首页 > 最新文献

Studies in African Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
The development of subject case marking in Omotic Mao 略语中主语格标注的发展
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-11-13 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i2.118036
M. Ahland
The Mao subgroup of the Omotic family shows various degrees of development of morphological subject case marking which results from largely internal, but very similar historical pathways across the group. These different patterns find their source in an older prenominal demonstrative + NP + bound postnominal form construction; in this construction the bound postnominal form of this construction is itself related to (and often reduced from) the corresponding prenominal demonstrative.  Evidence of such a construction is found in each of the four Mao languages but in only three of the languages has the construction become clearly associated with marking grammatical subjects. The pathway toward subject case marking appears to have begun with the demonstrative construction becoming associated with topical referents in discourse. In three of the four Mao languages, the prenominal demonstrative then became associated with definiteness (a typologically common development from topic-marking devices); in those same three languages the frequent co-association between topics and grammatical subjects led to the postnominal form developing subject case marking status. The prenominal definite marker (the erstwhile demonstrative) eventually became emancipated from the postnominal case marker to various degrees across the Mao group. The degree to which subject-development and emancipation between the prenominal and postnominal portions of this demonstrative construction has become established in each of the languages has led to the diverse patterns across the subgroup.
毛氏亚群的形态主格标记表现出不同程度的发展,这主要是由于内部的,但在整个群体中非常相似的历史途径。这些不同的模式源于一个古老的名词前指示+ NP +限定后名词形式结构;在这个结构中,这个结构的限定后名形式本身与相应的前名指示代词相关(并且经常从其派生)。在四种毛语中都发现了这种结构的证据,但只有三种语言中这种结构与标记语法主语明显相关。主语格标记的途径似乎是从指示结构与话语中的主题指称物相关联开始的。在四种毛语中的三种中,名前指示语随后与确定性联系在一起(这是主题标记手段在类型学上的共同发展);在这三种语言中,主题和语法主语之间的频繁关联导致后名形式发展为主语格标记状态。名词前定标记(以前的指示)最终在不同程度上从名词后格标记中解放出来。这种指示结构的前名和后名部分之间的主体发展和解放在每种语言中得到确立的程度,导致了整个亚群的不同模式。
{"title":"The development of subject case marking in Omotic Mao","authors":"M. Ahland","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i2.118036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i2.118036","url":null,"abstract":"The Mao subgroup of the Omotic family shows various degrees of development of morphological subject case marking which results from largely internal, but very similar historical pathways across the group. These different patterns find their source in an older prenominal demonstrative + NP + bound postnominal form construction; in this construction the bound postnominal form of this construction is itself related to (and often reduced from) the corresponding prenominal demonstrative.  Evidence of such a construction is found in each of the four Mao languages but in only three of the languages has the construction become clearly associated with marking grammatical subjects. The pathway toward subject case marking appears to have begun with the demonstrative construction becoming associated with topical referents in discourse. In three of the four Mao languages, the prenominal demonstrative then became associated with definiteness (a typologically common development from topic-marking devices); in those same three languages the frequent co-association between topics and grammatical subjects led to the postnominal form developing subject case marking status. The prenominal definite marker (the erstwhile demonstrative) eventually became emancipated from the postnominal case marker to various degrees across the Mao group. The degree to which subject-development and emancipation between the prenominal and postnominal portions of this demonstrative construction has become established in each of the languages has led to the diverse patterns across the subgroup.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42966649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dominance-as-markedness 显性作为标记
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-11-13 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i2.118039
Katherine Hout
This paper examines a formal consequence of the assumption that dominance is equivalent to markedness (Casali 2016): if dominant ATR values are marked and therefore specified, while recessive values are unmarked and unspecified, then no phonological process in a language with ATR dominance should require reference to the recessive value. This claim is examined in light of new data and analyses of ATR harmony and three other vowel assimilation patterns in Bari (Eastern Nilotic; BFA). I demonstrate that all four of these processes are analyzable without reference to the recessive value of ATR, supporting the characterization of dominance as markedness, and markedness as specification
