首页 > 最新文献

心理学报最新文献

英文 中文
Effects associated with long-term training in sports requiring high levels of strategy on brain white matter structure in expert players: A DTI study 需要高水平策略的长期运动训练对专家运动员脑白质结构的影响:一项DTI研究
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00798
Yapeng Qi, Yixuan Wang, Huahong Zhu, Chenglin Zhou, Yingying Wang
existed between fractional anisotropy (FA) values and training time. Consistent with our hypothesis, the white matter microstructure properties of both the dorsal and ventral pathways in expert table tennis players significantly differed from those in nonplayers. Specifically, FA values in the bilateral corticospinal tracts, which mainly connect brain regions in the dorsal sensorimotor system, were higher in experts than in nonplayers. Compared with nonplayers, expert players also had higher FA values in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus and bilateral inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus of the ventral pathway, which are involved in higher-level cognitive processing, such as semantic processing or thinking. By contrast, no white matter region showed a higher FA value in nonplayers than in expert players, and no region was found with axial diffusivity difference between the groups. Additionally, radial diffusivity was lower in the left superior longitudinal fasciculus and bilateral inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus in experts than in nonplayers. Correlation analysis of the expert group showed significant positive correlations between training time and FA values in both the left superior longitudinal fasciculus in the ventral pathway and bilateral corticospinal tracts in the dorsal pathway. Taken together, these findings suggest that enhanced structural integrity of the white matter in both the dorsal and ventral pathways is associated with long-term, expert table tennis training. The observed structural plasticity is conducive to promoting cognitive processing of concrete sensorimotor and abstract information, which would enable expert players to excel at sports requiring a high level of strategy.
分数各向异性(FA)值与训练时间之间存在差异。与我们的假设一致,专业乒乓球运动员的背侧和腹侧通路的白质微观结构特性与非运动员的白质微结构特性显著不同。具体而言,双侧皮质脊髓束的FA值在专家中高于非专家,皮质脊髓束主要连接背侧感觉运动系统中的大脑区域。与非玩家相比,专业玩家在腹侧通路的左下纵束和双侧下枕额束中也有更高的FA值,这涉及更高级别的认知处理,如语义处理或思维。相比之下,没有一个白质区域在非玩家中显示出比专业玩家更高的FA值,也没有发现两组之间存在轴向扩散率差异的区域。此外,专家组左侧上纵束和双侧下枕额束的径向扩散率低于非专家组。专家组的相关分析显示,训练时间与腹侧通路左上纵束和背侧通路双侧皮质脊髓束的FA值均呈显著正相关。总之,这些发现表明,背侧和腹侧通路白质结构完整性的增强与长期的专业乒乓球训练有关。观察到的结构可塑性有助于促进对具体感觉运动和抽象信息的认知处理,这将使专业选手能够在需要高水平策略的运动中脱颖而出。
{"title":"Effects associated with long-term training in sports requiring high levels of strategy on brain white matter structure in expert players: A DTI study","authors":"Yapeng Qi, Yixuan Wang, Huahong Zhu, Chenglin Zhou, Yingying Wang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00798","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00798","url":null,"abstract":"existed between fractional anisotropy (FA) values and training time. Consistent with our hypothesis, the white matter microstructure properties of both the dorsal and ventral pathways in expert table tennis players significantly differed from those in nonplayers. Specifically, FA values in the bilateral corticospinal tracts, which mainly connect brain regions in the dorsal sensorimotor system, were higher in experts than in nonplayers. Compared with nonplayers, expert players also had higher FA values in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus and bilateral inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus of the ventral pathway, which are involved in higher-level cognitive processing, such as semantic processing or thinking. By contrast, no white matter region showed a higher FA value in nonplayers than in expert players, and no region was found with axial diffusivity difference between the groups. Additionally, radial diffusivity was lower in the left superior longitudinal fasciculus and bilateral inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus in experts than in nonplayers. Correlation analysis of the expert group showed significant positive correlations between training time and FA values in both the left superior longitudinal fasciculus in the ventral pathway and bilateral corticospinal tracts in the dorsal pathway. Taken together, these findings suggest that enhanced structural integrity of the white matter in both the dorsal and ventral pathways is associated with long-term, expert table tennis training. The observed structural plasticity is conducive to promoting cognitive processing of concrete sensorimotor and abstract information, which would enable expert players to excel at sports requiring a high level of strategy.