首页 > 最新文献

心理学报最新文献

英文 中文
Processing priority for avatar reference in online games: Evidence from behavioral and ERPs studies 网络游戏中角色参考的处理优先权:来自行为和erp研究的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00639
Min Cao, Heping Xie, Lijun Sun, Dongjing Zhang, Fanchang Kong, Zongkui Zhou
{"title":"Processing priority for avatar reference in online games: Evidence from behavioral and ERPs studies","authors":"Min Cao, Heping Xie, Lijun Sun, Dongjing Zhang, Fanchang Kong, Zongkui Zhou","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00639","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00639","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"639"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45711840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transitional probabilities and expectation for word length impact verbal statistical learning 过渡概率和词长期望影响语言统计学习
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00565
Wenbo Yu, Lu Wang, X. Qu, Tianlin Wang, Jingjing Zhang, Dandan Liang
{"title":"Transitional probabilities and expectation for word length impact verbal statistical learning","authors":"Wenbo Yu, Lu Wang, X. Qu, Tianlin Wang, Jingjing Zhang, Dandan Liang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00565","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00565","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"565"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48630743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Influence of an individual’s own gains and losses on the evaluation of friends’ gambling results: Evidence from ERPs 个人得失对朋友赌博结果评价的影响:来自ERP的证据
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00651
Tong Yue, Xiting Huang, Caizhen Yue, Liming Xue, Anguo Fu
on the evaluation of friends’ gains and losses would differ under different modes of self-construction. The results indicated that in the interdependent self-construction initiation group, the electrophysiological response mode that results from watching friends’ gambling was consistent with the result obtained in Experiment 1. However, after initiation of the independent self-construction of the subjects, the evaluation mode of the gambling results of a friend differed from that of the interdependent self-construction initiation group. In terms of the FRN index, regardless of one’s own gains and losses, the differences in FRN for friends’ gains and losses were no longer significant. In terms of the P300 index, the P300 discrepancy resulting from watching the gains and losses of a friend after suffering own losses was not significant. However, after having gained benefits, a higher P300 amplitude was induced by watching friends losing money in gambling than when watching them gain money. In summary, this study shows that: (1) the evaluation model for one’s friends’ gains and losses differs depending on the own experienced gains and losses. Under favorable conditions, an individual is more inclined to show indifference to his friends’ gains and losses, which may be because the individual employs stronger egocentric tendency in such a case. (2) The independent self-construction initiation group shows more indifference and competition when observing the gains and losses of a friend than in case of the interdependent self-construction group.
在不同的自我建构模式下,对朋友得失的评价会有所不同。结果表明,在相互依存的自我建构启动组中,观看朋友赌博产生的电生理反应模式与实验1的结果一致。然而,在受试者开始独立的自我建构后,朋友对赌博结果的评价模式与相互依存的自我建构启动组不同。在FRN指数方面,无论自己的得失,朋友得失的FRN差异不再显著。在P300指数方面,观察朋友在遭受自身损失后的得失所产生的P300差异并不显著。然而,在获得利益后,观看朋友在赌博中输钱会比观看他们赚钱时诱发更高的P300振幅。总之,本研究表明:(1)对朋友得失的评价模型因自己经历的得失而不同。在有利的条件下,个人更倾向于对朋友的得失漠不关心,这可能是因为在这种情况下,个人表现出更强的自我中心倾向。(2) 独立的自我构建启动组在观察朋友的得失时比相互依赖的自我构建组表现出更多的冷漠和竞争。
{"title":"Influence of an individual’s own gains and losses on the evaluation of friends’ gambling results: Evidence from ERPs","authors":"Tong Yue, Xiting Huang, Caizhen Yue, Liming Xue, Anguo Fu","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00651","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00651","url":null,"abstract":"on the evaluation of friends’ gains and losses would differ under different modes of self-construction. The results indicated that in the interdependent self-construction initiation group, the electrophysiological response mode that results from watching friends’ gambling was consistent with the result obtained in Experiment 1. However, after initiation of the independent self-construction of the subjects, the evaluation mode of the gambling results of a friend differed from that of the interdependent self-construction initiation group. In terms of the FRN index, regardless of one’s own gains and losses, the differences in FRN for friends’ gains and losses were no longer significant. In terms of the P300 index, the P300 discrepancy resulting from watching the gains and