首页 > 最新文献

心理学报最新文献

英文 中文
Spillover effects of workplace ostracism on employee family life: The Role of need for affiliation and work-home segmentation preference 工作场所排斥对员工家庭生活的溢出效应:归属需求和工作-家庭分割偏好的作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01146
Xincai Deng, Shan He, Ping Lyu, Xing Zhou, Yijiao Ye, Honglin Meng, Yurou Kong
the indirect influence of workplace ostracism on family undermining and family satisfaction via work stress. This study has several theoretical and practical implications. Based on the conservation of resources theory, this study demonstrates an increase in negative spillover and a decrease in positive spillover; moreover, this study maps the antecedent variables of family undermining and family satisfaction in the workplace. Furthermore, this study shows that work stress is a key transmission mechanism by which workplace ostracism penetrates the work-family interface and causes a spillover effect, which is a response to the a calling for disclosing “the black box” of the spillover effect of workplace ostracism. Finally, by constructing a moderated mediation model and investigating the influence of workplace ostracism on individuals with a unique need for affiliation and work-home segmentation preference, this study specifies the boundary conditions of the spillover effect for workplace ostracism and contributes valid evidence for the conservation of resources theory. In practice, our study can help service enterprises and their managers to understand the spillover effect of workplace ostracism on the family domain more accurately, and to reduce the negative impact of workplace ostracism by taking effective measures, such as building a harmonious, tolerant, and friendly organizational cultural atmosphere.
职场排斥通过工作压力对家庭破坏和家庭满意度的间接影响。本研究具有一定的理论和实践意义。基于资源守恒理论,本研究表明负溢出增加,正溢出减少;此外,本研究还绘制了工作场所家庭破坏和家庭满意度的先行变量图。此外,本研究表明,工作压力是职场排斥渗透到工作-家庭界面并产生溢出效应的关键传导机制,这是对a呼吁披露职场排斥溢出效应“黑匣子”的回应。最后,通过构建一个调节中介模型,考察工作场所排斥对具有独特归属需求和工作-家庭分割偏好的个体的影响,本研究明确了工作场所排斥溢出效应的边界条件,为资源保护理论提供了有效的证据。在实践中,我们的研究可以帮助服务企业及其管理者更准确地理解职场排斥对家庭领域的溢出效应,并通过采取有效措施,如营造和谐、宽容、友好的组织文化氛围,减少职场排斥的负面影响。
{"title":"Spillover effects of workplace ostracism on employee family life: The Role of need for affiliation and work-home segmentation preference","authors":"Xincai Deng, Shan He, Ping Lyu, Xing Zhou, Yijiao Ye, Honglin Meng, Yurou Kong","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01146","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01146","url":null,"abstract":"the indirect influence of workplace ostracism on family undermining and family satisfaction via work stress. This study has several theoretical and practical implications. Based on the conservation of resources theory, this study demonstrates an increase in negative spillover and a decrease in positive spillover; moreover, this study maps the antecedent variables of family undermining and family satisfaction in the workplace. Furthermore, this study shows that work stress is a key transmission mechanism by which workplace ostracism penetrates the work-family interface and causes a spillover effect, which is a response to the a calling for disclosing “the black box” of the spillover effect of workplace ostracism. Finally, by constructing a moderated mediation model and investigating the influence of workplace ostracism on individuals with a unique need for affiliation and work-home segmentation preference, this study specifies the boundary conditions of the spillover effect for workplace ostracism and contributes valid evidence for the conservation of resources theory. In practice, our study can help service enterprises and their managers to understand the spillover effect of workplace ostracism on the family domain more accurately, and to reduce the negative impact of workplace ostracism by taking effective measures, such as building a harmonious, tolerant, and friendly organizational cultural atmosphere.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45613968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The influence of the matching of modality presentation mode and perceptual learning style on the bidialectal switching cost of Cantonese-Mandarin 情态呈现方式与知觉学习风格的匹配对粤语-普通话双方言转换成本的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01059
Qiang Xing, Xiao Wu, Jiawei Wang, Zhonglu Zhang
to study the influence of different stimulus modalities on bidialectal switching cost. Then, Experiment 2 used a four-factor (perception learning style, modality presentation mode, language, task) mixed experiment design to study the impact of bidialectal switching cost from the perspective of the matching between the perceived learning style and the stimulus modality. Experiment 3 used a single factor design to study the effect of learning under the optimal presentation paradigm on the cost of bidialectal switching by setting up a control group. Both Experiment 2 and Experiment 3 screened visual learners and auditory learners through The Learning Channel Preference Checklist. The results showed the following: (1) Skilled Cantonese-Mandarin bidialectal speakers experienced a switching cost under the different stimulus modality conditions, with the participants who were given visual cues demonstrating a lower switching cost than those given auditory cues. It should be noted that these results were quite different from previous studies, which showed a higher switching cost with visual cues compared to auditory cues. (2) There was an interaction between the perceptual learning style and the stimulus modality. Under the matching condition, the bidialectal switching cost was lower than in the non-matching condition, and under the visual-visual condition, the switching cost was the lowest. (3) The switching cost was smaller under matching conditions compared to random presentation conditions; that is, the presentation mode that matched the perception learning style with the channel presentation mode was the best stimulus presentation mode. Based on the above results, it can be concluded that the channel can affects the cost of bidialectal switching—namely, when the modality presentation mode matches the perceptual learning style of a bidialectal speaker—the bidialectal switching cost is smaller. With the popularization of Mandarin, an increasing number of people in China’s dialect areas have become bidialectal speakers. The results of this study will be helpful in providing theoretical support to improve teaching activities in dialect areas.
