首页 > 最新文献

Acta Linguistica Hafniensia最新文献

英文 中文
On the disappearance of the BE perfect in Late Modern English 论晚期现代英语BE完成时的消失
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1351845
T. McFadden
Abstract Previous work on auxiliary selection in the history of English has revealed that perfect-like constructions in earlier stages of the language were quite distinct from those in the modern language, and furthermore that important changes in the construction with have around 1350 rendered it quite distinct semantically and syntactically from the one with be. While this led to a marked expansion of perfects with have, it did not effect a decrease in those with be, which remained quite stable up to around 1700. The current paper presents research based on more recently available corpus data, exploring what happened in the period after 1700, when the construction with be, which is no longer found in the contemporary language, finally did begin to recede. The most straightforward finding is that the inflection point of this change can now be dated to around the year 1800. Evidence is then presented showing that, contrary to appearances, the loss of be does not seem to be related to increasing lexical restrictions on the perfect construction. Finally, the mechanics of the change and its relevance for the syntactic distribution of participles are discussed, along with arguments that what underlies the loss of the be perfect is actually a restriction on the kinds of VPs that can build stative-resultative participial structures in the language.
以往关于英语历史上助词选择的研究表明,英语早期阶段的完美式结构与现代语言中的完美式结构截然不同,此外,1350年左右的“with”结构的重大变化使其在语义和句法上与“be”结构截然不同。虽然这导致拥有完美智商的人显著增加,但没有影响拥有完美智商的人减少,直到1700年左右,完美智商一直保持相当稳定。本文基于最新的语料库数据进行了研究,探讨了1700年后发生的事情,当时在当代语言中不再出现的be结构终于开始消退。最直接的发现是,这种变化的拐点现在可以追溯到1800年左右。有证据表明,与表面现象相反,be的消失似乎与对完成结构的词汇限制的增加无关。最后,本文讨论了这种变化的机制及其与分词句法分布的相关性,并提出了一种观点,即“be完成”的消失实际上是对语言中可以构建动-结果分词结构的vp的限制。
{"title":"On the disappearance of the BE perfect in Late Modern English","authors":"T. McFadden","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1351845","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1351845","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Previous work on auxiliary selection in the history of English has revealed that perfect-like constructions in earlier stages of the language were quite distinct from those in the modern language, and furthermore that important changes in the construction with have around 1350 rendered it quite distinct semantically and syntactically from the one with be. While this led to a marked expansion of perfects with have, it did not effect a decrease in those with be, which remained quite stable up to around 1700. The current paper presents research based on more recently available corpus data, exploring what happened in the period after 1700, when the construction with be, which is no longer found in the contemporary language, finally did begin to recede. The most straightforward finding is that the inflection point of this change can now be dated to around the year 1800. Evidence is then presented showing that, contrary to appearances, the loss of be does not seem to be related to increasing lexical restrictions on the perfect construction. Finally, the mechanics of the change and its relevance for the syntactic distribution of participles are discussed, along with arguments that what underlies the loss of the be perfect is actually a restriction on the kinds of VPs that can build stative-resultative participial structures in the language.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"48 1","pages":"159 - 175"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83266323","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Developing a new perfect: the rise of the Icelandic vera búinn að-perfect 发展一种新的完美:冰岛vera búinn a - ð-perfect的兴起
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1357267
Höskuldur Thráinsson
Abstract Icelandic is basically a “straight-have-perfect” language, where constructions with be + participle are virtually restricted to stative expressions like er farinn “is gone”. But since around 1600, Icelandic has been developing a new perfect consisting of the verb “be” together with the adjective (or participle) búinn + infinitive. The word búinn normally means “finished, done” so it is not surprising that the earliest examples of this construction typically involved transitive telic predicates and animate agentive subjects and had a clear resultative reading. Gradually, the construction developed into a more general perfect, which today can be used with predicates of different types and have a universal and even to some extent experiential (existential) reading. This development is traced and the restrictions on this new perfect in the modern language are described. It is shown that this new perfect has been gaining ground for centuries and is apparently still on the rise, as can be seen from the fact that it is more popular among young speakers than with the older generations. It is also acquired early and apparently more frequent in child language than the standard have-perfect.