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41865258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Spillover effects of third-party punishment on cooperation: A norm-based explanation 第三方惩罚对合作的溢出效应:一个基于规范的解释
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00758
Sijing Chen, Yilin Xing, Yi-Yao Weng, Chang Li
stronger incentive to defect. However, the results revealed a significantly higher cooperation level in LC. A plausible explanation is that the defection cost in form of punishment served as a norm reminder, but cost in form of tax lacked this function, implying that even defectors are not necessarily benefit maximizers. The results of Experiment 2 confirmed this explanation: compared with unpunished defectors, the punished ones manifested a higher level of norm activation. The bootstrap analysis showed that the norm activation completely mediated TPP and cooperation. Experiment 2 also found a spillover effect of TPP: the punished defectors still demonstrated a high cooperation in a new different game where the sanction was absent. Finally, Experiment 3 found another spillover effect of TPP: bystanders who did not experience the punishment in person but witnessed it showed a significantly higher cooperation in subsequent interactions. In conclusion, oftentimes, people defect simply because they are unaware of the existence of a certain norm, and activating people’s norms through TPP can significantly reduce their selfish behaviors. In addition to being an economic means to reduce defectors’ payoff, TPP serves as a norm reminder. The two spillover effects found in this study suggest that TPP as a means of norm activation may be more efficient than as an economic means because of its cost-effectiveness. These findings shed new light on the understanding of extensive cooperation among genetically unrelated individuals.
更强烈的叛逃动机。然而,研究结果显示,LC的合作水平明显更高。一个合理的解释是,惩罚形式的叛逃成本起到了规范提醒的作用,但税收形式的成本缺乏这一功能,这意味着即使叛逃者也不一定是利益最大化者。实验2的结果证实了这一解释:与未受惩罚的叛逃者相比,受惩罚的脱北者表现出更高水平的规范激活。bootstrap分析表明,规范激活完全介导了TPP和合作。实验2还发现了TPP的溢出效应:在没有制裁的情况下,被惩罚的脱北者在一个新的不同游戏中仍然表现出高度合作。最后,实验3发现了TPP的另一个溢出效应:没有亲身经历惩罚但目睹惩罚的旁观者在随后的互动中表现出显著更高的合作。总之,人们叛逃往往只是因为他们不知道某个规范的存在,而通过TPP激活人们的规范可以显著减少他们的自私行为。除了作为减少叛逃者回报的经济手段外,TPP还起到了规范提醒的作用。本研究中发现的两种溢出效应表明,TPP作为规范激活的手段可能比作为经济手段更有效,因为它具有成本效益。这些发现为理解基因无关个体之间的广泛合作提供了新的线索。
{"title":"Spillover effects of third-party punishment on cooperation: A norm-based explanation","authors":"Sijing Chen, Yilin Xing, Yi-Yao Weng, Chang Li","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00758","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00758","url":null,"abstract":"stronger incentive to defect. However, the results revealed a significantly higher cooperation level in LC. A plausible explanation is that the defection cost in form of punishment served as a norm reminder, but cost in form of tax lacked this function, implying that even defectors are not necessarily benefit maximizers. The results of Experiment 2 confirmed this explanation: compared with unpunished defectors, the punished ones manifested a higher level of norm activation. The bootstrap analysis showed that the norm activation completely mediated TPP and cooperation. Experiment 2 also found a spillover effect of TPP: the punished defectors still demonstrated a high cooperation in a new different game where the sanction was absent. Finally, Experiment 3 found another spillover effect of TPP: bystanders who did not experience the punishment in person but witnessed it showed a significantly higher cooperation in subsequent interactions. In conclusion, oftentimes, people defect simply because they are unaware of the existence of a certain norm, and activating people’s norms through TPP can significantly reduce their selfish behaviors. In addition to being an economic means to reduce defectors’ payoff, TPP serves as a norm reminder. The two spillover effects found in this study suggest that TPP as a means of norm activation may be more efficient than as an economic means because of its cost-effectiveness. These findings shed new light on the understanding of extensive cooperation among genetically unrelated individuals.