首页 > 最新文献

心理学报最新文献

英文 中文
N170 adaptation effect of the sub-lexical phonological and semantic processing in Chinese character reading N170亚词语音语义加工在汉字阅读中的适应效应
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-08-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00807
Rui Zhang, Zhenhu Wang, Xiaojuan Wang, Jianfeng Yang
The event-related potential (ERP) studies have revealed a component (N170) near tempo-occipital electrodes was sensitive to visual words. However, its role in word reading is still controversial. A common view is that the N170 engaged in the visual/orthographic processing, whereas some evidence has shown the N170 involved in phonological and semantic processing. By taking advantage of the Chinese writing system's ideographic property, the current study directly examined whether the N170 was sensitive to the phonological and semantic processing in reading Chinese characters. Two ERP experiments were conducted in a neural adaption paradigm by manipulating the repetition of the sub-lexical phonetic/semantic radical. The ERP data were collected while participants performed a phonological judgment task on the 4th character after silently reading the four characters consecutively. The phonological similarity (Experiment 1) and semantic similarity (Experiment 2) were manipulated among the four characters. Experiment 1 examined the neural adaption of the four characters sharing the phonetic radical (e.g., 敏 , 侮 , 悔 , 莓 ), the character's pronunciation (e.g., 妹 , 枚 , 镁 , 莓 ), both of them (e.g., 酶 , 梅 , 霉 , 莓 ), or neither (e.g., 淮 , 崛 , 郎 , 莓 ) respectively. Experiment 2 examined the neural adaption of the four characters sharing the semantic radical (e.g., 狡 , 狂 , 猜 , 狒 ), the character's meaning (e.g., 豹 , 鹿 , 羚 , 狒 ), of them (e.g., 狮 , 狼 , 狐 , 狒 ), , 崛 , 郎 , 狒 ) In both experiments, the results showed a significant neural adaption at N170 in all of the four conditions. The amplitude of the N170 observed in the 1st character decreased in the 2nd – 4th characters. In Experiment 1, the N170 neural adaptation at the left PO7 electrode was sensitive to the repetition of the phonetic radical, and the repetition of the character's pronunciation, but not to the repetition of both. These results indicated the left mid-fusiform gyrus might be sensitive to the visual/orthographic and phonological processing but not to the orthography-to-phonology mapping in Chinese character reading. In Experiment 2, the N170 neural adaption at the left PO7 electrode was only sensitive to the repetition of the character’s meaning, which indicated the semantic processing might modulate the left N170 in character reading. The N170 neural adaption at the right PO8 electrode was sensitive to the repetition of the semantic radical and the character's meaning, which suggested that right N170 was involved in visual/orthographic and semantic processing in reading characters. In sum, the findings showed that the N170 was involved in the visual/orthographic processing and engaged in the phonological and semantic processing in Chinese character reading. Furthermore, the left N170 was sensitive to the character's phonological and semantic information, whereas the right N170 was sensitive to the character’s meaning and its semantic radical.
事件相关电位(ERP)研究表明,颞枕电极附近的一个成分(N170)对视觉单词敏感。然而,它在单词阅读中的作用仍然存在争议。一种常见的观点是N170参与视觉/拼写处理,而一些证据表明N170参与语音和语义处理。利用汉语书写系统的表意特性,本研究直接考察了N170在阅读汉字时是否对语音和语义处理敏感。通过操纵亚词汇语音/语义部首的重复,在神经适应范式中进行了两个ERP实验。当参与者在连续默读四个字符后,对第四个字符进行语音判断任务时,收集ERP数据。对四个汉字的语音相似度(实验1)和语义相似度(实验2)进行了处理。实验1检验了共享语音部首的四个字符(例如。,敏 , 侮 , 悔 , 莓 ), 字符的发音(例如。,妹 , 枚 , 镁 , 莓 ), 它们两者(例如。,酶 , 梅 , 霉 , 莓 ), 或者两者都不(例如。,淮 , 崛 , 郎 , 莓 ) 分别地实验2检验了共享语义部首的四个字符(例如。,狡 , 狂 , 猜 , 狒 ), 字符的含义(例如。,豹 , 鹿 , 羚 , 狒 ), 其中(例如。,狮 , 狼 , 狐 , 狒 ), 崛 , 郎 , 狒 ) 在这两个实验中,结果显示,在所有四种条件下,N170都具有显著的神经适应。在第1个字符中观察到的N170的振幅在第2-4个字符中减小。在实验1中,左侧PO7电极处的N170神经适应对声旁的重复和字符发音的重复敏感,但对两者的重复不敏感。这些结果表明,在汉字阅读中,左梭状中回可能对视觉/拼写和语音处理敏感,但对拼写-语音映射不敏感。在实验2中,左侧PO7电极处的N170神经适应仅对字符含义的重复敏感,这表明语义处理可能在字符阅读中调节左侧N170。右侧PO8电极的N170神经适应对语义部首的重复和字符的含义敏感,这表明右侧N170参与了阅读字符的视觉/拼写和语义处理。总之,研究结果表明,N170参与了汉字阅读中的视觉/拼写处理,并参与了语音和语义处理。此外,左边的N170对字符的语音和语义信息敏感,而右边的N170则对字符的含义及其语义词根敏感。
{"title":"N170 adaptation effect of the sub-lexical phonological and semantic processing in Chinese character reading","authors":"Rui Zhang, Zhenhu Wang, Xiaojuan Wang, Jianfeng Yang","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00807","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00807","url":null,"abstract":"The event-related potential (ERP) studies have revealed a component (N170) near tempo-occipital electrodes was sensitive to visual words. However, its role in word reading is still controversial. A common view is that the N170 engaged in the visual/orthographic processing, whereas some evidence has shown the N170 involved in phonological and semantic processing. By taking advantage of the Chinese writing system's ideographic property, the current study directly examined whether the N170 was sensitive to the phonological and semantic processing in reading Chinese characters. Two ERP experiments were conducted in a neural adaption paradigm by manipulating the repetition of the sub-lexical phonetic/semantic radical. The ERP data were collected while participants performed a phonological judgment task on the 4th character after silently reading the four characters consecutively. The phonological similarity (Experiment 1) and semantic similarity (Experiment 2) were manipulated among the four