losses of a friend after suffering own losses was not significant. However, after having gained benefits, a higher P300 amplitude was induced by watching friends losing money in gambling than when watching them gain money. In summary, this study shows that: (1) the evaluation model for one’s friends’ gains and losses differs depending on the own experienced gains and losses. Under favorable conditions, an individual is more inclined to show indifference to his friends’ gains and losses, which may be because the individual employs stronger egocentric tendency in such a case. (2) The independent self-construction initiation group shows more indifference and competition when observing the gains and losses of a friend than in case of the interdependent self-construction group.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47442869","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The influence of feature-based statistical regularity of singletons on the attentional suppression effect 基于特征的单子统计规律对注意抑制效果的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-06-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00555
Zhang Fan, Wang Aijun, Zhang Ming
{"title":"The influence of feature-based statistical regularity of singletons on the attentional suppression effect","authors":"Zhang Fan, Wang Aijun, Zhang Ming","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00555","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00555","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"555"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42080186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The establishment of Chinese Emotion Regulation Word System (CERWS) and its pilot test 汉语情绪调节词系统(CERWS)的建立及其中试
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-05-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00445
Yuan Jiajin, Zhang Yicheng, Chen Shengdong, Luo Li, RU Yishan
{"title":"The establishment of Chinese Emotion Regulation Word System (CERWS) and its pilot test","authors":"Yuan Jiajin, Zhang Yicheng, Chen Shengdong, Luo Li, RU Yishan","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00445","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00445","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"445"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48270635","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The influence of anger on delay discounting: The mediating role of certainty and control 愤怒对延迟折扣的影响:确定性和控制性的中介作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-05-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456
Xiyan Song, Yahua Cheng, Zhouxiutian Xie, Nanyan Gong, Lei Liu
Simultaneously, we explored whether positive emotions associated with certainty-control produced increases in delay gratification. The focus was on pleasure as a positive, certainty-control-associated emotion. In Experiment 1, the results showed that compared with fear and neutral participants, angry participants were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. In Experiment 2a, the results showed that compared with fear and neural participants, angry participants were more likely to experience certainty-control feelings. Then, in Experiment 2b, the results showed that compared with low certainty-control participants, high certainty-control participants were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. In Experiment 3, the results showed that compared with fear and neutral participants, angry and pleasant participants experienced more certainty-control feelings and were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. Furthermore, the mediation analysis showed that certainty-control feelings played a complete mediating role in the effect of anger and pleasure on delay discounting. Converging evidence from the three experiments indicated that incidental anger can influence delay discounting. Compared with fear and neutral feelings, those experiencing anger were more likely to choose larger and delayed rewards (Experiment 1). Importantly, these two experiments provide direct process evidence by showing that the certainty and control appraisal tendencies triggered by anger may underlie its delay gratification-enhancing effects (Experiments 2 and Experiment 3). Furthermore, experiencing certainty-control-associated emotions (i.e., anger and pleasure), regardless of valence, increased to the likelihood that individuals would choose larger and delayed rewards (Experiment 3). The current research supports the hypotheses that anger increases delay gratification and that certainty and control appraisal tendencies drive this effect. These findings have important implications for understanding the mechanism underlying the effect of specific negative emotions on intertemporal choice.