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43028243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Average percept in ensemble perception is based on morphed average object: Evidence from average facial attractiveness 综合感知中的平均感知基于变形平均对象:来自平均面部吸引力的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00714
Tian Xin-Ran, Hou Wen-Xia, OU Yuxiao, Yi Bing, Cehn Wenfeng, Shang Junchen
{"title":"Average percept in ensemble perception is based on morphed average object: Evidence from average facial attractiveness","authors":"Tian Xin-Ran, Hou Wen-Xia, OU Yuxiao, Yi Bing, Cehn Wenfeng, Shang Junchen","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00714","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00714","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"714"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44546210","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Effects of contextual facilitation and inhibitory reaction in lexical ambiguity resolution for the Han and Uyghur nationalities 语境促进和抑制反应在汉、维吾尔族词汇歧义消解中的作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00746
Yang Qun, Zhang Jijia, Fan Conghui
was significantly bigger than that of the Uyghur students in 200 ms. The inhibitory reaction effect was found in two SOAs for the Han students but only found in 1000 ms for Uyghur students. In lexical ambiguity resolution, the ability to extract the accurate meaning and suppress the irrelevant meaning according to the context is important. According to the context, the Uyghur students could activate accurate and irrelevant meanings in the sentence but could not immediately reject the irrelative meaning. The Uyghur students took a long time to inhibit the improper meaning of the ambiguity words.
在200ms内显著大于维吾尔族学生。汉族学生在两个SOA中发现了抑制反应效应,而维吾尔族学生仅在1000ms内发现了抑制作用。在词汇歧义消解中,根据上下文提取准确含义和抑制无关含义的能力至关重要。根据语境,维吾尔族学生可以激活句子中准确和无关的含义,但不能立即拒绝不相关的含义。维吾尔族学生花了很长时间来抑制歧义词的不当含义。
{"title":"Effects of contextual facilitation and inhibitory reaction in lexical ambiguity resolution for the Han and Uyghur nationalities","authors":"Yang Qun, Zhang Jijia, Fan Conghui","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00746","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00746","url":null,"abstract":"was significantly bigger than that of the Uyghur students in 200 ms. The inhibitory reaction effect was found in two SOAs for the Han students but only found in 1000 ms for Uyghur students. In lexical ambiguity resolution, the ability to extract the accurate meaning and suppress the irrelevant meaning according to the context is important. According to the context, the Uyghur students could activate accurate and irrelevant meanings in the sentence but could not immediately reject the irrelative meaning. The Uyghur students took a long time to inhibit the improper meaning of the ambiguity words.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47143495","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Propranolol rescued secondary trauma induced by immediate extinction 心得安可挽救立即消失的继发性创伤
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00603
Hongbo Wang, Xiaoli Xing, Huiying Wang
{"title":"Propranolol rescued secondary trauma induced by immediate extinction","authors":"Hongbo Wang, Xiaoli Xing, Huiying Wang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00603","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00603","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"603"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47958565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The neural mechanism of the aesthetics of dynamic animal-stick figures 动态动物简笔画美学的神经机制