characters. Experiment 1 examined the neural adaption of the four characters sharing the phonetic radical (e.g., 敏 , 侮 , 悔 , 莓 ), the character's pronunciation (e.g., 妹 , 枚 , 镁 , 莓 ), both of them (e.g., 酶 , 梅 , 霉 , 莓 ), or neither (e.g., 淮 , 崛 , 郎 , 莓 ) respectively. Experiment 2 examined the neural adaption of the four characters sharing the semantic radical (e.g., 狡 , 狂 , 猜 , 狒 ), the character's meaning (e.g., 豹 , 鹿 , 羚 , 狒 ), of them (e.g., 狮 , 狼 , 狐 , 狒 ), , 崛 , 郎 , 狒 ) In both experiments, the results showed a significant neural adaption at N170 in all of the four conditions. The amplitude of the N170 observed in the 1st character decreased in the 2nd – 4th characters. In Experiment 1, the N170 neural adaptation at the left PO7 electrode was sensitive to the repetition of the phonetic radical, and the repetition of the character's pronunciation, but not to the repetition of both. These results indicated the left mid-fusiform gyrus might be sensitive to the visual/orthographic and phonological processing but not to the orthography-to-phonology mapping in Chinese character reading. In Experiment 2, the N170 neural adaption at the left PO7 electrode was only sensitive to the repetition of the character’s meaning, which indicated the semantic processing might modulate the left N170 in character reading. The N170 neural adaption at the right PO8 electrode was sensitive to the repetition of the semantic radical and the character's meaning, which suggested that right N170 was involved in visual/orthographic and semantic processing in reading characters. In sum, the findings showed that the N170 was involved in the visual/orthographic processing and engaged in the phonological and semantic processing in Chinese character reading. Furthermore, the left N170 was sensitive to the character's phonological and semantic information, whereas the right N170 was sensitive to the character’s meaning and its semantic radical.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43801377","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation 愤怒对恶意创造力的影响及其情绪调节策略
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-08-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00847
Rui Cheng, Kelong Lu, N. Hao
.,
.,
{"title":"The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation","authors":"Rui Cheng, Kelong Lu, N. Hao","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00847","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00847","url":null,"abstract":".,","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45649312","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
The role of novel semantic association in the promoting effect of insight on memory 新颖语义联想在洞察力对记忆的促进作用中的作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-08-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00837
Shi Chen, Zheng Liang, Xiang Li, Yanran Chen, Qingbai Zhao, Quanlei Yu, Songqing Li, Zhijin Zhou, Lizhong Liu
Previous empirical research has found the effect of insight on promoting memory retention during problem solving. Furthermore, neuroimaging studies have revealed that the amygdala, which is assumed to be associated with Aha experience, plays an important role in long-term memory of insightful events. While the emotional Aha experience is a key characteristic of insightful problem solving, some researchers emphasized that the core processes in creating insights involve breaking the mental set and forming novel and valuable associations. However, the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying the promoting effect of insight on memory have not yet been fully explored. In this study, the paradigm of choice of answers to Chinese Chengyu riddles was adopted to investigate how the process of forming novel associations impacts the effect of insight on promoting subsequent memory. Two experiments were conducted in this study. In Experiment 1, the paradigm of choice of answers to Chinese Chengyu riddles consisted of two phases. In the learning phase, participants were asked to select the novel and suitable answer to the Chengyu riddle from four options; after one week, in the testing phase they were asked to recall the answer that they chose in the learning phase. The novel association and normal association condition were distinguished according to the selections of participants. The paradigm used in Experiment 2 was similar to that of Experiment 1. Additionally, the functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) was used to record the neural activity in the learning phase. The results of Experiment 1 showed that the score of Aha experience in the learning phase and accuracy in the testing phase were significantly higher in the novel association condition compared to the normal association condition. In addition, Experiment 2 showed that the activity in the brain regions related to insight, including the hippocampus, amygdala, middle frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus, were significantly greater in the successful recall compared to the failed recall in novel association condition. Further analysis indicated that successful recall of novel association involved more activity in the right hippocampus compared to the recall of normal association. The current study verified the promoting effect of insight on memory, and indicated that forming novel semantic associations and related activity in the hippocampus may underlie this effect.