冰岛语基本上是一种“直接完成”的语言,带有be +分词的结构实际上仅限于er farinn“is gone”这样的静态表达。但自1600年左右以来,冰岛语一直在发展一种新的完成式,由动词“be”和形容词(或分词)búinn +不定式组成。búinn这个词通常意味着“完成,完成”,所以这并不奇怪,这种结构的最早的例子通常涉及及物谓语和动画代理主体,并有明确的结果阅读。渐渐地,这个结构发展成为一个更普遍的完成体,今天它可以与不同类型的谓语一起使用,具有普遍的,甚至在某种程度上具有经验的(存在的)阅读。对这一发展进行了追溯,并描述了现代语言中对这一新完成式的限制。这表明,这个新的完成式已经流行了几个世纪,而且显然仍在上升,从这个事实可以看出,它在年轻人中比在老一辈中更受欢迎。与标准的have-perfect相比,它在儿童语言中获得的时间也更早,而且明显更频繁。
{"title":"Developing a new perfect: the rise of the Icelandic vera búinn að-perfect","authors":"Höskuldur Thráinsson","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1357267","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1357267","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Icelandic is basically a “straight-have-perfect” language, where constructions with be + participle are virtually restricted to stative expressions like er farinn “is gone”. But since around 1600, Icelandic has been developing a new perfect consisting of the verb “be” together with the adjective (or participle) búinn + infinitive. The word búinn normally means “finished, done” so it is not surprising that the earliest examples of this construction typically involved transitive telic predicates and animate agentive subjects and had a clear resultative reading. Gradually, the construction developed into a more general perfect, which today can be used with predicates of different types and have a universal and even to some extent experiential (existential) reading. This development is traced and the restrictions on this new perfect in the modern language are described. It is shown that this new perfect has been gaining ground for centuries and is apparently still on the rise, as can be seen from the fact that it is more popular among young speakers than with the older generations. It is also acquired early and apparently more frequent in child language than the standard have-perfect.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"3 1","pages":"118 - 142"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73058330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The have/be alternation in contemporary Faroese 当代法罗语中的have/be交替
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1358071
C. Heycock, H. Petersen
Abstract This paper investigates the status of the alternation in Faroese between have (hava) and be (vera) as auxiliaries combining with the past participle/supine. We present the results of two online questionnaire studies and argue that the data indicate that in Faroese the have/be alternation is an alternation of perfect auxiliaries, like in Danish but unlike the other Scandinavian languages, where be can only be used to convey resultant state.
摘要本文研究了法罗语中have (hava)和be (vera)作为助动词与过去分词/supine结合的情况。我们提出了两项在线问卷研究的结果,并认为数据表明,在法罗语中,have/be交替是完成助动词的交替,就像丹麦语一样,但与其他斯堪的纳维亚语言不同,在这些语言中,be只能用来表达结果状态。
{"title":"The have/be alternation in contemporary Faroese","authors":"C. Heycock, H. Petersen","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1358071","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1358071","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This paper investigates the status of the alternation in Faroese between have (hava) and be (vera) as auxiliaries combining with the past participle/supine. We present the results of two online questionnaire studies and argue that the data indicate that in Faroese the have/be alternation is an alternation of perfect auxiliaries, like in Danish but unlike the other Scandinavian languages, where be can only be used to convey resultant state.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"57 1","pages":"143 - 158"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77036412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The regrammation of paradigms: the development of auxiliaries in Danish 范式重组:丹麦语助词的发展
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1385120
Lars Heltoft
Abstract This article surveys the development of voice and tense auxiliaries in Scandinavian with a focus on Danish. Voice is the first category (documented indirectly by Gothic) to show periphrastic forms in paradigmatic cooperation with inflectional forms; and these periphrastic forms are modelled on predicative constructions. Modern Danish has introduced verbal constructions at the expense of all predicative morphology, and the Old Scandinavian auxiliary verbs hafa/hava ‘have’ and vera/wæra ‘be’, verða/wartha ‘become’ have undergone semantic changes, including specialisation and markedness shift. This regrammation process involves semantic changes that must be described in terms of paradigm (re)organisation and cannot be captured in terms of changes along the parameters of the cline of grammaticalisation. Two main types of auxiliaries are distinguished: inflectional auxiliaries, with predication scope (tense, mood and voice) as part of otherwise inflectional paradigms, and, constructional auxiliaries (copula verbs) with predicate scope. Modern Danish inflectional auxiliaries express perfect tense and active voice with transitive verbs (have ‘have’), perfect tense, active voice and telicity with intransitive verbs (være ‘be’ and have ‘have’), stativity (copula verbs være ‘be’ and blive ‘become, turn’) and passive voice and telicity in the periphrastic passive (være ‘be’ and blive ‘become, be’). Their meaning potentials differ according to the paradigms they are part of.