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00575
Zhao Xueru, Li Ting, Jinhui Li, Xian-You He, Zhang Wei, Guangyao Chen
“The love of beauty is an essential part of all healthy human nature.” Aesthetic need is a high-level spiritual pursuit of human beings. In recent years, researchers have gradually paid more and more attention to the importance of beauty. Although researchers have carried out many aesthetic studies, most have focused on the study of static stimuli and not dynamic stimuli such as flying birds or fast trotting horses. Thus, research in cognitive experimental aesthetics and cognitive neuroaesthetics has not addressed the following questions: beautiful. In Experiment 2 we explored neural mechanisms that underlie aesthetic judgment of dynamic animal-stick figures and compared the neural mechanisms between the aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures and static ones. 20 participants who did not participate in Experiment 1 were scanned while they performed aesthetic judgments on dynamic animal-stick figures and matched static animal-stick figures. Results revealed that regions of occipital lobe, frontal lobe, hippocampus, cingulate cortex, insula, orbital frontal cortex (OFC) and amygdala were commonly activated in the aesthetic judgments of both dynamic and static animal-stick figures. The neural networks involved in aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures overlapped with those involved in aesthetic judgments of static animal-stick figures. Furthermore, compared to static animal-stick figures, stronger activations of lingual gyrus and middle temporal gyrus (MT/V5) were found in the aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures. However, compared to dynamic animal-stick figures, no significant activations were found in beautiful judgments of static animal-stick figures. In summary, the present study indicated that the dynamic property of animal-stick figures affected aesthetic judgment and dynamic animal-stick figures were more beautiful than static ones.
“爱美是一切健康人性的重要组成部分。”审美需要是人类高层次的精神追求。近年来,研究者越来越重视美的重要性。尽管研究人员进行了许多美学研究,但大多数研究都集中在静态刺激的研究上,而不是动态刺激,如飞行的鸟类或快速奔跑的马。因此,认知实验美学和认知神经美学的研究并没有解决以下问题:美。在实验2中,我们探索了动态动物棒状图形审美判断的神经机制,并比较了动态动物条形图形和静态动物条形图形审美判断之间的神经机制。20名未参加实验1的参与者在对动态动物棒形和匹配静态动物棒形进行审美判断时被扫描。结果表明,在动态和静态动物棒状图形的审美判断中,枕叶、额叶、海马、扣带皮层、岛叶、眶额皮层(OFC)和杏仁核区域通常被激活。参与动态动物棒形的审美判断的神经网络与参与静态动物棒形审美判断的神经元网络重叠。此外,与静态动物棒状图形相比,在动态动物棒状图形的审美判断中,舌回和颞中回(MT/V5)的激活更强。然而,与动态动物棒状图形相比,在静态动物棒状图形的美丽判断中没有发现显著的激活。总之,本研究表明,动物棒形的动态特性影响审美判断,动态动物棒形比静态动物棒形更美观。
{"title":"The neural mechanism of the aesthetics of dynamic animal-stick figures","authors":"Zhao Xueru, Li Ting, Jinhui Li, Xian-You He, Zhang Wei, Guangyao Chen","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00575","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00575","url":null,"abstract":"“The love of beauty is an essential part of all healthy human nature.” Aesthetic need is a high-level spiritual pursuit of human beings. In recent years, researchers have gradually paid more and more attention to the importance of beauty. Although researchers have carried out many aesthetic studies, most have focused on the study of static stimuli and not dynamic stimuli such as flying birds or fast trotting horses. Thus, research in cognitive experimental aesthetics and cognitive neuroaesthetics has not addressed the following questions: beautiful. In Experiment 2 we explored neural mechanisms that underlie aesthetic judgment of dynamic animal-stick figures and compared the neural mechanisms between the aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures and static ones. 20 participants who did not participate in Experiment 1 were scanned while they performed aesthetic judgments on dynamic animal-stick figures and matched static animal-stick figures. Results revealed that regions of occipital lobe, frontal lobe, hippocampus, cingulate cortex, insula, orbital frontal cortex (OFC) and amygdala were commonly activated in the