先前的实证研究已经发现,在解决问题的过程中,洞察力对促进记忆保持的作用。此外,神经影像学研究表明,杏仁核被认为与Aha经历有关,在对深刻事件的长期记忆中发挥着重要作用。虽然情绪化的Aha体验是有洞察力的问题解决的一个关键特征,但一些研究人员强调,创造洞察力的核心过程包括打破思维定势,形成新颖而有价值的联想。然而,洞察力对记忆促进作用的认知和神经机制尚未得到充分探索。本研究采用汉语承语谜语答案选择范式,考察小说联想的形成过程如何影响洞察力对后续记忆的促进作用。本研究进行了两个实验。在实验一中,汉语承语谜语的答案选择范式分为两个阶段。在学习阶段,参与者被要求从四个选项中选择新颖、合适的成渝谜语答案;一周后,在测试阶段,他们被要求回忆他们在学习阶段选择的答案。根据参与者的选择来区分新联想和正常联想条件。实验2中使用的范例与实验1中使用的范例相似。此外,功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)用于记录学习阶段的神经活动。实验1的结果表明,与正常联想条件相比,在新联想条件下,学习阶段的Aha体验得分和测试阶段的准确性显著较高。此外,实验2表明,与新联想条件下的回忆失败相比,成功回忆中与洞察力相关的大脑区域,包括海马体、杏仁核、额中回、颞上回和颞中回的活动显著更大。进一步的分析表明,与正常联想的回忆相比,成功回忆新联想涉及右侧海马的更多活动。目前的研究证实了洞察力对记忆的促进作用,并表明在海马体中形成新的语义联想和相关活动可能是这种作用的基础。
{"title":"The role of novel semantic association in the promoting effect of insight on memory","authors":"Shi Chen, Zheng Liang, Xiang Li, Yanran Chen, Qingbai Zhao, Quanlei Yu, Songqing Li, Zhijin Zhou, Lizhong Liu","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00837","url":null,"abstract":"Previous empirical research has found the effect of insight on promoting memory retention during problem solving. Furthermore, neuroimaging studies have revealed that the amygdala, which is assumed to be associated with Aha experience, plays an important role in long-term memory of insightful events. While the emotional Aha experience is a key characteristic of insightful problem solving, some researchers emphasized that the core processes in creating insights involve breaking the mental set and forming novel and valuable associations. However, the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying the promoting effect of insight on memory have not yet been fully explored. In this study, the paradigm of choice of answers to Chinese Chengyu riddles was adopted to investigate how the process of forming novel associations impacts the effect of insight on promoting subsequent memory. Two experiments were conducted in this study. In Experiment 1, the paradigm of choice of answers to Chinese Chengyu riddles consisted of two phases. In the learning phase, participants were asked to select the novel and suitable answer to the Chengyu riddle from four options; after one week, in the testing phase they were asked to recall the answer that they chose in the learning phase. The novel association and normal association condition were distinguished according to the selections of participants. The paradigm used in Experiment 2 was similar to that of Experiment 1. Additionally, the functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) was used to record the neural activity in the learning phase. The results of Experiment 1 showed that the score of Aha experience in the learning phase and accuracy in the testing phase were significantly higher in the novel association condition compared to the normal association condition. In addition, Experiment 2 showed that the activity in the brain regions related to insight, including the hippocampus, amygdala, middle frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus, were significantly greater in the successful recall compared to the failed recall in novel association condition. Further analysis indicated that successful recall of novel association involved more activity in the right hippocampus compared to the recall of normal association. The current study verified the promoting effect of insight on memory, and indicated that forming novel semantic associations and related activity in the hippocampus may underlie this effect.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41590606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of normative misperception on food waste in dining out: Mechanism analyses and countermeasures 规范性误认对餐饮浪费的影响:机制分析与对策
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-08-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00904
Sijing Chen, Xue Pu, Yue Zhu, Hao Wang, Jianwei Liu
moderated the relationship between the misperception and behavior: misperception still existed, but its prescriptive power declined. The findings of this study highlight the importance of considering the distinction between the two types of normative misperception in social norm campaigns, and suggest two possible ways of correcting people’s normative misperception: providing descriptive normative information to decrease people’s behavioral misperception and providing injunctive normative information to ameliorate attitudinal misperception’s detrimental effect on behavior.