本文研究了显性等同于标记性假设的一个正式结果(Casali 2016):如果显性ATR值被标记并因此被指定,而隐性值未被标记和未指定,那么在具有ATR优势的语言中,任何语音过程都不需要参考隐性值。这一说法是根据新的数据和分析的ATR和谐和其他三个元音同化模式巴里(东尼罗河;论坛)。我证明了所有这四个过程都是可以分析的,而不参考ATR的隐性值,支持显性作为标记性的特征,标记性作为规范
{"title":"Dominance-as-markedness","authors":"Katherine Hout","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i2.118039","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i2.118039","url":null,"abstract":"This paper examines a formal consequence of the assumption that dominance is equivalent to markedness (Casali 2016): if dominant ATR values are marked and therefore specified, while recessive values are unmarked and unspecified, then no phonological process in a language with ATR dominance should require reference to the recessive value. This claim is examined in light of new data and analyses of ATR harmony and three other vowel assimilation patterns in Bari (Eastern Nilotic; BFA). I demonstrate that all four of these processes are analyzable without reference to the recessive value of ATR, supporting the characterization of dominance as markedness, and markedness as specification","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45518793","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical proximity of a Xri corpus to Khoekhoegowab Xri语料库与Khoekhoegowab的词汇接近性
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-11-13 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i2.118040
W. Haacke, J. Snyman
The present paper examines a corpus of some 1 130 Xri concepts collected by Jan Snyman (UNISA) in the early 1970s. This collection is by far the largest corpus of vocabulary of self-declared "Grie­kwa" speakers available, and is unlikely to be surpassed in future, as the language is mori­bund. 1096 concepts of this Xri corpus are compared dialectometrically to the equivalents in Namibian Khoekhoegowab (Khoekhoe, formerly “Nama/Damara”).  According to this method the rate of cognation between the Xri corpus and its Khoekhoe equivalents is 69% and consequently sets Xri aside as a lect distinct from Khoekhoegowab. While the entire word list is provided in an appen­dix, distinctive correspondences of the segmental phonology are discussed.  The crucial question whether Xri had a three-tone system like !Ora or a four-tone system like Khoekhoegowab cannot be investigated reliably because of the absence of tonal data for Xri. The fact, however, that Xri (like !Ora) distinguishes certain voiced and voiceless consonants permits the conclusion that it too had a pre-tonogenetic system where voicing still is distinctive and – assumedly - has not caused tonal depression as in Khoekhoegowab.   Although no dialectomeric comparison of !Ora and Xri lexicon has been undertaken, certain systematic phonological contrasts between these two lects set Xri aside also from !Ora as a distinct lect in this dialect continuum
本文考察了Jan Snyman (UNISA)在1970年代初收集的大约1130个Xri概念的语料库。这个集合是迄今为止最大的自称“Grie-kwa”的语料库,并且在未来不太可能被超越,因为这种语言已经奄奄一息。这个Xri语料库的1096个概念与纳米比亚Khoekhoegowab (Khoekhoe,以前的“Nama/Damara”)中的对应概念进行了方言比较。根据该方法,Xri语料库与Khoekhoe语料库之间的认知率为69%,从而将Xri作为与Khoekhoegowab不同的选择。虽然整个单词表在附录中提供,但我们讨论了音韵学的不同对应关系。Xri是否有像Ora一样的三音系统,还是像Khoekhoegowab一样的四音系统,这个关键问题无法可靠地调查,因为缺乏Xri的音调数据。然而,事实上,Xri(像!Ora一样)区分了某些浊音和清音,这使得它也有一个前语调发生系统,在这个系统中,发声仍然是独特的,并且-假设-没有像Khoekhoegowab那样引起音调低沉。虽然没有对Ora和Xri词汇的方言进行比较,但这两个词汇之间的某些系统语音对比使Xri也从Ora中脱颖而出,成为方言连续体中的一个独特的词汇
{"title":"Lexical proximity of a Xri corpus to Khoekhoegowab","authors":"W. Haacke, J. Snyman","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i2.118040","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i2.118040","url":null,"abstract":"The present paper examines a corpus of some 1 130 Xri concepts collected by Jan Snyman (UNISA) in the early 1970s. This collection is by far the largest corpus of vocabulary of self-declared \"Grie­kwa\" speakers available, and is unlikely to be surpassed in future, as the language is mori­bund. 1096 concepts of this Xri corpus are compared dialectometrically to the equivalents in Namibian Khoekhoegowab (Khoekhoe, formerly “Nama/Damara”).  According to this method the rate of cognation between the Xri corpus and its Khoekhoe equivalents is 69% and consequently sets Xri aside as a lect distinct from Khoekhoegowab. While the entire word list is provided in an appen­dix, distinctive correspondences of the segmental phonology are discussed.  The crucial question whether Xri had a three-tone system like !Ora or a four-tone system like Khoekhoegowab cannot be investigated reliably because of the absence of tonal data for Xri. The fact, however, that Xri (like !Ora) distinguishes certain voiced and voiceless consonants permits the conclusion that it too had a pre-tonogenetic system where voicing still is distinctive and – assumedly - has not caused tonal depression as in Khoekhoegowab.   Although no dialectomeric comparison of !Ora and Xri lexicon has been undertaken, certain systematic phonological contrasts between these two lects set Xri aside also from !Ora as a distinct lect in this dialect continuum","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47662111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Nominal and verbal number in Bilugu Opo 毕鲁固语的名义数和言语数