aesthetic judgments of both dynamic and static animal-stick figures. The neural networks involved in aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures overlapped with those involved in aesthetic judgments of static animal-stick figures. Furthermore, compared to static animal-stick figures, stronger activations of lingual gyrus and middle temporal gyrus (MT/V5) were found in the aesthetic judgments of dynamic animal-stick figures. However, compared to dynamic animal-stick figures, no significant activations were found in beautiful judgments of static animal-stick figures. In summary, the present study indicated that the dynamic property of animal-stick figures affected aesthetic judgment and dynamic animal-stick figures were more beautiful than static ones.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41473841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abnormalities in pain sensitivity among individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from meta-analysis 自闭症谱系障碍患者疼痛敏感性异常:来自荟萃分析的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00613
Wenyun Zhang, Xiaoyun Li, Junjie Yao, Qian Ye, W. Peng
{"title":"Abnormalities in pain sensitivity among individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from meta-analysis","authors":"Wenyun Zhang, Xiaoyun Li, Junjie Yao, Qian Ye, W. Peng","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00613","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00613","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"613"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48239996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Effects of prediction error and acute stress on retrieval-extinction of fear memories of different strengths 预测误差和急性应激对不同强度恐惧记忆提取-消退的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00587
Junjiao Li, Wei Chen, Yanjian Hu, Jingwen Caoyang, Xifu Zheng
(1) to test the effect of memory strength in a human on (2) examine the of during on destabilizing different strength memories and (3) to test the possible of post-reactivation exogenous stress to the of retrieval-extinction study. We manipulated memory strength through two kinds of acquisition procedures on the day, varied the predictability of the unconditioned stimulus (US) occurrence after the conditioned Twenty-four hours later, a reminder containing a single PE was used to reactivate memories, which was followed by a stress task (Social Evaluate Cold Pressor test, SECPT) or not before extinction. After 24 hours, a test of spontaneous recovery and reinstatement was utilized to measure the return of fear in each condition. All participants were divided into three conditions: CS-Predictable US_no Stress Group, CS-Unpredictable US_no Stress Group and the CS-Unpredictable US_Stress Group. Skin conductance response (SCR) and fear-potentiated startle response (FPS) were used as measurements of conditioned fear. The results showed that there was a relatively stronger increase in fear response (SCR) from Day 1 to Day 2 in the CS-Unpredictable US condition than the CS-Predictable US condition, which may suggest a difference in memory strength between conditions. And for the weak fear memory (CS-predictable US), the reactivation that involved a single PE and was followed by extinction training prevented the spontaneous recovery, especially on the SCR measurement. On the other hand, in the enhanced memory condition (CS-unpredictable US), the extinguished memory returned in the memory test on the third day, which suggests a failure of memory destabilization. Furthermore, when the post-reactivation acute stress task was adopted in the enhanced memory condition, the return of fear further increased, compared with the no stress manipulation conditions. These results indicate that PE used to destabilize weak memories is insufficient to destabilize strong memories; and that post-reactivation acute stress cannot nullify this deficit which is due to boundary conditions (e.g., strength). We discuss possible interpretations of these results and the implications for the translation of retrieval-extinction to clinical practice.