摘要本文以丹麦语为研究对象,考察了斯堪的纳维亚语中语态助动词和时态助动词的发展。语态是第一类(由哥特间接记录)表现出与屈折形式合作的迂回形式;这些委婉形式是模仿谓语结构的。现代丹麦语以牺牲所有谓语形态为代价引入了动词性结构,古斯堪的纳维亚助动词hafa/hava“有”和vera/wæra“be”,ver /wa /wartha“成为”经历了语义变化,包括专业化和标记转移。这种重组过程涉及语义变化,必须用范式(重新)组织来描述,而不能用沿着语法化变化的参数来描述。主要有两种类型的助词:屈折式助词,其谓语范围(时态、语气和语态)是屈折式助词范式的一部分;构词式助词(连词动词)具有谓语范围。现代丹麦语屈折助动词用及物动词(have have)表达完成时和主动语态,用不及物动词(være be和have have)表达完成时、主动语态和远性,用连动动词være be和believe变成,turn)表达定态和远性,用委婉被动语态表达被动语态和远性(være be和believe变成,be)。它们的意义潜力根据它们所处的范式的不同而不同。
{"title":"The regrammation of paradigms: the development of auxiliaries in Danish","authors":"Lars Heltoft","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1385120","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1385120","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article surveys the development of voice and tense auxiliaries in Scandinavian with a focus on Danish. Voice is the first category (documented indirectly by Gothic) to show periphrastic forms in paradigmatic cooperation with inflectional forms; and these periphrastic forms are modelled on predicative constructions. Modern Danish has introduced verbal constructions at the expense of all predicative morphology, and the Old Scandinavian auxiliary verbs hafa/hava ‘have’ and vera/wæra ‘be’, verða/wartha ‘become’ have undergone semantic changes, including specialisation and markedness shift. This regrammation process involves semantic changes that must be described in terms of paradigm (re)organisation and cannot be captured in terms of changes along the parameters of the cline of grammaticalisation. Two main types of auxiliaries are distinguished: inflectional auxiliaries, with predication scope (tense, mood and voice) as part of otherwise inflectional paradigms, and, constructional auxiliaries (copula verbs) with predicate scope. Modern Danish inflectional auxiliaries express perfect tense and active voice with transitive verbs (have ‘have’), perfect tense, active voice and telicity with intransitive verbs (være ‘be’ and have ‘have’), stativity (copula verbs være ‘be’ and blive ‘become, turn’) and passive voice and telicity in the periphrastic passive (være ‘be’ and blive ‘become, be’). Their meaning potentials differ according to the paradigms they are part of.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"24 1","pages":"255 - 285"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87188336","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The choice of the perfect auxiliary in contemporary spoken Danish 当代丹麦语口语中助动词的选择
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1369689
Anu Laanemets
Abstract This article examines the distribution and the usage pattern of the two perfect auxiliaries – have and be – in contemporary spoken Danish in order to investigate whether the choice between the two auxiliaries is undergoing systematic changes. To do so the LANCHART corpus of contemporary spoken Danish is used. First, the macro-level distributional pattern is established through quantitative apparent and real-time analysis. In the subsequent qualitative micro-level analysis, particular attention is paid to cases where the selection of the auxiliary deviates from what could be expected based on Sorace’s Auxiliary Selection Hierarchy and grammars of Danish. The investigation focuses on the verbs gå ‘go’ and komme ‘come’ – the two most frequent verbs of motion in the corpus. The apparent time analysis of gå indicates an increased use of have across generations. In real time, no change is registered, indicating that any change is stemming from generational differences. With komme – although unexpected examples with have are attested – no statistically significant change is registered neither in apparent nor in real time. The micro-level analysis shows that the variation in auxiliary selection largely conforms to expectations; however, deviations are registered and discussed.