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-07-03 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i1.114929
J. Smolders
Opo (a.k.a. Opuuo, Tʼapo [lgn]), a Koman language spoken in Ethiopia and South Sudan, has complex and interesting systems of both NOMINAL NUMBER and VERBAL NUMBER. This paper provides a description and analysis of these systems as found in the Bilugu dialect of Ethiopia, using Corbett's (2000) model of number systems as a theoretical framework. In Bilugu Opo, NOMINAL NUMBER marking is divided along the animacy hierarchy into two systems. The TOP SYSTEM, encompassing all human referents, marks singular ~ plural opposition via a variety of morphological strategies (lexical, derivational, and inflectional) and patterns (marked plural, marked singular, and both marked). The SECOND SYSTEM, encompassing all non-human referents, encodes GENERAL NUMBER. In the Opo verb system, VERBAL NUMBER (Corbett 2000) or PLURACTIONALITY (Newman 1990) is attested for just under half of verbs. These verbs can be organized into two groups: a large group which derive a plural stem through morpho-phonemic means (tone modification, vowel gemination, and reduplication) with unpredictable semantics, and a small group which derive a plural stem through lexical means (suppletion and stem alternation) with more predictable semantics.
Opo(又名Opuuo,T’apo[lgn])是埃塞俄比亚和南苏丹的一种科曼语,有着复杂而有趣的名义数字和口头数字系统。本文以Corbett(2000)的数字系统模型为理论框架,对埃塞俄比亚比卢古方言中的这些系统进行了描述和分析。在Bilugu Opo中,NOMINAL NUMBER标记沿着动画层次划分为两个系统。TOP系统包括所有人类指称,通过各种形态策略(词汇、派生和屈折)和模式(标记复数、标记单数和两者都标记)标记单数~复数对立。第二个系统,包括所有非人类参照物,编码通用数。在Opo动词系统中,动词数量(Corbett 2000)或PLURATIONALITY(Newman 1990)被证明适用于不到一半的动词。这些动词可分为两组:一大组通过语义不可预测的形态音素手段(音调修饰、元音重叠和重叠)派生出复数词干,另一小组通过语义更可预测的词汇手段(补充和词干交替)派生出复复数词干。
{"title":"Nominal and verbal number in Bilugu Opo","authors":"J. Smolders","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i1.114929","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i1.114929","url":null,"abstract":"Opo (a.k.a. Opuuo, Tʼapo [lgn]), a Koman language spoken in Ethiopia and South Sudan, has complex and interesting systems of both NOMINAL NUMBER and VERBAL NUMBER. This paper provides a description and analysis of these systems as found in the Bilugu dialect of Ethiopia, using Corbett's (2000) model of number systems as a theoretical framework. In Bilugu Opo, NOMINAL NUMBER marking is divided along the animacy hierarchy into two systems. The TOP SYSTEM, encompassing all human referents, marks singular ~ plural opposition via a variety of morphological strategies (lexical, derivational, and inflectional) and patterns (marked plural, marked singular, and both marked). The SECOND SYSTEM, encompassing all non-human referents, encodes GENERAL NUMBER. In the Opo verb system, VERBAL NUMBER (Corbett 2000) or PLURACTIONALITY (Newman 1990) is attested for just under half of verbs. These verbs can be organized into two groups: a large group which derive a plural stem through morpho-phonemic means (tone modification, vowel gemination, and reduplication) with unpredictable semantics, and a small group which derive a plural stem through lexical means (suppletion and stem alternation) with more predictable semantics.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45683523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The map of ti in Kituba - testing and expanding the typological model of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators 基图巴语的ti图——连接语多义类型模型的检验与拓展
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-07-03 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i1.114931
A. Andrason
The present article analyzes the polysemy of the element ti in Kituba from the perspective of cognitive linguistics, by applying the framework of dynamic semantic maps and waves. The qualitative and quantitative corpus study, enhanced by evidence provided by Kituba native speakers, demonstrates the following: although ti spans most parts of the typological map of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators, its center of prototypicality is located in the initial stage (comitative) and two intermediate stages (possessive and certain types of coordinate-hood) available along the grammaticalization pathway underlying the map. This suggests a semi-advanced grammaticalization profile for ti. The study also proposes certain changes in the typological map of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators, postulating new components of the map (or grammaticalization stages), and alternative linking directions. Additionally, a possible manner of introducing quantitative data (related to prototypicality) to the qualitative map of polysemy is presented. The resulting model is argued to exhibit properties typical of complexity: structural intricacy, gradience, fuzziness, and multi-causality.