研究不同刺激方式对双发作转换成本的影响。然后,实验2采用四因素(感知学习风格、模态呈现模式、语言、任务)混合实验设计,从感知学习风格与刺激模态匹配的角度研究了双发作转换成本的影响。实验3采用单因素设计,通过建立对照组,研究最佳呈现范式下的学习对双向言语转换成本的影响。实验2和实验3均通过学习渠道偏好量表对视觉学习者和听觉学习者进行了筛选。研究结果表明:(1)在不同的刺激模态条件下,熟练的广东话普通话双音使用者经历了转换成本,给予视觉提示的参与者比给予听觉提示的参与者表现出更低的转换成本。需要注意的是,这些结果与之前的研究截然不同,之前的研究表明,与听觉线索相比,视觉线索的转换成本更高。(2) 知觉学习方式与刺激方式之间存在交互作用。在匹配条件下,双向切换成本低于非匹配条件,在视觉条件下,切换成本最低。(3) 与随机呈现条件相比,在匹配条件下的切换成本较小;也就是说,感知学习风格与通道呈现模式相匹配的呈现模式是最佳的刺激呈现模式。基于以上结果,可以得出结论,通道会影响双向切换的成本,即当模态呈现模式与双向说话者的感知学习风格相匹配时,双向切换成本较小。随着普通话的普及,中国方言区越来越多的人成为了双方言使用者。研究结果将有助于为改进方言区教学活动提供理论支持。
{"title":"The influence of the matching of modality presentation mode and perceptual learning style on the bidialectal switching cost of Cantonese-Mandarin","authors":"Qiang Xing, Xiao Wu, Jiawei Wang, Zhonglu Zhang","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01059","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01059","url":null,"abstract":"to study the influence of different stimulus modalities on bidialectal switching cost. Then, Experiment 2 used a four-factor (perception learning style, modality presentation mode, language, task) mixed experiment design to study the impact of bidialectal switching cost from the perspective of the matching between the perceived learning style and the stimulus modality. Experiment 3 used a single factor design to study the effect of learning under the optimal presentation paradigm on the cost of bidialectal switching by setting up a control group. Both Experiment 2 and Experiment 3 screened visual learners and auditory learners through The Learning Channel Preference Checklist. The results showed the following: (1) Skilled Cantonese-Mandarin bidialectal speakers experienced a switching cost under the different stimulus modality conditions, with the participants who were given visual cues demonstrating a lower switching cost than those given auditory cues. It should be noted that these results were quite different from previous studies, which showed a higher switching cost with visual cues compared to auditory cues. (2) There was an interaction between the perceptual learning style and the stimulus modality. Under the matching condition, the bidialectal switching cost was lower than in the non-matching condition, and under the visual-visual condition, the switching cost was the lowest. (3) The switching cost was smaller under matching conditions compared to random presentation conditions; that is, the presentation mode that matched the perception learning style with the channel presentation mode was the best stimulus presentation mode. Based on the above results, it can be concluded that the channel can affects the cost of bidialectal switching—namely, when the modality presentation mode matches the perceptual learning style of a bidialectal speaker—the bidialectal switching cost is smaller. With the popularization of Mandarin, an increasing number of people in China’s dialect areas have become bidialectal speakers. The results of this study will be helpful in providing theoretical support to improve teaching activities in dialect areas.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41496977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
How can successful people share their goodness with the world: The psychological mechanism underlying the upper social classes’ redistributive preferences and the role of humility 成功人士如何与世界分享他们的善良:上层社会阶层的再分配偏好和谦逊的作用背后的心理机制
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01161
Jie Bai, Shen‐Long Yang, Bu-Xiao Xu, Yongyu Guo
A large number of studies conducted in Europe and the Americas have explored the negative relationships between social class and redistributive preferences in recent years. However, few studies have addressed the cross-cultural consistency or explored the internal mechanism and intervention strategies of the effects of social class on redistributive preferences. The present study aimed to systematically and deeply explore the relationships between social class and redistributive preferences through three studies in the context of Chinese society. Study 1 explored the direct relationship between social class and redistributive preferences. Based on national data from the Chinese General Social Survey of 2015, 8376 participants from all provinces and autonomous regions of China and indexes of measuring social class and redistributive preferences were obtained. Based on the inequality maintenance model of social class, Study 2 further explored the mediating role of attribution for the rich–poor gap between social class and redistributive preferences. 621 urban and rural residents were investigated by using objective and subjective socioeconomic status (SES) scales, a rich–poor attribution questionnaire, and a redistributive preferences scale. Study 3 was devoted to exploring the intervention effect of humility on the redistributive preferences of the upper social classes. A sample of 103 undergraduates from the upper social class were randomly assigned to humility priming group or control group. The results showed that all social class indexes can strongly and negatively predict redistributive preferences, meaning that, as in Western society, upper social-class Chinese individuals also tend to have lower redistributive preferences than those from lower social classes. In addition, the influences of social class on redistributive preferences could be partly mediated through the attribution for the rich–poor gap. Compared with individuals from a subjectively lower class, upper-class individuals tended to attribute the gap between rich and poor to internal causes. That is to say, they tended to attribute the rich–poor gap to personal factors, such as abilities, efforts, and ambition. This attitude lowered upper-class individuals’ redistributive preferences even further. Finally, a short video was used to prime participants’ feelings of humility. Compared with a control group that watched a neutral video, those upper-class undergraduates who watched life stories of people with humble qualities experienced higher states of emotional humility. Priming a humble state lowered their tendency to attribute the gap between rich and poor to internal causes, and further improved their redistributive preferences to a significant extent. In conclusion, these three studies deeply explore the relationships between social class and redistributive preferences in the context of Chinese society. Combined with other studies performed in Western societies, these results sh