摘要本文考察了当代丹麦语口语中两个完成助语have和be的分布和使用模式,以探讨这两个助语的选择是否正在发生系统性的变化。为此,使用了当代丹麦语口语的LANCHART语料库。首先,通过定量表观和实时分析,建立宏观层面的分布格局。在随后的定性微观层面分析中,我们特别关注了根据Sorace的辅助选择层次和丹麦语语法,助词的选择偏离预期的情况。调查的重点是动词“go”和“come”,这是语料库中最常见的两个动词。对g的表观时间分析表明,跨代使用的增加。实际上,没有变化被记录下来,这表明任何变化都是由代际差异引起的。对于komme,尽管有一些意想不到的例子被证实,但无论是明显的还是实时的,都没有统计上的显著变化。微观层面分析表明,辅助选择的变化在很大程度上符合预期;然而,偏差被记录和讨论。
{"title":"The choice of the perfect auxiliary in contemporary spoken Danish","authors":"Anu Laanemets","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1369689","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1369689","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article examines the distribution and the usage pattern of the two perfect auxiliaries – have and be – in contemporary spoken Danish in order to investigate whether the choice between the two auxiliaries is undergoing systematic changes. To do so the LANCHART corpus of contemporary spoken Danish is used. First, the macro-level distributional pattern is established through quantitative apparent and real-time analysis. In the subsequent qualitative micro-level analysis, particular attention is paid to cases where the selection of the auxiliary deviates from what could be expected based on Sorace’s Auxiliary Selection Hierarchy and grammars of Danish. The investigation focuses on the verbs gå ‘go’ and komme ‘come’ – the two most frequent verbs of motion in the corpus. The apparent time analysis of gå indicates an increased use of have across generations. In real time, no change is registered, indicating that any change is stemming from generational differences. With komme – although unexpected examples with have are attested – no statistically significant change is registered neither in apparent nor in real time. The micro-level analysis shows that the variation in auxiliary selection largely conforms to expectations; however, deviations are registered and discussed.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"15 1","pages":"232 - 254"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78594954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Loanword adaptation in Hungarian: unexpected vowel harmony in material of Latin and Slavic provenance 匈牙利语外来词的改编:拉丁和斯拉夫语源材料中意想不到的元音和谐
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1341811
A. Hyllested
Abstract In a 1992 article, Eugen Helimski showed that the accented vowel of Old and Middle High German, Medieval Latin and Pannonian Slavic lexemes governed whether they acquired front-vowel or back-vowel harmony when entering Hungarian as loanwords. While the German material appears exceptionless, some words of Slavic and Latin provenance exhibit unexpected back-vowel harmony. The present article submits that if a labial sound follows the originally accented vowel, front-vowel harmony is blocked. This conditioned rule applies without exception to both Slavic and Latin loanwords; it is thus an economical solution. It follows that variation in Slavic loanwords in Hungarian cannot serve as a testimony of Old Slavic accent shifts, but merely of the place of the original (pitch) accent; and that the Slavic language that provided loanwords to Medieval Hungarian must have been fairly uniform. As for Latin loanwords, it likewise renders an appeal to late accent shifts unnecessary. Helimski also discovered that a subset of Latin words containing a medial cluster *-CiV- could trigger front-vowel harmony even if the original accent fell on a back vowel. Here, I argue that the distribution of front and back vocalism in this type is further governed by the vowel of the initial syllable. This minor rule possibly applies to Slavic as well.