调节了误解与行为之间的关系:误解仍然存在,但其规定力有所下降。本研究的发现强调了在社会规范运动中考虑两种类型的规范误解之间的区别的重要性,并提出了纠正人们规范性错误认知的两种可能途径:提供描述性规范性信息以减少人们的行为错误认知;提供强制性规范性信息来改善态度错误认知对行为的有害影响。
{"title":"The impact of normative misperception on food waste in dining out: Mechanism analyses and countermeasures","authors":"Sijing Chen, Xue Pu, Yue Zhu, Hao Wang, Jianwei Liu","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00904","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00904","url":null,"abstract":"moderated the relationship between the misperception and behavior: misperception still existed, but its prescriptive power declined. The findings of this study highlight the importance of considering the distinction between the two types of normative misperception in social norm campaigns, and suggest two possible ways of correcting people’s normative misperception: providing descriptive normative information to decrease people’s behavioral misperception and providing injunctive normative information to ameliorate attitudinal misperception’s detrimental effect on behavior.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43947345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Changes of teachers' subjective well-being in mainland China (2002~2019): The perspective of cross-temporal meta-analysis 2002~2019年中国大陆教师主观幸福感变化:跨时间元分析视角
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-08-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00875
Sufei Xin, Xin Liang, Liang Sheng, Zhi-Min Zhao
{"title":"Changes of teachers' subjective well-being in mainland China (2002~2019): The perspective of cross-temporal meta-analysis","authors":"Sufei Xin, Xin Liang, Liang Sheng, Zhi-Min Zhao","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00875","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00875","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42724296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
The mediating effect of aggression motivation on the relationship between trait anger and reactive aggression: A longitudinal study 攻击动机对特质愤怒与反应性攻击关系的中介作用:一项纵向研究
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00788
Rui Li, L. Xia
Trait anger is a widely recognized susceptibility personality factor for reactive aggression. However, to the best of our knowledge, the longitudinal effect of trait anger on reactive aggression is unclear. More importantly, even though reactive aggression differs from proactive aggression by their motivations, the motivation mechanism underlying the effect of trait anger on reactive aggression is not known. Thus, the present study attempts to explore the longitudinal effect of trait anger on reactive aggression, and the mediating role of reactive aggression motivation, as well as the relationship between the two different motivations. Reactive aggression refers to the behavior or tendency to respond to perceived provocation with hostile and angry feeling. It is also called impulsive, hostile, or hot-blooded aggression. The motivation of the reactive aggression is comprised of hostile motivation (unique motivation) and moral approval motivation (common motivation). Hostile attribution bias is a typically representative variable of hostile motivation, and moral disengagement is the representative variable of moral approval motivation. A three-wave longitudinal study with the time interval of 6 months was conducted to test our hypotheses. A total of 1007 undergraduates (mean age = 19.00 years, SD = 0.99) from 5 provinces in China completed a series of questionnaires concerning trait anger, hostile attribution bias, moral disengagement, reactive aggression, and proactive aggression in their classrooms. SPSS 20.0 was used to conduct churn rate, reliability, and common method bias tests, and to calculate descriptive statistics. Mplus 7.0 was used to conduct item parceling and structural equation modeling. A cross-lagged model was developed for trait anger predicting reactive aggression from hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement. Moreover, longitudinal relationships among trait anger, hostile attribution bias, moral disengagement, and proactive aggression were also tested. The results indicate that there is no serious churn problem for participants, for all variables considered. All measurements show good reliability, and there is no serious common method bias. Moreover, trait anger at Wave 1 significantly predicts hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2, hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2 significantly predict reactive aggression at Wave 3, moral disengagement at Wave 1 significantly predicts hostile attribution bias at Wave 2, and hostile attribution bias at Wave 2 significantly predicts moral disengagement at Wave 3. After controlling for gender, trait anger at Wave 1 significantly predicts reactive aggression at Wave 3 through hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2. Furthermore, the path of hostile attribution bias predicting proactive aggression is non-significant. Moral disengagement at Wave 1 significantly predicts proactive aggression at Wave 2. This study supports that i