本文从认知语言学的角度,运用动态语义图和语义波的框架,分析了基图巴语中“ti”一词的多义性。在基图巴语母语者提供的证据的支持下,质性和定量的语料库研究表明:虽然它跨越了连接词的多义类型地图的大部分,但其原型性的中心位于该地图的语法化路径上的初始阶段(拟态)和两个中间阶段(所有格和某些类型的座标)。这表明ti具有半高级的语法化特征。本研究还提出了连接语多义类型地图的某些变化,假设了地图的新组成部分(或语法化阶段),以及不同的连接方向。此外,还提出了一种将定量数据(与原型性有关)引入多义词定性图的可能方法。由此产生的模型被认为表现出典型的复杂性:结构复杂性、梯度、模糊性和多因果性。
{"title":"The map of ti in Kituba - testing and expanding the typological model of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators","authors":"A. Andrason","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i1.114931","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i1.114931","url":null,"abstract":"The present article analyzes the polysemy of the element ti in Kituba from the perspective of cognitive linguistics, by applying the framework of dynamic semantic maps and waves. The qualitative and quantitative corpus study, enhanced by evidence provided by Kituba native speakers, demonstrates the following: although ti spans most parts of the typological map of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators, its center of prototypicality is located in the initial stage (comitative) and two intermediate stages (possessive and certain types of coordinate-hood) available along the grammaticalization pathway underlying the map. This suggests a semi-advanced grammaticalization profile for ti. The study also proposes certain changes in the typological map of the polysemy of conjunctive coordinators, postulating new components of the map (or grammaticalization stages), and alternative linking directions. Additionally, a possible manner of introducing quantitative data (related to prototypicality) to the qualitative map of polysemy is presented. The resulting model is argued to exhibit properties typical of complexity: structural intricacy, gradience, fuzziness, and multi-causality.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44489292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A typological overview of Eegimaa (Jóola Banjal) Eegimaa的类型学概述(Jóola Banjal)
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-07-03 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i1.114928
Serge Sagna
This paper presents some of the most prominent properties of Eegimaa, a Jóola/Diola2 language spoken in the Basse-Casamance (Southern Senegal). The phonological features examined include [ATR] vowel harmony, backness harmony, lenition, and Eegimaa’s typologically unusual geminate consonants. Most of the paper, however, focuses on Eegimaa morphology. My analysis of the noun class system separates morphological classes from agreement classes (genders), and presents the most important principles of semantic categorization, including shape encoding. I also show that Eegimaa classifies nouns and verbs by the same overt linguistic means, namely, noun class prefixes. I argue that this overt classification of nouns and verbs reflects parallel semantic categorization of entities and events. Other prominent typological features include associative plural marking and nominal TAM marking with the inactualis suffix, which also expresses alienability contrasts.