近年来,在欧洲和美洲进行的大量研究探讨了社会阶层与再分配偏好之间的负面关系。然而,很少有研究涉及跨文化的一致性,或探讨社会阶层对再分配偏好影响的内在机制和干预策略。本研究旨在通过三项研究,系统深入地探讨中国社会背景下的社会阶层与再分配偏好之间的关系。研究1探讨了社会阶层和再分配偏好之间的直接关系。基于2015年中国社会综合调查的全国数据,获得了来自中国各省、自治区的8376名参与者以及衡量社会阶层和再分配偏好的指标。基于社会阶层的不平等维持模型,研究2进一步探讨了归因在社会阶层贫富差距和再分配偏好之间的中介作用。采用主观和客观社会经济地位量表、贫富归因问卷和再分配偏好量表对621名城乡居民进行了调查。研究3致力于探索谦逊对上层社会再分配偏好的干预作用。103名来自上流社会阶层的大学生被随机分配到谦逊启动组或对照组。结果表明,所有社会阶层指数都能有力地、负向地预测再分配偏好,这意味着,与西方社会一样,中国上层社会的个人也往往比下层社会的个人具有更低的再分配偏好。此外,社会阶层对再分配偏好的影响可以部分通过贫富差距的归因来调节。与主观上来自下层的个人相比,上层个人倾向于将贫富差距归因于内部原因。也就是说,他们倾向于将贫富差距归因于个人因素,如能力、努力和抱负。这种态度进一步降低了上层阶级个人的再分配偏好。最后,用一段短视频来激发参与者的谦逊感。与观看中性视频的对照组相比,那些观看有谦逊品质的人的生活故事的上流社会本科生经历了更高的情感谦逊状态。建立一个谦逊的国家降低了他们将贫富差距归因于内部原因的倾向,并在很大程度上进一步改善了他们的再分配偏好。总之,这三项研究深入探讨了中国社会背景下社会阶层与再分配偏好之间的关系。结合在西方社会进行的其他研究,这些结果表明,在某种程度上,社会阶层和再分配偏好之间的负面关系是跨文化的。这一机制的探索为社会阶层的不平等维持模型提供了支撑数据和丰富内容。谦逊是一种重要的干预策略,这一发现将进一步深入了解社会再分配。这些结果表明,为了让更多的人能够获得经济发展的好处,社会治理可以通过道德教育、文化发展和培养社区精神来培养个人的谦逊。
{"title":"How can successful people share their goodness with the world: The psychological mechanism underlying the upper social classes’ redistributive preferences and the role of humility","authors":"Jie Bai, Shen‐Long Yang, Bu-Xiao Xu, Yongyu Guo","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01161","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01161","url":null,"abstract":"A large number of studies conducted in Europe and the Americas have explored the negative relationships between social class and redistributive preferences in recent years. However, few studies have addressed the cross-cultural consistency or explored the internal mechanism and intervention strategies of the effects of social class on redistributive preferences. The present study aimed to systematically and deeply explore the relationships between social class and redistributive preferences through three studies in the context of Chinese society. Study 1 explored the direct relationship between social class and redistributive preferences. Based on national data from the Chinese General Social Survey of 2015, 8376 participants from all provinces and autonomous regions of China and indexes of measuring social class and redistributive preferences were obtained. Based on the inequality maintenance model of social class, Study 2 further explored the mediating role of attribution for the rich–poor gap between social class and redistributive preferences. 621 urban and rural residents were investigated by using objective and subjective socioeconomic status (SES) scales, a rich–poor attribution questionnaire, and a redistributive preferences scale. Study 3 was devoted to exploring the intervention effect of humility on the redistributive preferences of the upper social classes. A sample of 103 undergraduates from the upper social class were randomly assigned to humility priming group or control group. The results showed that all social class indexes can strongly and negatively predict redistributive preferences, meaning that, as in Western society, upper social-class Chinese individuals also tend to have lower redistributive preferences than those from lower social classes. In addition, the influences of social class on redistributive preferences could be partly mediated through the attribution for the rich–poor gap. Compared with individuals from a subjectively lower class, upper-class individuals tended to attribute the gap between rich and poor to internal causes. That is to say, they tended to attribute the rich–poor gap to personal factors, such as abilities, efforts, and ambition. This attitude lowered upper-class individuals’ redistributive preferences even further. Finally, a short video was used to prime participants’ feelings of humility. Compared with a control group that watched a neutral video, those upper-class undergraduates who watched life stories of people with humble qualities experienced higher states of emotional humility. Priming a humble state lowered their tendency to attribute the gap between rich and poor to internal causes, and further improved their redistributive preferences to a significant extent. In conclusion, these three studies deeply explore the relationships between social class and redistributive preferences in the context of Chinese society. Combined with other studies performed in Western societies, these results sh","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43789666","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of the angry emoji position on consumers’ perception of the sender’s anger 愤怒的表情符号位置对消费者感知发送者愤怒的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01133
Wui Ruijuan, Chen Jiuqi, L. Yan
Emoji are widely adopted in smartphones, for input methods, and on social networks. As ubiquitous characters, emoji transcend linguistic borders and are gaining worldwide popularity. The purpose of this paper is to examine the effect of the position of the angry emoji in negative online consumer reviews on the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger. The present study first proposed the main location effect of the position of the angry emoji on the consumers’ perception of the sender’s anger. That is, compared with the angry emoji at the end of a sentence, the angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to a stronger perception of anger. Based on visual information processing of the location effect, the current research proposed that the position salience perception and the sentiment-strengthening perception of the angry emoji serially mediated the above main effect. Further, we hypothesized that word review extremity moderated the effect of the position of the angry emoji on the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger. For this paper, we conducted one eye-tracking experiment and three laboratory experiments. In Study 1, we conducted a pretest, which was the eye-tracking experiment. The product used in the pretest was a thermos mug. Study 1 was a 3 (one emoji at the end of a sentence vs. one in the middle of a sentence vs. no emoji) × 2 (feature description: feature one precedes feature two vs. feature two precedes feature one) between-subjects design. The product used in Study 1 was a laptop. Study 2 was a 2 (the position of the angry emoji at the end vs. in the middle of a sentence) × 2 (feature description: feature one precedes feature two vs. feature two precedes feature one) between-subjects design. Study 2 used a gel-ink pen refill as the target product. In Study 2, we measured the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger, the angry emoji sentiment-strengthening perception, and the position salience perception created by the angry emoji. Study 3 was a 2 (the position of the angry emoji at the end vs. in the middle of a sentence) × 2 (word review extremity: moderate vs. extreme) between-subjects design. Study 3 used a coat as the target product. The results of the pretest demonstrated the effectiveness of visual information processing on the location effect. The position of the angry emoji influenced the participants’ attention. The angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to higher fixation counts and longer fixation durations. The results of Study 1 demonstrated the main effect in this paper, which was that an angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to a stronger perception of the sender’s anger than did an angry emoji at the end of a sentence. The results of Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1 and tested the serial-mediating roles of the position salience perception and the sentiment-strengthening perception of the angry emoji. The results of Study 3 replicated the results of Study 2 and tested the moderating ro