同时,我们探讨了与确定性控制相关的积极情绪是否会增加延迟满足。研究的重点是快乐是一种积极的、与确定性控制相关的情绪。在实验1中,结果显示,与恐惧和中立的参与者相比,愤怒的参与者更倾向于选择大的和延迟的奖励。在实验2a中,结果显示,与恐惧和神经参与者相比,愤怒的参与者更有可能体验到确定性控制感。实验2b的结果表明,与低确定性控制的被试相比,高确定性控制的被试更倾向于选择大奖励和延迟奖励。在实验3中,结果表明,与恐惧和中立的参与者相比,愤怒和愉快的参与者经历了更多的确定性控制感,更有可能选择大的和延迟的奖励。此外,中介分析表明,确定性控制情绪在愤怒和快乐对延迟折扣的影响中起完全的中介作用。三个实验的证据表明,偶然的愤怒会影响延迟折扣。与恐惧情绪和中性情绪相比,愤怒情绪更倾向于选择更大的、延迟的奖励(实验1)。重要的是,这两个实验提供了直接的过程证据,表明愤怒触发的确定性和控制性评价倾向可能是其延迟满足增强效应的基础(实验2和实验3)。此外,体验确定性控制相关情绪(即愤怒和快乐),无论其效价如何,(实验3)。目前的研究支持愤怒会增加延迟满足的假设,确定性和控制评价倾向驱动了这种效应。这些发现对于理解特定负面情绪对跨期选择的影响机制具有重要意义。
{"title":"The influence of anger on delay discounting: The mediating role of certainty and control","authors":"Xiyan Song, Yahua Cheng, Zhouxiutian Xie, Nanyan Gong, Lei Liu","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456","url":null,"abstract":"Simultaneously, we explored whether positive emotions associated with certainty-control produced increases in delay gratification. The focus was on pleasure as a positive, certainty-control-associated emotion. In Experiment 1, the results showed that compared with fear and neutral participants, angry participants were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. In Experiment 2a, the results showed that compared with fear and neural participants, angry participants were more likely to experience certainty-control feelings. Then, in Experiment 2b, the results showed that compared with low certainty-control participants, high certainty-control participants were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. In Experiment 3, the results showed that compared with fear and neutral participants, angry and pleasant participants experienced more certainty-control feelings and were more likely to choose large and delayed rewards. Furthermore, the mediation analysis showed that certainty-control feelings played a complete mediating role in the effect of anger and pleasure on delay discounting. Converging evidence from the three experiments indicated that incidental anger can influence delay discounting. Compared with fear and neutral feelings, those experiencing anger were more likely to choose larger and delayed rewards (Experiment 1). Importantly, these two experiments provide direct process evidence by showing that the certainty and control appraisal tendencies triggered by anger may underlie its delay gratification-enhancing effects (Experiments 2 and Experiment 3). Furthermore, experiencing certainty-control-associated emotions (i.e., anger and pleasure), regardless of valence, increased to the likelihood that individuals would choose larger and delayed rewards (Experiment 3). The current research supports the hypotheses that anger increases delay gratification and that certainty and control appraisal tendencies drive this effect. These findings have important implications for understanding the mechanism underlying the effect of specific negative emotions on intertemporal choice.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45135128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Does conformity lead to gains? The effect of workplace ostracism on performance evaluation from a self-presentational view 从众会带来收益吗?自我表征视角下的职场排斥对绩效评估的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-04-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00400
Ping Jiang, Lihua Zhang
Workplace ostracism is an increasingly widespread and serious social problem nowadays. Many studies have shown that being rejected in the workplace can significantly increase employees’ psychological stress, lead to health problems, and even cause deviant workplace behaviors, thereby ultimately damaging the normal operation of an organization and lowering organizational performance. However, existing research regarding the impact of workplace exclusion on individual performance has reached inconsistent conclusions; thus, this field involves hidden mechanisms that need to be further explored. To address this theoretical gap, we drew upon the self-presentational perspective and hypothesized that employees’ collectivism values strengthen the likelihood that employees will create facades of conformity when suffering from workplace ostracism. We further assumed that whether or not a supervisor is a collectivist determines how the employees’ creation of facades of conformity affect subsequent performance ratings. We tested these hypotheses in an experimental study and a field sample of supervisor-employee dyads. For the experimental study, we recruited 142 full-time Chinese workers in different industries as participants through the authors’ alumni networks. The respondents were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions: the ostracism condition ( N = 70) versus the inclusion condition ( N = 72). Participants first completed a measure of