(1) 测试人类记忆强度对(2)在不稳定不同强度记忆的过程中的影响和(3)测试再激活后外源性应激对检索灭绝研究的可能性。我们在当天通过两种获取程序操纵记忆强度,改变条件刺激后非条件刺激(US)发生的可预测性。24小时后,使用包含单个PE的提醒来重新激活记忆,然后进行压力任务(社会评估冷压力测试,SECPT)或不在消失前进行。24小时后,使用自发恢复和恢复测试来测量每种情况下恐惧的恢复。所有参与者被分为三种情况:CS可预测US_no压力组、CS不可预测US_no压力组和CS不可预见US_Stress组。皮肤电导反应(SCR)和恐惧增强的惊吓反应(FPS)被用作条件恐惧的测量。结果表明,在CS Unpredictable US条件下,从第1天到第2天,恐惧反应(SCR)的增加相对较强,这可能表明不同条件下的记忆强度存在差异。对于较弱的恐惧记忆(CS可预测的US),涉及单个PE的再激活以及随后的消退训练阻止了自发恢复,尤其是在SCR测量方面。另一方面,在增强记忆条件下(CS不可预测US),熄灭的记忆在第三天的记忆测试中返回,这表明记忆不稳定失败。此外,当在增强记忆条件下采用再激活后急性应激任务时,与无应激操作条件相比,恐惧的回归进一步增加。这些结果表明,用于破坏弱记忆的PE不足以破坏强记忆;并且再激活后的急性应力不能消除由于边界条件(例如强度)引起的这种缺陷。我们讨论了对这些结果的可能解释,以及将检索灭绝转化为临床实践的意义。
{"title":"Effects of prediction error and acute stress on retrieval-extinction of fear memories of different strengths","authors":"Junjiao Li, Wei Chen, Yanjian Hu, Jingwen Caoyang, Xifu Zheng","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00587","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00587","url":null,"abstract":"(1) to test the effect of memory strength in a human on (2) examine the of during on destabilizing different strength memories and (3) to test the possible of post-reactivation exogenous stress to the of retrieval-extinction study. We manipulated memory strength through two kinds of acquisition procedures on the day, varied the predictability of the unconditioned stimulus (US) occurrence after the conditioned Twenty-four hours later, a reminder containing a single PE was used to reactivate memories, which was followed by a stress task (Social Evaluate Cold Pressor test, SECPT) or not before extinction. After 24 hours, a test of spontaneous recovery and reinstatement was utilized to measure the return of fear in each condition. All participants were divided into three conditions: CS-Predictable US_no Stress Group, CS-Unpredictable US_no Stress Group and the CS-Unpredictable US_Stress Group. Skin conductance response (SCR) and fear-potentiated startle response (FPS) were used as measurements of conditioned fear. The results showed that there was a relatively stronger increase in fear response (SCR) from Day 1 to Day 2 in the CS-Unpredictable US condition than the CS-Predictable US condition, which may suggest a difference in memory strength between conditions. And for the weak fear memory (CS-predictable US), the reactivation that involved a single PE and was followed by extinction training prevented the spontaneous recovery, especially on the SCR measurement. On the other hand, in the enhanced memory condition (CS-unpredictable US), the extinguished memory returned in the memory test on the third day, which suggests a failure of memory destabilization. Furthermore, when the post-reactivation acute stress task was adopted in the enhanced memory condition, the return of fear further increased, compared with the no stress manipulation conditions. These results indicate that PE used to destabilize weak memories is insufficient to destabilize strong memories; and that post-reactivation acute stress cannot nullify this deficit which is due to boundary conditions (e.g., strength). We discuss possible interpretations of these results and the implications for the translation of retrieval-extinction to clinical practice.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48455324","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Outwardly strong but inwardly weak, pretensions to wealth? Exploring the impact of heterogeneous high self-esteem on materialism in a self-threat situation 外表强大但内心软弱,自诩富有?自我威胁情境下异质性高自尊对物质主义的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00667
Baoyan Yang, Shasha Chen, S. Su, F. Chen