Eugen Helimski在1992年的一篇文章中指出,古高地德语和中古高地德语、中世纪拉丁语和潘诺尼亚斯拉夫语词汇的重读元音决定了它们作为外来词进入匈牙利语时是否获得前元音或后元音的和谐。虽然德语材料似乎没有例外,但一些来自斯拉夫语和拉丁语的单词表现出意想不到的后元音和谐。本文认为,如果一个阴唇音跟在原来重读的元音后面,前元音的和谐就会被阻断。这条条件性规则毫无例外地适用于斯拉夫语和拉丁语的外来词;因此,这是一个经济的解决方案。由此可见,匈牙利语中斯拉夫外来词的变化不能作为古斯拉夫口音变化的证据,而仅仅是原始(音高)口音的位置;为中世纪匈牙利语提供外来词的斯拉夫语一定是相当统一的。至于拉丁外来词,它同样使得对后期重音变化的呼吁变得不必要。赫利姆斯基还发现,拉丁单词的一个子集包含一个中间音节*- civ -可以触发前元音和谐,即使原来的重音落在后元音上。在这里,我认为在这种类型中,前后元音的分布进一步受首音节元音的支配。这个小规则可能也适用于斯拉夫语。
{"title":"Loanword adaptation in Hungarian: unexpected vowel harmony in material of Latin and Slavic provenance","authors":"A. Hyllested","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1341811","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1341811","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In a 1992 article, Eugen Helimski showed that the accented vowel of Old and Middle High German, Medieval Latin and Pannonian Slavic lexemes governed whether they acquired front-vowel or back-vowel harmony when entering Hungarian as loanwords. While the German material appears exceptionless, some words of Slavic and Latin provenance exhibit unexpected back-vowel harmony. The present article submits that if a labial sound follows the originally accented vowel, front-vowel harmony is blocked. This conditioned rule applies without exception to both Slavic and Latin loanwords; it is thus an economical solution. It follows that variation in Slavic loanwords in Hungarian cannot serve as a testimony of Old Slavic accent shifts, but merely of the place of the original (pitch) accent; and that the Slavic language that provided loanwords to Medieval Hungarian must have been fairly uniform. As for Latin loanwords, it likewise renders an appeal to late accent shifts unnecessary. Helimski also discovered that a subset of Latin words containing a medial cluster *-CiV- could trigger front-vowel harmony even if the original accent fell on a back vowel. Here, I argue that the distribution of front and back vocalism in this type is further governed by the vowel of the initial syllable. This minor rule possibly applies to Slavic as well.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"1 1","pages":"1 - 11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90831789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Prototypical adverbs: from comparative concept to typological prototype 原型副词:从比较概念到类型原型
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1292801
Pernilla Hallonsten Halling
Abstract While adjectives and their potential universality have been much debated, adverbs remain rather neglected in the typological and cognitive literature. From a typological perspective, adjectives can be dealt with using a comparative concept: rather than assuming from the outset the existence of a class of adjectives, a particular language-independent definition of adjectives is used as a heuristic for examining recurrent form-meaning combinations. In the present article, adverb is addressed as a comparative concept in the same vein: an adverb is a lexeme that denotes a descriptive property and can be used to narrow the predication of a verb. This comparative concept is applied to a sample of 41 languages from the whole world. The results show that although there are diverse structural possibilities in terms of different adverbial constructions of varying spread and productivity, simple adverbs are found in a considerable number of unrelated languages, even in some cases where adjectives cannot be found. Clear adverb subtypes reminiscent of semantic types of adjectives further emerge, leading to a discussion of whether the comparative concepts in this case allow us to uncover a substantial cross-linguistic prototype.
虽然形容词及其潜在的普遍性一直备受争议,但副词在类型学和认知文学中仍然相当被忽视。从类型学的角度来看,形容词可以使用比较概念来处理:而不是从一开始就假设一类形容词的存在,一个特定的独立于语言的形容词定义被用作检查反复出现的形式-意义组合的启发式。在本文中,副词以同样的方式作为比较概念来处理:副词是一个表示描述性属性的词汇,可以用来缩小动词的谓语。这一比较概念被应用于全球41种语言的样本。结果表明,尽管不同的副词结构具有不同的结构可能性,其传播范围和生产力也各不相同,但在相当数量的不相关语言中,甚至在某些无法找到形容词的情况下,也可以发现简单副词。清晰的副词子类型进一步出现,使人联想到形容词的语义类型,从而导致讨论在这种情况下的比较概念是否允许我们发现一个实质性的跨语言原型。
{"title":"Prototypical adverbs: from comparative concept to typological prototype","authors":"Pernilla Hallonsten Halling","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1292801","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1292801","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract While adjectives and their potential universality have been much debated, adverbs remain rather neglected in the typological and cognitive literature. From a typological perspective, adjectives can be dealt with using a comparative concept: rather than assuming from the outset the existence of a class of adjectives, a particular language-independent definition of adjectives is used as a heuristic for examining recurrent form-meaning