特质愤怒是公认的易受反应性攻击影响的人格因素。然而,据我们所知,特质愤怒对反应性攻击的纵向影响尚不清楚。更重要的是,尽管反应性攻击与主动性攻击的动机不同,但特质愤怒对反应性攻击影响的动机机制尚不清楚。因此,本研究试图探讨特质愤怒对反应性攻击的纵向影响,反应性攻击动机的中介作用,以及两种不同动机之间的关系。反应性攻击是指对感知到的带有敌意和愤怒情绪的挑衅做出反应的行为或倾向。它也被称为冲动、敌对或热血攻击。反应性攻击的动机包括敌对动机(独特动机)和道德认同动机(共同动机)。敌意归因偏差是敌意动机的典型代表变量,道德脱离是道德认同动机的代表变量。为了验证我们的假设,进行了一项时间间隔为6个月的三波纵向研究。来自中国5个省份的1007名本科生(平均年龄19.00岁,SD=0.99)在课堂上完成了一系列关于特质愤怒、敌对归因偏见、道德脱离、反应性攻击和主动性攻击的问卷调查。SPSS 20.0用于进行流失率、可靠性和常用方法偏差测试,并计算描述性统计数据。使用Mplus 7.0进行项目分组和结构方程建模。建立了一个跨滞后模型,用于预测来自敌对归因偏见和道德脱离的反应性攻击。此外,还测试了特质愤怒、敌对归因偏见、道德脱离和主动攻击之间的纵向关系。结果表明,对于所有考虑的变量,参与者不存在严重的流失问题。所有测量都显示出良好的可靠性,并且没有严重的常见方法偏差。此外,第1波的特质愤怒显著预测第2波的敌意归因偏见和道德脱离,第2波中的敌意归因偏差和道德脱离显著预测第3波中的反应性攻击,第1波中的道德脱离显著预示第2波时的敌意归因偏误,第二波的敌对归因偏见显著预测了第三波的道德脱离。在控制性别后,第1波的特质愤怒通过第2波的敌对归因偏见和道德脱离显著预测了第3波的反应性攻击。此外,敌意归因偏差预测主动攻击的路径并不显著。第1波的道德脱离显著预测了第2波的主动攻击。本研究支持了特质愤怒会促进反应性攻击的观点,并进一步表明,特质愤怒可以通过反应性攻击动机(包括以敌对归因偏见为代表的敌对动机和以道德脱离为代表的道德认可动机)的中介作用纵向预测反应性攻击。本研究构建了人格和反应性攻击的动机模型,从而发展了人格和攻击的理论和研究。此外,这项研究的结果表明,预防和干预反应性攻击可以有效地关注攻击的个性和动机因素。
{"title":"The mediating effect of aggression motivation on the relationship between trait anger and reactive aggression: A longitudinal study","authors":"Rui Li, L. Xia","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00788","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00788","url":null,"abstract":"Trait anger is a widely recognized susceptibility personality factor for reactive aggression. However, to the best of our knowledge, the longitudinal effect of trait anger on reactive aggression is unclear. More importantly, even though reactive aggression differs from proactive aggression by their motivations, the motivation mechanism underlying the effect of trait anger on reactive aggression is not known. Thus, the present study attempts to explore the longitudinal effect of trait anger on reactive aggression, and the mediating role of reactive aggression motivation, as well as the relationship between the two different motivations. Reactive aggression refers to the behavior or tendency to respond to perceived provocation with hostile and angry feeling. It is also called impulsive, hostile, or hot-blooded aggression. The motivation of the reactive aggression is comprised of hostile motivation (unique motivation) and moral approval motivation (common motivation). Hostile attribution bias is a typically representative variable of hostile motivation, and moral disengagement is the representative variable of moral approval motivation. A three-wave longitudinal study with the time interval of 6 months was conducted to test our hypotheses. A total of 1007 undergraduates (mean age = 19.00 years, SD = 0.99) from 5 provinces in China completed a series of questionnaires concerning trait anger, hostile attribution bias, moral disengagement, reactive aggression, and proactive aggression in their classrooms. SPSS 20.0 was used to conduct churn rate, reliability, and common method bias tests, and to calculate descriptive statistics. Mplus 7.0 was used to conduct item parceling and structural equation modeling. A cross-lagged model was developed for trait anger predicting reactive aggression from hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement. Moreover, longitudinal relationships among trait anger, hostile attribution bias, moral disengagement, and proactive aggression were also tested. The results indicate that there is no serious churn problem for participants, for all variables considered. All measurements show good reliability, and there is no serious common method bias. Moreover, trait anger at Wave 1 significantly predicts hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2, hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2 significantly predict reactive aggression at Wave 3, moral disengagement at Wave 1 significantly predicts hostile attribution bias at Wave 2, and hostile attribution bias at Wave 2 significantly predicts moral disengagement at Wave 3. After controlling for gender, trait anger at Wave 1 significantly predicts reactive aggression at Wave 3 through hostile attribution bias and moral disengagement at Wave 2. Furthermore, the path of hostile attribution bias predicting proactive aggression is non-significant. Moral disengagement at Wave 1 significantly predicts proactive aggression at Wave 2. This study supports that i","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42196049","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Effects of trial history on cross-modal non-spatial inhibition of return 试验史对跨模态非空间返回抑制的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00681