表情符号被广泛应用于智能手机、输入法和社交网络。作为无处不在的字符,表情符号超越了语言的界限,在世界范围内越来越受欢迎。本文的目的是研究愤怒表情符号在负面在线消费者评论中的位置对消费者对发送者愤怒的感知的影响。本研究首次提出愤怒表情符号的位置对消费者感知发送者愤怒的主要位置效应。也就是说,与句尾的愤怒表情相比,句中间的愤怒表情会让人更强烈地感受到愤怒。基于位置效应的视觉信息加工,本研究提出愤怒表情符号的位置显著性感知和情绪强化感知依次介导上述主效应。此外,我们假设单词回顾极端调节了愤怒表情符号的位置对消费者对发送者愤怒的感知的影响。在本文中,我们进行了一次眼动追踪实验和三次室内实验。在研究1中,我们进行了一个预测试,即眼球追踪实验。在预试中使用的产品是一个保温杯。研究1是一个3(句尾有一个表情符号vs.句中有一个表情符号vs.没有表情符号)x2(特征描述:特征一在特征二之前vs.特征二在特征一之前)的受试者设计。研究1中使用的产品是一台笔记本电脑。研究2是一个2(愤怒表情符号在句子结尾和中间的位置)x2(特征描述:特征一在特征二之前,特征二在特征一之前)的被试设计。研究2使用凝胶墨水笔芯作为目标产品。在研究2中,我们测量了消费者对发送者愤怒的感知,愤怒表情符号的情绪强化感知,以及愤怒表情符号产生的位置突出感知。研究3是一个2(愤怒表情符号在句子结尾和中间的位置)x2(单词复习极限:中度和极端)的受试者设计。研究3使用涂层作为目标产品。前测结果证明了视觉信息处理对定位效应的影响。愤怒表情符号的位置影响了参与者的注意力。句子中间的愤怒表情符号会导致更高的注视次数和更长的注视时间。研究1的结果证明了本文的主要效果,即句子中间的愤怒表情符号比句子末尾的愤怒表情符号更能让人感受到发送者的愤怒。研究2的结果重复了研究1的结果,并测试了愤怒表情符号的位置显著性感知和情绪强化感知的串行中介作用。研究3的结果重复了研究2的结果,并测试了单词复习极限在愤怒表情符号的位置与消费者对发送者愤怒的感知之间的关系中的调节作用。当我们考虑极端单词复习时,愤怒表情符号的位置对发送者对愤怒的感知的影响不显著;然而,当我们考虑适度的单词回顾时,句子中间的愤怒表情符号显著增强了消费者对发送者愤怒的感知。本研究在几个维度上扩展了现有文献。首先,它补充了营销领域关于表情符号对消费者反应影响的文献。其次,补充了应用情境和区位效应影响的相关文献。第三,本研究为表情符号功能提供了实证证据。第四,本研究对网络消费者评论文献进行了补充。
{"title":"The effect of the angry emoji position on consumers’ perception of the sender’s anger","authors":"Wui Ruijuan, Chen Jiuqi, L. Yan","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01133","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01133","url":null,"abstract":"Emoji are widely adopted in smartphones, for input methods, and on social networks. As ubiquitous characters, emoji transcend linguistic borders and are gaining worldwide popularity. The purpose of this paper is to examine the effect of the position of the angry emoji in negative online consumer reviews on the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger. The present study first proposed the main location effect of the position of the angry emoji on the consumers’ perception of the sender’s anger. That is, compared with the angry emoji at the end of a sentence, the angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to a stronger perception of anger. Based on visual information processing of the location effect, the current research proposed that the position salience perception and the sentiment-strengthening perception of the angry emoji serially mediated the above main effect. Further, we hypothesized that word review extremity moderated the effect of the position of the angry emoji on the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger. For this paper, we conducted one eye-tracking experiment and three laboratory experiments. In Study 1, we conducted a pretest, which was the eye-tracking experiment. The product used in the pretest was a thermos mug. Study 1 was a 3 (one emoji at the end of a sentence vs. one in the middle of a sentence vs. no emoji) × 2 (feature description: feature one precedes feature two vs. feature two precedes feature one) between-subjects design. The product used in Study 1 was a laptop. Study 2 was a 2 (the position of the angry emoji at the end vs. in the middle of a sentence) × 2 (feature description: feature one precedes feature two vs. feature two precedes feature one) between-subjects design. Study 2 used a gel-ink pen refill as the target product. In Study 2, we measured the consumers’ perceptions of the sender’s anger, the angry emoji sentiment-strengthening perception, and the position salience perception created by the angry emoji. Study 3 was a 2 (the position of the angry emoji at the end vs. in the middle of a sentence) × 2 (word review extremity: moderate vs. extreme) between-subjects design. Study 3 used a coat as the target product. The results of the pretest demonstrated the effectiveness of visual information processing on the location effect. The position of the angry emoji influenced the participants’ attention. The angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to higher fixation counts and longer fixation durations. The results of Study 1 demonstrated the main effect in this paper, which was that an angry emoji in the middle of a sentence led to a stronger perception of the sender’s anger than did an angry emoji at the end of a sentence. The results of Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1 and tested the serial-mediating roles of the position salience perception and the sentiment-strengthening perception of the angry emoji. The results of Study 3 replicated the results of Study 2 and tested the moderating ro","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48746641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grades four to six: A cross-lagged study 小学四至六年级班主任协商管理行为与师生关系及学生外化问题行为的交互关系:交叉滞后研究
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01120
Qili Xie, Huizhen Zheng, Guangrong Jiang, Zhihong Ren, Yanfei Fan, Jiahuai Liu, Wen Zhang
problem negatively impact on the and future development of head students’ by adopting management behavior and the - student relationship on students’ externalizing problem behaviors. the transactional model that students’ externalizing problem behaviors may also affect teachers’ negotiation management behavior and the teacher - student relationship. present aims to explore the reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grades four to six in behaviors at Time 1 reduced the head teachers’ negotiation management behavior, decreased the closeness of the teacher - student relationship, and increased the conflicts of the teacher - student relationship at Time 2, which further affected the students’ externalizing problem behaviors, the teacher - student relationship, and the head teacher’s negotiation management behavior at Time 3. These observations deepen the understanding of the complex reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grade four to six in China. Additionally, the findings have important implications for preventing and intervening in students’ externalizing problem behaviors. The results reflect that the head teachers need to be aware of the negative “driver” role of students’ externalizing problem behaviors, and consciously use the positive “driver” role of negotiation management behavior to break the negative cycle driven by students’ externalizing problem behaviors.