combinations. In the present article, adverb is addressed as a comparative concept in the same vein: an adverb is a lexeme that denotes a descriptive property and can be used to narrow the predication of a verb. This comparative concept is applied to a sample of 41 languages from the whole world. The results show that although there are diverse structural possibilities in terms of different adverbial constructions of varying spread and productivity, simple adverbs are found in a considerable number of unrelated languages, even in some cases where adjectives cannot be found. Clear adverb subtypes reminiscent of semantic types of adjectives further emerge, leading to a discussion of whether the comparative concepts in this case allow us to uncover a substantial cross-linguistic prototype.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"15 1","pages":"37 - 52"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74636349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From three genders to two: the sociolinguistics of gender shift in the Jämtlandic dialect of Sweden 从三种性别到两种:Jämtlandic瑞典方言中性别转换的社会语言学
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2017.1286811
Briana Van Epps, G. Carling
Abstract The influence of standard language varieties on rural dialects is an important factor involved in dialect loss, which is widespread in Europe. In this study, we look at how the three-gender system in the Jämtlandic dialect of Sweden is changing under pressure from the two-gender system of Standard Swedish. The Jämtlandic dialect is an understudied Swedish dialect and an interesting object of study, in part because of the social and economic changes that have occurred over the past century. We performed a survey using profiled stimuli to elicit indefinite articles, definite articles and anaphoric pronouns for 36 target nouns. An analysis was conducted on the traditionalness of gender agreement in the material. We consider linguistic features (traditional gender and type of agreement), as well as sociological features (age, gender, education, geographical location, socioeconomic status, and language attitudes). The results show that most participants maintain the traditional three-gender system to a large degree. Age is the most significant predictor of traditionalness. While the youngest participants show the highest variability in gender assignment, they still retain the three-gender system to some degree. In addition, participants to whom the dialect is very important tend to use more traditional agreement.
标准语言变体对农村方言的影响是导致方言流失的一个重要因素,这种现象在欧洲普遍存在。在这项研究中,我们看看Jämtlandic瑞典方言中的三性别系统是如何在标准瑞典语的两性别系统的压力下发生变化的。Jämtlandic方言是一种未被充分研究的瑞典方言,也是一个有趣的研究对象,部分原因是过去一个世纪以来发生的社会和经济变化。我们进行了一项调查,使用侧面刺激引出36个目标名词的不定冠词、定冠词和回指代词。对材料中性别一致的传统进行了分析。我们考虑语言特征(传统性别和协议类型),以及社会学特征(年龄、性别、教育、地理位置、社会经济地位和语言态度)。结果表明,大多数参与者在很大程度上保持了传统的三性别制度。年龄是传统最重要的预测因素。虽然最年轻的参与者在性别分配上表现出最高的可变性,但他们在某种程度上仍然保留了三性别系统。此外,对方言非常重要的参与者倾向于使用更传统的协议。
{"title":"From three genders to two: the sociolinguistics of gender shift in the Jämtlandic dialect of Sweden","authors":"Briana Van Epps, G. Carling","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2017.1286811","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2017.1286811","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The influence of standard language varieties on rural dialects is an important factor involved in dialect loss, which is widespread in Europe. In this study, we look at how the three-gender system in the Jämtlandic dialect of Sweden is changing under pressure from the two-gender system of Standard Swedish. The Jämtlandic dialect is an understudied Swedish dialect and an interesting object of study, in part because of the social and economic changes that have occurred over the past century. We performed a survey using profiled stimuli to elicit indefinite articles, definite articles and anaphoric pronouns for 36 target nouns. An analysis was conducted on the traditionalness of gender agreement in the material. We consider linguistic features (traditional gender and type of agreement), as well as sociological features (age, gender, education, geographical location, socioeconomic status, and language attitudes). The results show that most participants maintain the traditional three-gender system to a large degree. Age is the most significant predictor of traditionalness. While the youngest participants show the highest variability in gender assignment, they still retain the three-gender system to some degree. In addition, participants to whom the dialect is very important tend to use more traditional agreement.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"12 1","pages":"53 - 84"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81605017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Bulletin du Cercle Linguistique de Copenhague 2015-2016 哥本哈根语言圈公报2015-2016