本文介绍了塞内加尔南部Basse-Casamance地区使用的Jóola/Diola2语言Eegimaa的一些最突出的特征。研究的音韵学特征包括[ATR]元音和谐,后音和谐,弱音和Eegimaa的类型上不寻常的双元音辅音。然而,本文的大部分内容都集中在Eegimaa的形态学上。我对名词类系统的分析将形态类与协议类(性别)分开,并提出了语义分类的最重要原则,包括形状编码。我还表明Eegimaa通过同样明显的语言手段对名词和动词进行分类,即名词类前缀。我认为这种名词和动词的明显分类反映了实体和事件的平行语义分类。其他突出的类型学特征包括联想复数标记和带有inactualis后缀的名义TAM标记,这也表达了可让与性对比。
{"title":"A typological overview of Eegimaa (Jóola Banjal)","authors":"Serge Sagna","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i1.114928","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i1.114928","url":null,"abstract":"This paper presents some of the most prominent properties of Eegimaa, a Jóola/Diola2 language spoken in the Basse-Casamance (Southern Senegal). The phonological features examined include [ATR] vowel harmony, backness harmony, lenition, and Eegimaa’s typologically unusual geminate consonants. Most of the paper, however, focuses on Eegimaa morphology. My analysis of the noun class system separates morphological classes from agreement classes (genders), and presents the most important principles of semantic categorization, including shape encoding. I also show that Eegimaa classifies nouns and verbs by the same overt linguistic means, namely, noun class prefixes. I argue that this overt classification of nouns and verbs reflects parallel semantic categorization of entities and events. Other prominent typological features include associative plural marking and nominal TAM marking with the inactualis suffix, which also expresses alienability contrasts.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"69690483","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Probing the interaction of language contact and internal innovation 探究语言接触与内部创新的互动关系
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-07-03 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v48i1.114932
Hannah Gibson, L. Marten
The Bantu language Rangi is spoken at the northern borderlands of Tanzania, where Bantu, Cushitic and Nilotic languages meet. In many regards, Rangi exhibits the morphosyntax typically associated with East African Bantu: SVO word order, an extensive system of agreement and predominantly head-marking morphology. However, the language also exhibits a number of features which are unusual from a comparative and typological perspective, and which may have resulted from language contact. Four of these features are examined in detail in this paper: 1) Verb-auxiliary order found in the future tense, 2) clause-final negation, 3) a three-way distinction in verbal deictic markers, and 4) an inclusive/exclusive distinction in personal possessive pronouns. These features are assessed with reference to three criteria: syntactic structure, lexical/morphological form and geographic distribution. The examination shows that two of the unusual features result from a combination of internal and external factors, while the other two appear not to be related to external influence through contact. The results of the study show the complex interaction between internal and external factors in language change, and the importance of investigating potentially contact-induced change in detail to develop a more complex and fine-grained understanding of the morphosyntactic process of innovation involved.
班图语Rangi在坦桑尼亚北部边境地区使用,班图语、库希特语和尼罗河语在这里交汇。在许多方面,兰吉语表现出与东非班图语典型相关的形态句法:SVO语序,一个广泛的一致系统,主要是头部标记形态。然而,从比较和类型学的角度来看,这种语言也表现出一些不同寻常的特征,这些特征可能是语言接触的结果。本文详细研究了这四个特征:1)将来时的动助动词顺序,2)从句-最终否定,3)言语指示标记的三向区别,4)人称所有代词的包容/排斥区别。这些特征的评估参考了三个标准:句法结构,词汇/形态和地理分布。检查表明,其中两个异常特征是由内部和外部因素共同作用的结果,而另外两个似乎与通过接触受到的外部影响无关。研究结果显示了语言变化中内部和外部因素之间复杂的相互作用,以及详细研究潜在的接触诱发变化的重要性,以便对所涉及的创新的形态句法过程进行更复杂和更细致的理解。
{"title":"Probing the interaction of language contact and internal innovation","authors":"Hannah Gibson, L. Marten","doi":"10.32473/sal.v48i1.114932","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v48i1.114932","url":null,"abstract":"The Bantu language Rangi is spoken at the northern borderlands of Tanzania, where Bantu, Cushitic and Nilotic languages meet. In many regards, Rangi exhibits the morphosyntax typically associated with East African Bantu: SVO word order, an extensive system of agreement and predominantly head-marking morphology. However, the language also exhibits a number of features which are unusual from a comparative and typological perspective, and which may have resulted from language contact. Four of these features are examined in detail in this paper: 1) Verb-auxiliary order found in the future tense, 2) clause-final negation, 3) a three-way distinction in verbal deictic markers, and 4) an inclusive/exclusive distinction in personal possessive pronouns. These features are assessed with reference to three criteria: syntactic structure, lexical/morphological form and geographic distribution. The examination shows that two of the unusual features result from a combination of internal and external factors, while the other two appear not to be related to external influence through contact. The results of the study show the complex interaction between internal and external factors in language change, and the importance of investigating potentially contact-induced change in detail to develop a more complex and fine-grained understanding of the morphosyntactic process of innovation involved.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43561613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Quand le dictionnaire donne à voir la culture 当字典让你看到文化
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-12-11 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v47i1.107654
Sylvester N. Osu
L’ikwere, une langue Benue-Congo parlée au sud-est du Nigeria, est une langue à tradition orale, très peu décrite et très mal documentée. C’est donc pour contribuer à une meilleure documentation de cette langue que j’ai entrepris la confection d’un dictionnaire trilingue ikwere-anglais-français. Or, ce projet de dictionnaire se veut le reflet de la culture ikwere. En prenant comme exemple quelques noms de personne et de lieu mais aussi le calendrier et le vocabulaire du mariage, cet article montre comment j’aborde des aspects de la culture ikwere dans les entrées de ce projet de dictionnaire sur lequel je travaille à l’aide du logiciel Toolbox depuis quelques années. Ainsi, je donne pour les entrées, et lorsque cela me paraît à la fois pertinent pour mieux cerner l’entrée en question, et illustratif pour la culture ikwere, des renseignements élaborés dans le champ « notes et commentaires ». Ce projet de dictionnaire comprend actuellement 2360 entrées.