通过管理行为和学生关系对学生外化问题行为的影响,问题对学生的未来发展产生负向影响。学生外化问题行为的交易模型也会影响教师的谈判管理行为和师生关系。本研究旨在探讨四年级至六年级小学生在时间1行为中的外化问题行为降低了班主任的谈判管理行为,降低了时间2师生关系的亲密性,增加了时间2师生关系的冲突性。进而影响时间3学生的外化问题行为、师生关系和班主任的谈判管理行为。这些观察加深了对中国小学四至六年级班主任谈判管理行为、师生关系与小学生外化问题行为之间复杂的互惠关系的理解。此外,研究结果对预防和干预学生外化问题行为具有重要意义。研究结果表明,班主任需要意识到学生外化问题行为的消极“驱动”作用,并有意识地利用谈判管理行为的积极“驱动”作用,打破学生外化问题行为驱动的消极循环。
{"title":"The reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grades four to six: A cross-lagged study","authors":"Qili Xie, Huizhen Zheng, Guangrong Jiang, Zhihong Ren, Yanfei Fan, Jiahuai Liu, Wen Zhang","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01120","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01120","url":null,"abstract":"problem negatively impact on the and future development of head students’ by adopting management behavior and the - student relationship on students’ externalizing problem behaviors. the transactional model that students’ externalizing problem behaviors may also affect teachers’ negotiation management behavior and the teacher - student relationship. present aims to explore the reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grades four to six in behaviors at Time 1 reduced the head teachers’ negotiation management behavior, decreased the closeness of the teacher - student relationship, and increased the conflicts of the teacher - student relationship at Time 2, which further affected the students’ externalizing problem behaviors, the teacher - student relationship, and the head teacher’s negotiation management behavior at Time 3. These observations deepen the understanding of the complex reciprocal relationships between head teachers’ negotiation management behavior and teacher-student relationship and primary school students’ externalizing problem behaviors from grade four to six in China. Additionally, the findings have important implications for preventing and intervening in students’ externalizing problem behaviors. The results reflect that the head teachers need to be aware of the negative “driver” role of students’ externalizing problem behaviors, and consciously use the positive “driver” role of negotiation management behavior to break the negative cycle driven by students’ externalizing problem behaviors.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44134861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Effects of sentence structure and type of control verb on thematic role assignment: Evidence from eye movements 句子结构和控制动词类型对主题角色分配的影响:来自眼动的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01071
L. Fang, Linhong Xin, Zhang Man-Man, Bai Xuejun
{"title":"Effects of sentence structure and type of control verb on thematic role assignment: Evidence from eye movements","authors":"L. Fang, Linhong Xin, Zhang Man-Man, Bai Xuejun","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01071","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01071","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48758740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Complex Trial Protocol based on self-referential encoding: Discriminating the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent 基于自指编码的复杂审判方案:区分有罪与有知识的无辜者
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01105
Deng Xiaohong, Li Ting, Xue Chao, J. Rosenfeld, Lu Yang, Wang Ying, Zhan Xiaofei, Yan Gejun, Ouyang Dan
The concealed information test (CIT) is a lie detection method, which can determine if a criminal suspect knows the crime-related information and then infer whether the suspect is guilty. The CIT has high internal validity but it is easy to misjudge an innocent person who knows the crime-related information as a guilty one. Therefore, it is necessary to improve CIT's detection accuracy for the guilty and the knowledgeable innocent persons. The complex trial protocol (CTP) is a modified CIT which can effectively resist countermeasures, but it hasn’t been successfully applied to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent. The present study designed a novel CTP based on self-referential encoding, which measured the early posterior negativity (EPN) and P300. The probe or one of the irrelevant stimuli was randomly presented in the first phase of a trial, and the target (participant’s name) or one of the non-targets (others’ names) was randomly presented in the second phase of a trial. The participants needed to make self-related or self-unrelated responses in two phases when they saw the stimuli. The results revealed that: (1) the area under the curve (AUC) for P300 to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.922, which was significantly higher than 0.5 (the chance level). The AUC for P300 to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent was 0.605, which was not significantly different from 0.5, and the false positive rate of the knowledgeable innocent was 75%. The AUC for P300 to discriminate the knowledgeable innocent from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.859, which was significantly higher than 0.5. (2) The AUC for EPN to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.770, and the AUC for EPN to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent was 0.721. The two AUCs were significantly larger than 0.5. The false positive rate of the knowledgeable innocent was 12.5%. The AUC for EPN to discriminate the knowledgeable innocent from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.516, which was not significantly different from 0.5. In summary, the present study indicated that: (1) P300 can effectively discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent, but can not effectively discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent. P300 is easy to misjudge the knowledgeable innocent as the guilty person. (2) The discrimination of EPN was weaker than that of P300 in discriminating the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent. However, the discrimination of EPN was superior to that of P300 in discriminating the guilty from knowledgeable innocent. EPN is not easy to misjudge a knowledgeable innocent person as a guilty one. (3) When the CTP based on self-referential coding is applied to detect lies, P300 can be used to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent and EPN can be used to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent.