Zhang Ming, Sang Hanbin, Lu Ke, Wang Aijun
Previous laboratory studies have shown that an individual’s response in the current trial can be influenced by a previous trial, and this has been described as an effect of trial history. Existing studies have shown that there is a trial history effect with visual spatial inhibition of return (IOR), and some studies have shown that changes in stimulus modalities also affect reaction times (RTs). The present study used the “prime-neutral cue-target” paradigm to examine the trial history effect in cross-modal, non-spatial IOR and attempted to decrease the trial history effect. In two experiments, we mainly manipulated the cue-target modalities in the current trial (auditory-visual vs. visual-auditory modalities), cue validity in the current trial (cued vs. uncued) and cue validity in the previous trial (cued vs. uncued). Thirty participants were recruited in Experiment 1. The visual prime cue was a red or blue disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory prime cue was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (hong or lan). The visual neutral cue was a green disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory neutral cue was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (lv); The visual target was a red or blue disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory target was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (hong and lan). During the experiment, each trial began with a 400 ms fixation cross in the centre of the monitor, and a 300 ms visual or auditory prime cue was followed by a 200 ms fixation cross. After the 300 ms visual or auditory neutral cue, another fixation cross was presented for 300 ms, and then a 300 ms auditory or visual target was presented. The participants were asked to discriminate the identity of the target(i.e., either a colour disk or vocalization of hongor lan) within 1500 ms. Following a 1500 ms intertrial interval (ITI) with a blank screen, the next trial was initiated. Twenty-nine participants were recruited in Experiment 2, the ITI was 4500 ms, and the other parameters were identical to those in Experiment 1. Regarding the RTs results, Experiment 1 showed that the RTs for cued targets in the current trial were larger than RTs for uncued targets, which was a colour-based non-spatial IOR. The IOR effect size in the current trial showed an interaction between the cue validity in the previous trial and the cue-target modality in the current trial. The IOR effect size on the current trial after a valid cue trial was larger than the IOR effect size with an invalid cue in the previous trial when the current trial was a visual cue and auditory target; however, there was no difference in the IOR effect size when the cue was auditory, and the target was visual in the current trial. Furthermore, the analysis of the target modality across trials revealed that the valid cue, but not the invalid cue, in the previous trial, could induce a larger IOR effect size in the current trial with visual cues. A longer ITI (4500 ms) was used in Exp
先前的实验室研究表明,个体在当前试验中的反应可能会受到先前试验的影响,这被描述为试验史的影响。现有研究表明,视觉空间返回抑制(IOR)存在试验历史效应,一些研究表明,刺激方式的变化也会影响反应时间(RT)。本研究采用“主-中性线索-目标”范式来检验跨模态、非空间IOR中的试验历史效应,并试图降低试验历史效应。在两个实验中,我们主要操纵了当前试验中的线索目标模式(听觉-视觉模式与视觉-听觉模式)、当前试验的线索有效性(提示与未提示)和前一试验的线索合法性(提示和未提示)。实验1招募了30名参与者。视觉主线索是半径为2°视角的红色或蓝色圆盘,听觉主线索是75 dB的汉语语音。视觉中性提示是一个半径为2°视角的绿色圆盘,听觉中性提示是75 dB(lv)的汉语语音;视觉目标是半径为2°视角的红色或蓝色圆盘,听觉目标是75 dB(hong和lan)的汉语语音。在实验过程中,每次试验都从监视器中心400毫秒的注视交叉开始,300毫秒的视觉或听觉主要提示之后是200毫秒的注视十字。在300毫秒的视觉或听觉中性提示之后,另一个注视交叉出现300毫秒,然后出现300毫秒的听觉或视觉目标。参与者被要求在1500毫秒内辨别目标的身份(即彩色圆盘或“hong”或“lan”的发音)。在空白屏幕上进行1500毫秒的试验间隔(ITI)后,开始下一次试验。实验2中招募了29名参与者,ITI为4500ms,其他参数与实验1中的参数相同。关于RT的结果,实验1表明,当前试验中提示目标的RT大于未切割目标的RT,这是一种基于颜色的非空间IOR。当前试验中的IOR效应大小显示了前一试验中的线索有效性和当前试验中线索-目标模式之间的相互作用。当当前试验是视觉线索和听觉目标时,有效线索试验后当前试验的IOR效应大小大于先前试验中无效线索的IOR效果大小;然而,在当前试验中,当线索是听觉的,而目标是视觉的时,IOR效应大小没有差异。此外,对各试验中目标模态的分析表明,在当前的视觉提示试验中,前一试验中的有效提示(而非无效提示)可以诱导更大的IOR效应大小。与实验1相比,实验2中使用了更长的ITI(4500ms),结果表明,在当前试验中,视觉线索和听觉线索的IOR效应大小存在差异。当前试验中的IOR效应大小不受先前试验的有效性或当前试验是否有听觉线索或视觉线索的影响。这些结果表明,跨模态非空间IOR的试验之间存在相互作用,但这种影响与线索-目标模态有关。试验之间不仅存在线索有效性效应,而且存在目标模态转换效应。增加试验之间的时间间隔可以减少上一次试验对当前试验中IOR效果大小的影响。
