence, between-language priming and language proficiency. Almost all studies on between-language priming have focussed on grammatical structures. However, cross-linguistic influence has also been found to result in ungrammatical structures. In this study, we investigated whether ungrammatical adjective placement can be primed from a Germanic language to a Romance language and vice versa, and how to best account for any such priming. Furthermore, we examined the role of proficiency in explaining priming effects and whether this fits with an error-based learning account. Our results show that it is possible to prime ungrammatical structures, that this is lexically constrained, and that it is more likely to occur at lower levels of proficiency. We argue that the same mechanisms underlying grammatical priming can also explain our findings of ungrammatical priming.

在听到一种语言的结构后,双语者更有可能在另一种语言中产生相同的结构。这种语言间启动通常被解释为双语者两种语言之间共有的句法表征的证据,并且与熟练程度呈正相关。近年来,共有句法结构和结构启动被用来解释双语儿童的跨语言影响。本文考察了跨语言影响、语言启动与语言能力之间的关系。几乎所有关于语际启动的研究都集中在语法结构上。然而,跨语言的影响也被发现会导致语法结构不规则。在这项研究中,我们调查了不符合语法的形容词位置是否可以从日耳曼语引发到罗曼斯语,反之亦然,以及如何最好地解释任何这种引发。此外,我们研究了熟练程度在解释启动效应中的作用,以及这是否符合基于错误的学习解释。我们的研究结果表明,对非语法结构进行初始化是可能的,这在词汇上是受限制的,而且在较低的熟练程度下更有可能发生。我们认为语法启动的相同机制也可以解释我们对非语法启动的发现。
{"title":"On the Relation Between Cross-Linguistic Influence, Between-Language Priming and Language Proficiency: Priming of Ungrammatical Adjective Placement in Bilingual Spanish-Dutch and French-Dutch Children.","authors":"Chantal van Dijk,&nbsp;Sharon Unsworth","doi":"10.1162/opmi_a_00105","DOIUrl":"10.1162/opmi_a_00105","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>After hearing a structure in one language, bilinguals are more likely to produce the same structure in their other language. Such between-language priming is often interpreted as evidence for shared syntactic representations between a bilingual's two languages and is positively related to proficiency. Recently, shared syntactic structures and structural priming have been invoked to explain cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children. This paper examines the relation between cross-linguistic influence, between-language priming and language proficiency. Almost all studies on between-language priming have focussed on grammatical structures. However, cross-linguistic influence has also been found to result in <i>ungrammatical</i> structures. In this study, we investigated whether ungrammatical adjective placement can be primed from a Germanic language to a Romance language and vice versa, and how to best account for any such priming. Furthermore, we examined the role of proficiency in explaining priming effects and whether this fits with an error-based learning account. Our results show that it is possible to prime ungrammatical structures, that this is lexically constrained, and that it is more likely to occur at lower levels of proficiency. We argue that the same mechanisms underlying grammatical priming can also explain our findings of ungrammatical priming.</p>","PeriodicalId":32558,"journal":{"name":"Open Mind","volume":"7 ","pages":"732-756"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10575554/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Open Mind
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1