伊克维尔语是尼日利亚东南部的一种贝努-刚果语,是一种口头语言,很少被描述和记录。因此,为了更好地记录这种语言,我开始编写伊克维尔-英法三语词典。然而,这本词典草案旨在反映伊克维尔文化。为例的几个人的名字和地点,而且时机和婚姻的词汇方面,本文描述了如何深究该字典条目中的文化ikwere软件我工作赖以救命的Toolbox好几年了。因此,我在“注释和评论”字段中为条目提供详细的信息,在我看来,这与更好地理解问题条目和说明ikwere文化有关。这个词典项目目前包含2360个条目。
{"title":"Quand le dictionnaire donne à voir la culture","authors":"Sylvester N. Osu","doi":"10.32473/sal.v47i1.107654","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v47i1.107654","url":null,"abstract":"L’ikwere, une langue Benue-Congo parlée au sud-est du Nigeria, est une langue à tradition orale, très peu décrite et très mal documentée. C’est donc pour contribuer à une meilleure documentation de cette langue que j’ai entrepris la confection d’un dictionnaire trilingue ikwere-anglais-français. Or, ce projet de dictionnaire se veut le reflet de la culture ikwere. En prenant comme exemple quelques noms de personne et de lieu mais aussi le calendrier et le vocabulaire du mariage, cet article montre comment j’aborde des aspects de la culture ikwere dans les entrées de ce projet de dictionnaire sur lequel je travaille à l’aide du logiciel Toolbox depuis quelques années. Ainsi, je donne pour les entrées, et lorsque cela me paraît à la fois pertinent pour mieux cerner l’entrée en question, et illustratif pour la culture ikwere, des renseignements élaborés dans le champ « notes et commentaires ». Ce projet de dictionnaire comprend actuellement 2360 entrées.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46583149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The inclusion of prefixal material in Zulu reduplication 祖鲁叠字中前缀材料的包含
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-12-11 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v47i1.107653
T. Cook
The aim of this paper is twofold: to introduce new data that show reduplication in Zulu admits inflectional morphemes to the left of the verb stem rather than within the stem on the right, and to give an analysis of Zulu reduplication that does not require access to underlying morphosyntactic structure. Instead, reduplication is a template-filling copying operation that interacts with linear phonological structures and does not target morphosyntactic objects. The data presented here argue for an analysis in which the crucial distinction is between morphemes within the scope of reduplication and those outside it; the left/right asymmetry in the appearance of inflectional morphemes in the reduplicant is attributable to a process of Local Dislocation (Embick 2007). Reduplication is treated as a copying operation that has indirect access only to morphosyntactic structure through phonological operations. Consequently, reified subconstituents of the verb complex, such as the Macrostem (Hyman, Inkelas, and Sibanda 2009), are not accessible as such to the process of reduplication. However, data from VCV stems show that morphosyntactic structure affects reduplication in certain constructions. Tonal data is used to show that the “prefixal” affiliation of prefixal morphemes is voided if these morphemes are reduplicated.