{"title":"Effects of trial history on cross-modal non-spatial inhibition of return","authors":"Zhang Ming, Sang Hanbin, Lu Ke, Wang Aijun","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00681","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00681","url":null,"abstract":"Previous laboratory studies have shown that an individual’s response in the current trial can be influenced by a previous trial, and this has been described as an effect of trial history. Existing studies have shown that there is a trial history effect with visual spatial inhibition of return (IOR), and some studies have shown that changes in stimulus modalities also affect reaction times (RTs). The present study used the “prime-neutral cue-target” paradigm to examine the trial history effect in cross-modal, non-spatial IOR and attempted to decrease the trial history effect. In two experiments, we mainly manipulated the cue-target modalities in the current trial (auditory-visual vs. visual-auditory modalities), cue validity in the current trial (cued vs. uncued) and cue validity in the previous trial (cued vs. uncued). Thirty participants were recruited in Experiment 1. The visual prime cue was a red or blue disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory prime cue was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (hong or lan). The visual neutral cue was a green disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory neutral cue was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (lv); The visual target was a red or blue disk with a radius of 2° visual angle, and the auditory target was a verbal sound in Chinese at 75 dB (hong and lan). During the experiment, each trial began with a 400 ms fixation cross in the centre of the monitor, and a 300 ms visual or auditory prime cue was followed by a 200 ms fixation cross. After the 300 ms visual or auditory neutral cue, another fixation cross was presented for 300 ms, and then a 300 ms auditory or visual target was presented. The participants were asked to discriminate the identity of the target(i.e., either a colour disk or vocalization of hongor lan) within 1500 ms. Following a 1500 ms intertrial interval (ITI) with a blank screen, the next trial was initiated. Twenty-nine participants were recruited in Experiment 2, the ITI was 4500 ms, and the other parameters were identical to those in Experiment 1. Regarding the RTs results, Experiment 1 showed that the RTs for cued targets in the current trial were larger than RTs for uncued targets, which was a colour-based non-spatial IOR. The IOR effect size in the current trial showed an interaction between the cue validity in the previous trial and the cue-target modality in the current trial. The IOR effect size on the current trial after a valid cue trial was larger than the IOR effect size with an invalid cue in the previous trial when the current trial was a visual cue and auditory target; however, there was no difference in the IOR effect size when the cue was auditory, and the target was visual in the current trial. Furthermore, the analysis of the target modality across trials revealed that the valid cue, but not the invalid cue, in the previous trial, could induce a larger IOR effect size in the current trial with visual cues. A longer ITI (4500 ms) was used in Exp","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43676584","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the cognitive mechanism of irrelevant speech effect in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements 汉语阅读中不相关言语效应的认知机制探讨:来自眼动的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00729
WU Sanmei, Tian Liangsu, Chen Jiaqiao, Chen Guangyao, Wang Jingxin
the (ISE). key to for namely, the Phonological-Interference Hypothesis and the Semantic-Interference Hypothesis. have investigated the effect in Chinese the mechanisms for the also unclear. Accordingly, with the present research we examined the irrelevant speech effect in Chinese using eye movement measures. simple sentences as stimuli, there was also no significant difference between the silent and intelligible background speech condition. However, in Experiment 2, which used more complex sentences, normal reading was disrupted in the intelligible background speech condition compared to silence, revealing an ISE for these more difficult sentences. Compared with the silent condition, the intelligible background speech produced longer reading times and average fixation duration, more numbers of fixations and regressions, longer regression path reading time and longer total fixation times. Finally, Experiment 3 also produced evidence for an ISE, with longer total reading times, more fixations, and longer regression path reading times and total reading times in the intelligible background speech condition compared with silence. To sum up, the results of the current three experiments suggest that: (1) unintelligible speech does not disrupt normal reading significantly, contrary to the Phonological-Interference Hypothesis; (2) intelligible background speech can disrupt the reading of complex (but not simpler) sentences and also paragraph reading, supporting the Semantic-Interference Hypothesis. Such findings suggest that irrelevant speech might disrupt later stages of lexical processing and semantic integration in reading, and that this effect is modulated by the difficulty of the reading task.