隐藏信息测试(CIT)是一种测谎方法,它可以确定犯罪嫌疑人是否知道与犯罪有关的信息,进而推断犯罪嫌疑人是否有罪。CIT具有较高的内部效度,但很容易将知道犯罪相关信息的无辜者误判为犯罪人。因此,有必要提高CIT对罪犯和有知识的无辜者的检测准确率。复杂审判协议(CTP)是一种改进的CIT,可以有效地抵抗对抗措施,但尚未成功地应用于区分有罪与知情无辜者。本研究设计了一种基于自我参照编码的CTP,测量早期后验负性(EPN)和P300。在试验的第一阶段随机呈现探针或其中一个无关刺激,在试验的第二阶段随机呈现目标(参与者的名字)或非目标之一(其他人的名字)。参与者在看到刺激时需要分两个阶段做出自我相关或自我无关的反应。结果表明:(1)P300区分有罪与无知无辜者的曲线下面积(AUC)为0.922,显著高于0.5(机会水平)。P300区分有罪与知识无辜者的AUC为0.605,与0.5无显著差异,知识无辜者的假阳性率为75%。P300区分知识无辜者和非知识无辜者的AUC为0.859,显著高于0.5。(2) EPN区分有罪与无知无辜者的AUC为0.770,区分有罪与无知无辜者的AUC为0.721。两个auc均显著大于0.5。有知识的无辜者的假阳性率为12.5%。EPN区分知识无辜者和非知识无辜者的AUC为0.516,与0.5差异不显著。综上所述,本研究表明:(1)P300能有效区分有罪与无知无辜者,但不能有效区分有罪与无知无辜者。人们很容易把有知识的无辜者误认为有罪的人。(2) EPN对有罪者和无知无辜者的辨别能力弱于P300。而EPN在区分有罪与有知识的无辜者方面优于P300。EPN不容易把一个有知识的无辜的人误判为有罪的人。(3)将基于自指编码的CTP用于谎言检测时,P300可用于区分有罪与无知无辜者,EPN可用于区分有罪与有知识无辜者。
{"title":"The Complex Trial Protocol based on self-referential encoding: Discriminating the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent","authors":"Deng Xiaohong, Li Ting, Xue Chao, J. Rosenfeld, Lu Yang, Wang Ying, Zhan Xiaofei, Yan Gejun, Ouyang Dan","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01105","url":null,"abstract":"The concealed information test (CIT) is a lie detection method, which can determine if a criminal suspect knows the crime-related information and then infer whether the suspect is guilty. The CIT has high internal validity but it is easy to misjudge an innocent person who knows the crime-related information as a guilty one. Therefore, it is necessary to improve CIT's detection accuracy for the guilty and the knowledgeable innocent persons. The complex trial protocol (CTP) is a modified CIT which can effectively resist countermeasures, but it hasn’t been successfully applied to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent. The present study designed a novel CTP based on self-referential encoding, which measured the early posterior negativity (EPN) and P300. The probe or one of the irrelevant stimuli was randomly presented in the first phase of a trial, and the target (participant’s name) or one of the non-targets (others’ names) was randomly presented in the second phase of a trial. The participants needed to make self-related or self-unrelated responses in two phases when they saw the stimuli. The results revealed that: (1) the area under the curve (AUC) for P300 to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.922, which was significantly higher than 0.5 (the chance level). The AUC for P300 to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent was 0.605, which was not significantly different from 0.5, and the false positive rate of the knowledgeable innocent was 75%. The AUC for P300 to discriminate the knowledgeable innocent from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.859, which was significantly higher than 0.5. (2) The AUC for EPN to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.770, and the AUC for EPN to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent was 0.721. The two AUCs were significantly larger than 0.5. The false positive rate of the knowledgeable innocent was 12.5%. The AUC for EPN to discriminate the knowledgeable innocent from the unknowledgeable innocent was 0.516, which was not significantly different from 0.5. In summary, the present study indicated that: (1) P300 can effectively discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent, but can not effectively discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent. P300 is easy to misjudge the knowledgeable innocent as the guilty person. (2) The discrimination of EPN was weaker than that of P300 in discriminating the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent. However, the discrimination of EPN was superior to that of P300 in discriminating the guilty from knowledgeable innocent. EPN is not easy to misjudge a knowledgeable innocent person as a guilty one. (3) When the CTP based on self-referential coding is applied to detect lies, P300 can be used to discriminate the guilty from the unknowledgeable innocent and EPN can be used to discriminate the guilty from the knowledgeable innocent.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46455084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The double-edged-sword effect of empathy: The secondary traumatic stress and vicarious posttraumatic growth of psychological hotline counselors during the outbreak of COVID-19 共情的双刃剑效应:新冠肺炎疫情期间心理热线咨询师的继发性创伤应激与替代性创伤后成长
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-09-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00992
Lizu Lai, Zhihong Ren, Yifei Yan, Geng-feng Niu, Chunxiao Zhao, Mei Luo, Lin Zhang
The novel Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak increases public mental stress and pandemic-related trauma. Timely and comprehensive online mental health services have been deployed across China. Hotline counselors can be exposed to the stress and trauma experienced by callers indirectly. They may experience secondary traumatic stress (STS) as a result of their empathetic engagement with traumatized clients, and they may also experience vicarious posttraumatic growth (VPTG). The present study examined negative and positive change in hotline counselors concurrently and explored the mechanisms of both positive and negative outcomes. Participants were 776 counselors (81% female, average age = 42.57 ± 7.90, average years of experience = 12.44 ± 5.92) recruited from MOE-CCNU Mental Health Service Platform, the biggest official telephone-based and online psychological support platform in China during COVID-19 period. Empathy and VPTG were measured by revised Chinese versions of self-report questionnaires. STS was assessed by the corresponding subscale of the