collectivism values and reported their demographics. Then, they were asked to read a scenario from a computer screen, one which depicted a situation in which the participant was ostracized (or included) by team members in a hypothetical organization. After reading the scenario, participants finished the manipulation check of workplace ostracism and indicated the extent to which they would be likely to engage in creating facades of conformity. For the field study, we collected 254 dyadic data from a major air company in China at three time-spots. At Time 1, employees assessed perceived workplace ostracism and their collectivism values, as well as provided their demographic information. At Time 2 (one months after Time 1), employees rated their self-esteem level and the extent to which they create facades of conformity, conduct self-leadership, and implement impression management strategies. At Time 3 (one months after Time 2), supervisors provided a performance evaluation of the focal employee and their own collectivism values. We applied confirmatory factor analysis, regression analysis, and bootstrap methods via SPSS 22.0 and Mplus7.4 software to analyze the data. Empirical results supported our arguments that workplace ostracism was positively related to facades of conformity. The relationship between workplace ostracism and facades of conformity was stronger when employees had high levels of collectivism values. When supervisors themselves also hold high levels of collectivism values, the employees’ fac
职场排斥是一个日益普遍和严重的社会问题。许多研究表明,在工作场所被拒绝会显著增加员工的心理压力,导致健康问题,甚至导致越轨的工作行为,从而最终损害组织的正常运作,降低组织绩效。然而,关于工作场所排斥对个人表现的影响的现有研究得出了不一致的结论;因此,这一领域涉及到需要进一步探索的隐藏机制。为了解决这一理论空白,我们借鉴了自我表象的观点,并假设员工的集体主义价值观增强了员工在遭受职场排斥时创造顺从外表的可能性。我们进一步假设,主管是否是集体主义者,决定了员工对一致性外表的创造如何影响后续的绩效评级。我们在一项实验研究和一个主管-员工二人组的现场样本中检验了这些假设。在实验研究中,我们通过作者的校友网络招募了142名来自不同行业的全职中国工人作为参与者。受访者被随机分配到两种实验条件中的一种:排斥条件(N=70)和包容条件(N=72)。参与者首先完成了集体主义价值观的测量,并报告了他们的人口统计数据。然后,他们被要求从电脑屏幕上阅读一个场景,其中描述了参与者被假设组织中的团队成员排斥(或包括在内)的情况。在阅读该场景后,参与者完成了对工作场所排斥的操纵检查,并指出了他们可能在多大程度上参与创造顺从的外表。在实地研究中,我们从中国一家主要航空公司收集了三个时间点的254个二元数据。在时间1,员工评估了工作场所的排斥感和集体主义价值观,并提供了他们的人口统计信息。在时间2(时间1后一个月),员工评估了他们的自尊水平,以及他们在多大程度上创造了顺从的外表,进行了自我领导,并实施了印象管理策略。在时间3(时间2后一个月),主管对重点员工及其集体主义价值观进行了绩效评估。我们采用验证性因素分析、回归分析和bootstrap方法,通过SPSS 22.0和Mplus7.4软件对数据进行分析。实证结果支持了我们的论点,即工作场所的排斥与从众外表呈正相关。当员工具有高度的集体主义价值观时,工作场所的排斥与顺从外表之间的关系更为密切。当主管本身也持有高度的集体主义价值观时,员工的一致性外表与主管对工作表现的评价呈正相关。此外,工作场所的排斥通过员工的一致性外表对主管对员工绩效的评估具有条件的、积极的间接影响,因此只有当员工和主管的集体主义价值观都很高时,这种间接影响才是积极的。通过这些发现,我们对文献和管理实践做出了一些贡献。首先,我们提供了一种新的解释机制,通过考察创造一致性外表的中介作用,从自我表象的角度理解工作场所排斥与员工绩效之间的复杂关系。其次,我们对员工集体主义价值观的调节作用的研究结果为对负面工作体验(如工作排斥)的不同反应提供了解释。第三,通过探索领导者集体主义价值观的偶然作用,我们的研究是最早考虑旁观者在排斥过程中的作用的尝试之一。最后,我们通过研究新的标准丰富了一致性的研究领域,从而回应了调查一致性外表的前因和后果的呼吁。
{"title":"Does conformity lead to gains? The effect of workplace ostracism on performance evaluation from a self-presentational view","authors":"Ping Jiang, Lihua Zhang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00400","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00400","url":null,"abstract":"Workplace ostracism is an increasingly widespread and serious social problem nowadays. Many studies have shown that being rejected in the workplace can significantly increase employees’ psychological stress, lead to health problems, and even cause deviant workplace behaviors, thereby ultimately damaging the normal operation of an organization and lowering organizational performance. However, existing research regarding the impact of workplace exclusion on individual performance has reached inconsistent conclusions; thus, this field involves hidden mechanisms that need to be further explored. To address this theoretical gap, we drew upon the self-presentational perspective and hypothesized that employees’ collectivism values strengthen the likelihood that employees will create facades of conformity when suffering from workplace ostracism. We further assumed that whether or not a supervisor is a collectivist determines how the employees’ creation of facades of conformity affect subsequent performance ratings. We tested these hypotheses in an experimental study and a field sample of supervisor-employee dyads. For the experimental study, we recruited 142 full-time Chinese workers in different industries as participants through the authors’ alumni networks. The respondents were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions: the ostracism condition ( N = 70) versus the inclusion condition ( N = 72). Participants first completed a measure of collectivism values and reported their demographics. Then, they were asked to read a scenario from a computer screen, one which depicted a situation in which the participant was ostracized (or included) by team members in a hypothetical organization. After reading the scenario, participants