(ISE)。关键在于语音干扰假说和语义干扰假说。已经调查了影响汉语的机制也不清楚。因此,在本研究中,我们使用眼动测量来检验汉语中的无关言语效果。作为刺激的单句,无声和可懂的背景语音条件之间也没有显著差异。然而,在使用更复杂句子的实验2中,与沉默相比,在可理解的背景语音条件下,正常阅读被破坏,这揭示了这些更难的句子的ISE。与无声条件相比,可懂背景语音产生更长的阅读时间和平均注视持续时间,更多的注视和回归次数,更长的回归路径阅读时间和更长的总注视时间。最后,实验3也为ISE提供了证据,与沉默相比,在可理解的背景语音条件下,ISE具有更长的总阅读时间、更多的注视、更长的回归路径阅读时间和总阅读时间。综上所述,目前三个实验的结果表明:(1)与语音干扰假说相反,听不懂的语音不会显著干扰正常阅读;(2) 可理解的背景语音会干扰复杂(但不是简单)句子的阅读,也会干扰段落的阅读,这支持了语义干扰假说。这些发现表明,不相关的言语可能会破坏阅读中词汇处理和语义整合的后期阶段,而这种影响是由阅读任务的难度所调节的。
{"title":"Exploring the cognitive mechanism of irrelevant speech effect in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements","authors":"WU Sanmei, Tian Liangsu, Chen Jiaqiao, Chen Guangyao, Wang Jingxin","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00729","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00729","url":null,"abstract":"the (ISE). key to for namely, the Phonological-Interference Hypothesis and the Semantic-Interference Hypothesis. have investigated the effect in Chinese the mechanisms for the also unclear. Accordingly, with the present research we examined the irrelevant speech effect in Chinese using eye movement measures. simple sentences as stimuli, there was also no significant difference between the silent and intelligible background speech condition. However, in Experiment 2, which used more complex sentences, normal reading was disrupted in the intelligible background speech condition compared to silence, revealing an ISE for these more difficult sentences. Compared with the silent condition, the intelligible background speech produced longer reading times and average fixation duration, more numbers of fixations and regressions, longer regression path reading time and longer total fixation times. Finally, Experiment 3 also produced evidence for an ISE, with longer total reading times, more fixations, and longer regression path reading times and total reading times in the intelligible background speech condition compared with silence. To sum up, the results of the current three experiments suggest that: (1) unintelligible speech does not disrupt normal reading significantly, contrary to the Phonological-Interference Hypothesis; (2) intelligible background speech can disrupt the reading of complex (but not simpler) sentences and also paragraph reading, supporting the Semantic-Interference Hypothesis. Such findings suggest that irrelevant speech might disrupt later stages of lexical processing and semantic integration in reading, and that this effect is modulated by the difficulty of the reading task.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46575648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of group membership on unfairness perception under coexperience conditions 共同经历条件下群体成员对不公平感知的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00773
Sasa Lv, Xin Sun, L. Shen, Yuqing Wu, Shuo Zhao, Fei Wang, Zuojun Wang
{"title":"Effect of group membership on unfairness perception under coexperience conditions","authors":"Sasa Lv, Xin Sun, L. Shen, Yuqing Wu, Shuo Zhao, Fei Wang, Zuojun Wang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00773","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00773","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"773"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49425290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Precision requirement of working memory representations influences attentional guidance 工作记忆表征的精度要求影响注意力引导
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-07-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00694
Xiaowei Che, Huiyun Xu, Kaixuan Wang, Qian Zhang, Shouxin Li
{"title":"Precision requirement of working memory representations influences attentional guidance","authors":"Xiaowei Che, Huiyun Xu, Kaixuan Wang, Qian Zhang, Shouxin Li","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00694","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00694","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"694"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46088540","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
心理学报
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1