本文的目的有两个:引入新的数据,表明祖鲁语的重复允许词干左侧的屈折语素,而不是在右边的词干内,并给出祖鲁语重复的分析,不需要访问潜在的形态句法结构。相反,重复是一种模板填充复制操作,它与线性语音结构相互作用,不针对形态句法对象。这里提供的数据支持一种分析,其中关键的区别是在重复范围内的语素和在重复范围之外的语素;复制体中屈折语素出现的左右不对称可归因于局部错位过程(Embick 2007)。重复被视为一种拷贝操作,它只能通过语音操作间接访问形态句法结构。因此,动词复合体的具体化子成分,如Macrostem (Hyman, Inkelas, and Sibanda 2009),在重复过程中是不可接近的。然而,来自VCV茎的数据表明,形态句法结构影响某些结构的重复。音调数据用于显示前缀语素的“前缀”从属关系,如果这些语素是重复的,则无效。
{"title":"The inclusion of prefixal material in Zulu reduplication","authors":"T. Cook","doi":"10.32473/sal.v47i1.107653","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v47i1.107653","url":null,"abstract":"The aim of this paper is twofold: to introduce new data that show reduplication in Zulu admits inflectional morphemes to the left of the verb stem rather than within the stem on the right, and to give an analysis of Zulu reduplication that does not require access to underlying morphosyntactic structure. Instead, reduplication is a template-filling copying operation that interacts with linear phonological structures and does not target morphosyntactic objects. The data presented here argue for an analysis in which the crucial distinction is between morphemes within the scope of reduplication and those outside it; the left/right asymmetry in the appearance of inflectional morphemes in the reduplicant is attributable to a process of Local Dislocation (Embick 2007). Reduplication is treated as a copying operation that has indirect access only to morphosyntactic structure through phonological operations. Consequently, reified subconstituents of the verb complex, such as the Macrostem (Hyman, Inkelas, and Sibanda 2009), are not accessible as such to the process of reduplication. However, data from VCV stems show that morphosyntactic structure affects reduplication in certain constructions. Tonal data is used to show that the “prefixal” affiliation of prefixal morphemes is voided if these morphemes are reduplicated.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47455925","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The interaction of number and gender in Katcha 《恰恰》中数字与性别的互动
Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-12-11 DOI: 10.32473/sal.v47i1.107656
D. Turner
The Kadu languages of Sudan’s Nuba Mountains have been the subject of an ongoing controversy regarding whether they should be classified as Niger-Congo, Nilo-Saharan, or as an independent family. Against this background, I present novel data from nouns in Katcha. I show that not only does the number system have elements typical of both NiloSaharan and Niger-Congo, but that in its interaction with gender it is strikingly reminiscent of Afro-Asiatic, in ways that are typologically unusual. Where nouns are morphologically marked for number, the affix and not the root determines gender, leading to the type of gender polarity more commonly observed in Semitic. More unusually, and more controversially, the semantic basis of the third gender appears to be plurality. ‘Plural gender’ has been argued to exist in some Cushitic languages, but has never previously been documented outside that family.
苏丹努巴山区的卡杜语一直是争论的主题,争论的焦点是它们是否应该被归类为尼日尔-刚果语,尼罗-撒哈拉语,还是作为一个独立的家族。在此背景下,我提出了来自Katcha的名词的新数据。我不仅展示了数字系统具有尼罗哈拉和尼日尔-刚果的典型元素,而且在与性别的互动中,它以不同寻常的类型学方式,惊人地让人想起亚非语系。当名词在形态上被标记为数字时,词缀而不是词根决定性别,导致在闪米特语中更常见的性别极性类型。更不寻常、更有争议的是,第三性别的语义基础似乎是多元的。“复数性别”被认为存在于一些库希特语言中,但此前从未在库希特语系以外的语言中有过记载。
{"title":"The interaction of number and gender in Katcha","authors":"D. Turner","doi":"10.32473/sal.v47i1.107656","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32473/sal.v47i1.107656","url":null,"abstract":"The Kadu languages of Sudan’s Nuba Mountains have been the subject of an ongoing controversy regarding whether they should be classified as Niger-Congo, Nilo-Saharan, or as an independent family. Against this background, I present novel data from nouns in Katcha. I show that not only does the number system have elements typical of both NiloSaharan and Niger-Congo, but that in its interaction with gender it is strikingly reminiscent of Afro-Asiatic, in ways that are typologically unusual. Where nouns are morphologically marked for number, the affix and not the root determines gender, leading to the type of gender polarity more commonly observed in Semitic. More unusually, and more controversially, the semantic basis of the third gender appears to be plurality. ‘Plural gender’ has been argued to exist in some Cushitic languages, but has never previously been documented outside that family.","PeriodicalId":35170,"journal":{"name":"Studies in African Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43723265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Studies in African Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1