{"title":"Outwardly strong but inwardly weak, pretensions to wealth? Exploring the impact of heterogeneous high self-esteem on materialism in a self-threat situation","authors":"Baoyan Yang, Shasha Chen, S. Su, F. Chen","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00667","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00667","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"667"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45055603","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The relationship between dispositional self-construal and empathy foringroup and outgroup members’ pain: evidence from ERPs 倾向性自我建构与对群内外成员痛苦的移情关系——来自ERP的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00629
Jie Chen, Kelly Wu, Yupeng Shi, Xiaoqing Ai
Empathy to others’ pain is key to human social interaction and development. Previous studies suggest that pain empathy is influenced by numerous factors, including different characteristics of the observed target and the observer. Moreover, the relationship between an observer and the target also plays an important role. Self-construal, a personality trait, reflects the extent to which people consider themselves as connected to or separated from others. Recently, researchers have found temporal self-construal priming could modulate empathic neural responses to pain and change racial in-group bias measured in the neural response to pain. Unlike temporary, situational self-construal, the dispositional self-construal is a stable trait, the formation of which is influenced by long-term cultural experience. Previous ERP studies have found dispositional self-construal to modulate self-relevant processing more rapidly than temporal self-construal. Thus, the present study explores whether dispositional self-construal is related to empathic neural responses to others’ pain and whether in-group bias mediates such modulation. It has been shown that the temporal processing of empathy for pain consists of an early emotional sharing stage and a late cognitive evaluation stage. Accordingly, we assumed dispositional self-construal would correlate with either automatic emotional sharing or top-down controlled processes of empathy for pain. Twenty-seven participants study. participants the models the pictures selected from their fellow-townsman WeChat groups and other WeChat groups Next, we electroencephalography (EEG) while part in a word-priming paradigm, in which a word cue (“ingroup” or “outgroup”) was presented for 500 ms, indicating the identity of the model, followed by a picture of the model’s face touched either by painful or non-painful stimulation. Participants were instructed to judge whether the model in the picture was feeling pain by pressing a button using the left or right index fingers. After the EEG recording, participants rated the intensity of pain supposedly felt by the model as well as the degree of unpleasantness they felt when they saw each picture. Participants also completed the 24-item SCS on a seven-point scale to evaluate their interdependence and independence of self-construal. The ERP results showed a significant ingroup bias in the early N2 component. Painful stimuli elicited a greater negative shift relative to non-painful stimuli in response to ingroup faces. No such effect was observed on outgroup faces. The N2 component has previously been associated with automatic emotion sharing. A long latency empathic response was also observed at P3 over parietal electrodes. Painful stimuli elicited larger P3 amplitudes than the non-painful stimuli in both ingroup and outgroup face conditions. The P3 component is related to the cognitively controlled process of pain empathy. Importantly, correlation analysis revealed significant relationsh
同情他人的痛苦是人类社会互动和发展的关键。先前的研究表明,疼痛移情受到许多因素的影响,包括被观察目标和观察者的不同特征。此外,观察者和目标之间的关系也起着重要作用。自我建构是一种人格特征,反映了人们认为自己与他人联系或分离的程度。最近,研究人员发现,时间自我建构启动可以调节移情神经对疼痛的反应,并改变神经对疼痛反应中的种族偏见。与暂时的情境性自我建构不同,倾向性自我建构是一种稳定的特质,其形成受到长期文化经验的影响。先前的ERP研究发现,倾向性自我建构比时间性自我建构更快地调节自我相关处理。因此,本研究探讨了倾向性自我建构是否与移情神经对他人痛苦的反应有关,以及群体偏见是否介导了这种调节。研究表明,对疼痛移情的时间过程包括早期的情感分享阶段和晚期的认知评估阶段。因此,我们假设倾向性的自我建构将与自动的情感分享或自上而下控制的对疼痛的移情过程相关。27名参与者参与了研究。参与者将从他们的同乡微信群和其他微信群中选择的图片建模。接下来,我们在参与单词启动范式时进行脑电图(EEG),在该范式中,单词线索(“内组”或“外组”)被呈现500毫秒,表明模型的身份,然后是被疼痛或非疼痛刺激触摸的模型面部的照片。参与者被要求通过用左手或右手食指按下按钮来判断照片中的模型是否感到疼痛。脑电图记录后,参与者对模型所感受到的疼痛强度以及他们看到每张照片时感受到的不愉快程度进行评分。参与者还完成了24个项目的七分量表SCS,以评估他们自我解释的相互依存性和独立性。ERP结果显示早期N2成分存在显著的组内偏倚。与非疼痛刺激相比,疼痛刺激引起了更大的负向转变,以应对组内面孔。在外群脸上没有观察到这种影响。N2分量先前已经与自动情绪共享相关联。在顶叶电极上的P3处也观察到长潜伏期的移情反应。在组内和组外面部条件下,疼痛刺激比非疼痛刺激引起更大的P3振幅。P3成分与疼痛移情的认知控制过程有关。重要的是,相关性分析揭示了在组内和组外面孔条件下,倾向性自我建构得分(相互依存减去独立得分)和P3的幅度差异(痛苦减去无痛苦刺激条件)之间的显著关系。此外,组内和组外面孔条件之间的相关系数相似。此外,我们观察到相互依存的自我解释水平与感知疼痛和自我不愉快的主观评级之间存在正相关关系。总之,本研究表明,在N2早期有显著的组内偏倚,但在P3晚期没有。此外,倾向性自我建构与组内和组外对疼痛的移情相关,并且高度相互依赖性与增强P3对他人疼痛的反应相关,而与组内成员无关。