Professional Quality of Life Scale. The Chinese Meaning in Life Questionnaire was used to assess participants’ search for life meaning. Mindfulness was evaluated by Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Latent variable structural equation modeling was applied. After controlling age, trauma cases and total cases, counselors’ empathy was positively associated with STS and VPTG. Results revealed that empathy was positively associated with STS and VPTG. Mindfulness mediated this association between empathy and STS. Search for meaning mediated the relationship between empathy and VPTG. Additionally, the association between empathy and VPTG was also mediated through other four significant mediating pathways: (a) secondary trauma stress, (b) secondary traumatic stress and search for meaning, (c) mindfulness and search for meaning, and (d) mindfulness and secondary trauma stress and search for meaning. Our findings support that empathy lead to both positive and negative outcomes among hotline counselors during COVID-19 period in China, highlighting dialectical insights into trauma workers’ experiences. When counselors engage in others’ traumatic experience, their vicarious negative emotional experience may be a pathway to growth, and search for life meaning is an important factor in that growth. © 2021, Science Press. All rights reserved.
2019年新型冠状病毒疾病(新冠肺炎)的爆发增加了公众的心理压力和与流行病相关的创伤。及时、全面的在线心理健康服务已在中国各地展开。热线咨询师可能会间接接触到来电者所经历的压力和创伤。他们可能会因与受创伤客户的移情接触而经历继发性创伤压力(STS),也可能经历替代性创伤后成长(VPTG)。本研究同时考察了热线咨询师的消极和积极变化,并探讨了积极和消极结果的机制。参与者包括776名辅导员(81%为女性,平均年龄=42.57±7.90,平均工作年限=12.44±5.92),这些辅导员来自新冠肺炎期间中国最大的官方电话和在线心理支持平台——教育部-全国大学生心理健康服务平台。采用中文版自我报告问卷对移情和VPTG进行测量。STS通过相应的专业生活质量量表的分量表进行评估。采用“中国人生意义调查表”来评估参与者对人生意义的追求。正念通过正念注意意识量表进行评估。采用潜变结构方程建模。在控制了年龄、创伤病例和总病例后,辅导员的移情与STS和VPTG呈正相关。结果显示,移情与STS和VPTG呈正相关。正念介导了移情和STS之间的这种联系。寻找意义是移情与VPTG关系的中介。此外,移情和VPTG之间的联系也通过其他四种重要的中介途径进行中介:(a)继发性创伤压力,(b)继发性创伤性压力和寻找意义,(c)正念和寻找意义以及(d)正念与继发性创伤应力和寻找意义。我们的研究结果支持,在中国新冠肺炎期间,同情心会导致热线咨询师产生积极和消极的结果,突出了对创伤工作者经历的辩证见解。当咨询师参与他人的创伤经历时,他们替代性的负面情绪体验可能是成长的途径,而寻找生命意义是成长的重要因素。©2021,科学出版社。保留所有权利。
{"title":"The double-edged-sword effect of empathy: The secondary traumatic stress and vicarious posttraumatic growth of psychological hotline counselors during the outbreak of COVID-19","authors":"Lizu Lai, Zhihong Ren, Yifei Yan, Geng-feng Niu, Chunxiao Zhao, Mei Luo, Lin Zhang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00992","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00992","url":null,"abstract":"The novel Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak increases public mental stress and pandemic-related trauma. Timely and comprehensive online mental health services have been deployed across China. Hotline counselors can be exposed to the stress and trauma experienced by callers indirectly. They may experience secondary traumatic stress (STS) as a result of their empathetic engagement with traumatized clients, and they may also experience vicarious posttraumatic growth (VPTG). The present study examined negative and positive change in hotline counselors concurrently and explored the mechanisms of both positive and negative outcomes. Participants were 776 counselors (81% female, average age = 42.57 ± 7.90, average years of experience = 12.44 ± 5.92) recruited from MOE-CCNU Mental Health Service Platform, the biggest official telephone-based and online psychological support platform in China during COVID-19 period. Empathy and VPTG were measured by revised Chinese versions of self-report questionnaires. STS was assessed by the corresponding subscale of the Professional Quality of Life Scale. The Chinese Meaning in Life Questionnaire was used to assess participants’ search for life meaning. Mindfulness was evaluated by Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Latent variable structural equation modeling was applied. After controlling age, trauma cases and total cases, counselors’ empathy was positively associated with STS and VPTG. Results revealed that empathy was positively associated with STS and VPTG. Mindfulness mediated this association between empathy and STS. Search for meaning mediated the relationship between empathy and VPTG. Additionally, the association between empathy and VPTG was also mediated through other four significant mediating pathways: (a) secondary trauma stress, (b) secondary traumatic stress and search for meaning, (c) mindfulness and search for meaning, and (d) mindfulness and secondary trauma stress and search for meaning. Our findings support that empathy lead to both positive and negative outcomes among hotline counselors during COVID-19 period in China, highlighting dialectical insights into trauma workers’ experiences. When counselors engage in others’ traumatic experience, their vicarious negative emotional experience may be a pathway to growth, and search for life meaning is an important factor in that growth. © 2021, Science Press. All rights reserved.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44938911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Two new termination rules for multidimensional computerized classification testing 多维计算机分类测试的两个新的终止规则
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-09-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01044
Ren He, Chen Ping
{"title":"Two new termination rules for multidimensional computerized classification testing","authors":"Ren He, Chen Ping","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01044","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01044","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42558476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language evolution and ethnic group/dialect name affecting ethnic information processing: Three major Chinese dialects in Guangdong 语言演化与族群/方言名称对族群信息加工的影响:广东三大汉语方言