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2016.1260299
H. Haberland, A. Hansen, P. Nielsen, R. Bastiaanse, Jonas Nölle, Jonathon Lum, J. Schlossberg, William Mcgregor, Henrik Liljegren, Ditte Boeg Thomsen
{"title":"Bulletin du Cercle Linguistique de Copenhague 2015-2016","authors":"H. Haberland, A. Hansen, P. Nielsen, R. Bastiaanse, Jonas Nölle, Jonathon Lum, J. Schlossberg, William Mcgregor, Henrik Liljegren, Ditte Boeg Thomsen","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2016.1260299","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2016.1260299","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"391 1","pages":"104 - 106"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85003950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On holistic properties of morphological constructions: the case of Akan verb–verb nominal compounds 论形态结构的整体特性:以阿坎语动、动名词复合词为例
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/03740463.2016.1242331
Clement Kwamina Insaidoo Appah
Abstract Akan verb–verb nominal compounds exhibit unusual formal and semantic properties, including extreme formal exocentricity, where the composition of two verbs yields a noun some of whose semantic properties may not be directly coded in the constituents, and argument structure suppression, where no argument of either constituent can occur in the compound. The purpose of this paper is twofold. First, I delineate the membership of the class, showing that some of the constructions listed in the literature as verb–verb compounds do not belong to the class; they have formal features that betray them as affix-derived nominals. Secondly, I discuss the rather idiosyncratic properties of the compound. I argue that the form class is inherited from a meta-schema for compounding in Akan which bears a nominal output category. Again, it is a unique constructional property of Akan verb–verb compounds that, unlike other verb-involved compounds, they do not allow any argument of the constituents to become part of the compound. These extra-compositional holistic properties can be accounted for straightforwardly in a framework like Construction Morphology which does not assume that every property in a construction must emanate from its constituents. This study provides evidence for the view that constructions can have holistic properties.
阿坎语动词-动词名义复合词表现出不同寻常的形式和语义特性,包括极端的形式外心性,两个动词的组合产生一个名词,其中一些语义特性可能无法直接编码在组成部分中,以及论点结构抑制,其中任何一个组成部分都不能出现在复合词中。本文的目的是双重的。首先,我描述了这类的成员,表明文献中列出的一些动词-动词复合词并不属于这类;它们的形式特征表明它们是词缀派生的名词。其次,我讨论了化合物的特殊性质。我认为form类继承自Akan中用于复合的元模式,该模式具有名义输出类别。再一次,这是阿坎语动词-动词复合词的一个独特的结构特性,与其他涉及动词的复合词不同,它们不允许任何成分的论点成为复合词的一部分。这些额外组成的整体属性可以直接在结构形态学这样的框架中解释,而不是假设结构中的每个属性都必须来自其组成部分。本研究为结构可以具有整体属性的观点提供了证据。
{"title":"On holistic properties of morphological constructions: the case of Akan verb–verb nominal compounds","authors":"Clement Kwamina Insaidoo Appah","doi":"10.1080/03740463.2016.1242331","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2016.1242331","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Akan verb–verb nominal compounds exhibit unusual formal and semantic properties, including extreme formal exocentricity, where the composition of two verbs yields a noun some of whose semantic properties may not be directly coded in the constituents, and argument structure suppression, where no argument of either constituent can occur in the compound. The purpose of this paper is twofold. First, I delineate the membership of the class, showing that some of the constructions listed in the literature as verb–verb compounds do not belong to the class; they have formal features that betray them as affix-derived nominals. Secondly, I discuss the rather idiosyncratic properties of the compound. I argue that the form class is inherited from a meta-schema for compounding in Akan which bears a nominal output category. Again, it is a unique constructional property of Akan verb–verb compounds that, unlike other verb-involved compounds, they do not allow any argument of the constituents to become part of the compound. These extra-compositional holistic properties can be accounted for straightforwardly in a framework like Construction Morphology which does not assume that every property in a construction must emanate from its constituents. This study provides evidence for the view that constructions can have holistic properties.","PeriodicalId":35105,"journal":{"name":"Acta Linguistica Hafniensia","volume":"26 1","pages":"12 - 36"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79617069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
Acta Linguistica Hafniensia
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1