{"title":"The relationship between dispositional self-construal and empathy foringroup and outgroup members’ pain: evidence from ERPs","authors":"Jie Chen, Kelly Wu, Yupeng Shi, Xiaoqing Ai","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00629","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00629","url":null,"abstract":"Empathy to others’ pain is key to human social interaction and development. Previous studies suggest that pain empathy is influenced by numerous factors, including different characteristics of the observed target and the observer. Moreover, the relationship between an observer and the target also plays an important role. Self-construal, a personality trait, reflects the extent to which people consider themselves as connected to or separated from others. Recently, researchers have found temporal self-construal priming could modulate empathic neural responses to pain and change racial in-group bias measured in the neural response to pain. Unlike temporary, situational self-construal, the dispositional self-construal is a stable trait, the formation of which is influenced by long-term cultural experience. Previous ERP studies have found dispositional self-construal to modulate self-relevant processing more rapidly than temporal self-construal. Thus, the present study explores whether dispositional self-construal is related to empathic neural responses to others’ pain and whether in-group bias mediates such modulation. It has been shown that the temporal processing of empathy for pain consists of an early emotional sharing stage and a late cognitive evaluation stage. Accordingly, we assumed dispositional self-construal would correlate with either automatic emotional sharing or top-down controlled processes of empathy for pain. Twenty-seven participants study. participants the models the pictures selected from their fellow-townsman WeChat groups and other WeChat groups Next, we electroencephalography (EEG) while part in a word-priming paradigm, in which a word cue (“ingroup” or “outgroup”) was presented for 500 ms, indicating the identity of the model, followed by a picture of the model’s face touched either by painful or non-painful stimulation. Participants were instructed to judge whether the model in the picture was feeling pain by pressing a button using the left or right index fingers. After the EEG recording, participants rated the intensity of pain supposedly felt by the model as well as the degree of unpleasantness they felt when they saw each picture. Participants also completed the 24-item SCS on a seven-point scale to evaluate their interdependence and independence of self-construal. The ERP results showed a significant ingroup bias in the early N2 component. Painful stimuli elicited a greater negative shift relative to non-painful stimuli in response to ingroup faces. No such effect was observed on outgroup faces. The N2 component has previously been associated with automatic emotion sharing. A long latency empathic response was also observed at P3 over parietal electrodes. Painful stimuli elicited larger P3 amplitudes than the non-painful stimuli in both ingroup and outgroup face conditions. The P3 component is related to the cognitively controlled process of pain empathy. Importantly, correlation analysis revealed significant relationsh","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46676380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
心理学报
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1