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-09-25 DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00944
Limei Wu, Jijia Zhang, L. Meng, Xiaolin Zhang, Nanxin Huang, Jinqiao Zhang
There are three major dialects in Guangdong: Cantonese, Chaoshan, and Hakka. These dialects mainly formed during the southward migration of the Han people in the Central Plains, but they are different in language source, evolution history, and name. From the perspective of language itself, the Hakka dialect is the smallest variation of Mandarin, Cantonese medium, and Chaoshan the largest. In terms of language-variation completion time, the Hakka dialect occurred most recently, followed by Cantonese, with Chaoshan occurring the earliest. Regarding the dialect names, a morpheme of “Ke” exists in the name Hakka, which always reminds its speakers that their ancestors are from the Central Plains, while Cantonese and Chaoshan are named after each locality. An interesting question has been whether these differences among the three dialects affect the speaker’s information processing of the Central Plains group. processing of the Central Plains group, which resulted in a better memory effect on the Central Plains group than that on the unrelated group. The results of experiment 2 showed that the participants had the longest reaction time under the condition that their own ethnic group name was activated, but only the Hakka participants responded more slowly to the Henan ethnic group than to the unrelated ethnic group. The results of both experiments indicated that all the three dialect groups had processing advantages regarding the information of their own groups that manifested in the obvious referential effect of their own groups and the attention bias of their own information. Moreover, the Hakka participants’ cognition regarding the Central Plains group represented by “Henan people” is significantly different from that of Cantonese and Chaoshan dialect speakers. The research results suggested that language evolution affected ethnic information processing. The identity of ethnic groups with the same ancestry could be enhanced by keeping the characteristics of ancestral language completely and strengthening the relationship between dialect and ancestral language. The results have important implications for the construction of Chinese Community Consciousness.
广东有三大方言:广东话、潮汕话和客家话。这些方言主要形成于中原汉族南迁时期,但在语言来源、演化史、名称等方面各不相同。从语言本身来看,客家话是普通话变体最小的,粤语变体居中,潮汕方言变体最大。从语言变异完成时间来看,客家方言出现的时间最晚,其次是广东话,潮汕方言出现的时间最早。在方言名称方面,客家名称中有一个“客”的语素,它总是提醒说它的人他们的祖先来自中原,而粤语和潮汕则以每个地方命名。一个有趣的问题是,三种方言之间的差异是否会影响中原群体的说话者的信息加工。中原组的记忆效应优于非亲属组。实验2的结果表明,在激活自己民族名称时,被试的反应时间最长,但只有客家被试对河南民族的反应慢于对非亲属民族的反应。两个实验的结果都表明,三个方言群体对本群体的信息都具有加工优势,表现为本群体具有明显的参照效应和本群体信息的注意偏倚。客家被试对以“河南人”为代表的中原族群的认知与广东话、潮汕话被试的认知存在显著差异。研究结果表明,语言进化影响民族信息加工。完整地保持祖先语言的特征,加强方言与祖先语言的关系,可以增强同祖族群的认同。研究结果对华人社区意识的建构具有重要的启示意义。
{"title":"Language evolution and ethnic group/dialect name affecting ethnic information processing: Three major Chinese dialects in Guangdong","authors":"Limei Wu, Jijia Zhang, L. Meng, Xiaolin Zhang, Nanxin Huang, Jinqiao Zhang","doi":"10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00944","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00944","url":null,"abstract":"There are three major dialects in Guangdong: Cantonese, Chaoshan, and Hakka. These dialects mainly formed during the southward migration of the Han people in the Central Plains, but they are different in language source, evolution history, and name. From the perspective of language itself, the Hakka dialect is the smallest variation of Mandarin, Cantonese medium, and Chaoshan the largest. In terms of language-variation completion time, the Hakka dialect occurred most recently, followed by Cantonese, with Chaoshan occurring the earliest. Regarding the dialect names, a morpheme of “Ke” exists in the name Hakka, which always reminds its speakers that their ancestors are from the Central Plains, while Cantonese and Chaoshan are named after each locality. An interesting question has been whether these differences among the three dialects affect the speaker’s information processing of the Central Plains group. processing of the Central Plains group, which resulted in a better memory effect on the Central Plains group than that on the unrelated group. The results of experiment 2 showed that the participants had the longest reaction time under the condition that their own ethnic group name was activated, but only the Hakka participants responded more slowly to the Henan ethnic group than to the unrelated ethnic group. The results of both experiments indicated that all the three dialect groups had processing advantages regarding the information of their own groups that manifested in the obvious referential effect of their own groups and the attention bias of their own information. Moreover, the Hakka participants’ cognition regarding the Central Plains group represented by “Henan people” is significantly different from that of Cantonese and Chaoshan dialect speakers. The research results suggested that language evolution affected ethnic information processing. The identity of ethnic groups with the same ancestry could be enhanced by keeping the characteristics of ancestral language completely and strengthening the relationship between dialect and ancestral language. The results have important implications for the construction of Chinese Community Consciousness.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45352124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
心理学报
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1