finished the manipulation check of workplace ostracism and indicated the extent to which they would be likely to engage in creating facades of conformity. For the field study, we collected 254 dyadic data from a major air company in China at three time-spots. At Time 1, employees assessed perceived workplace ostracism and their collectivism values, as well as provided their demographic information. At Time 2 (one months after Time 1), employees rated their self-esteem level and the extent to which they create facades of conformity, conduct self-leadership, and implement impression management strategies. At Time 3 (one months after Time 2), supervisors provided a performance evaluation of the focal employee and their own collectivism values. We applied confirmatory factor analysis, regression analysis, and bootstrap methods via SPSS 22.0 and Mplus7.4 software to analyze the data. Empirical results supported our arguments that workplace ostracism was positively related to facades of conformity. The relationship between workplace ostracism and facades of conformity was stronger when employees had high levels of collectivism values. When supervisors themselves also hold high levels of collectivism values, the employees’ fac","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48387709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Temporal dynamics of eye movements and attentional modulation in perceptual judgments of structure-from-motion (SFM) 动-结构知觉判断的眼动时间动态与注意调节
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-04-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00337
Jinhong Ding, Ya-Min Wang, Jiang Yang
How attention plays a role in resolving ambiguous perceptual judgments is one of the age-old scientific questions. Understanding the processes of perceptual grouping, switching, processing speed, and awareness is a key step towards solving significant problems in applications such as computer vision and automatic driving involving three-dimensional space. The rotating three-dimensional (3D) structure from motion (SFM) is a well-known bistable ambiguous stimulus. Thus far, it is still an open question how attention, eye movements, and depth cues modulate perceptual judgments of rotating directions of a 3D SFM. As early as 1925, motion perception resulted from eye tracking signals was proposed by Helmholtz (Cavanagh, 1992). Pomerantz (1970) claimed that eye movement plays an important role in the occurrence of kinesthetic perception. Furthermore, neuroscience studies has supported a common neural basis for eye movement and attention transfer (Grosbras, Laird & Paus, 2005). The current study aimed to investigate the characteristics and the time course of eye movements during SFM perception by controlling exogenous visual cues and ascertain the effect of attention on SFM perception. Using advanced eyemovements analysis, we investigated how attention under both unambiguous and ambiguous depth cues modulate perceptual judgments of rotation directions in deepth. Twenty-two experiment. Their task was to indicate the rotation directions of 3D SFM by pressing the left (for clockwise CW percept from top view) or the right key of a mouse (for CCW percept) with their left or right thumb. A computer simulated structure-from-motion (a 3D rotating sphere) was created via 30 coherently moving dots with 0.2° diameter each along with an elliptical trajectory of different radii at a mean velocity of 5°/s. The luminant dots were randomly distributed in a spherical area extended 5°×5°. Under unambiguous depth conducted using methods from Bonneh, Adini & Polat (2015) and Hermens & Walker (2010). The statistical analysis revealed that perceptual judgments of rotation directions under unambiguous cues were faster and more confident than those under the ambiguous conditions. For the micro-saccade, peak velocity and amplitude were higher during perception of unambiguous 3D rotation than those during the ambiguous rotations. There was no significant difference in saccade duration. When participants judged the SFM as rotation of clockwise (left), their microsaccade rate towards left was significantly higher than that towards right and vise versa while the counter-clockwise judgment was made. Under the unambiguous condition, significant differences between CW-cw and CCW-ccw were found during time widows of 150~400 ms and 500~970 ms. In contrast, ambiguous conditions (AMB-cw and AMB-ccw) differed most during 700~950 ms, which indicated extra time of attentional processes. Our findings of temporal dynamics of the ambiguous and unambiguous perceptual judgments of 3D rotations in
注意力如何在解决模糊的感知判断中发挥作用是一个古老的科学问题。理解感知分组、切换、处理速度和感知的过程是解决计算机视觉和涉及三维空间的自动驾驶等应用中重大问题的关键一步。由运动产生的旋转三维结构(SFM)是一种众所周知的双稳态模糊刺激。到目前为止,注意力、眼球运动和深度线索如何调节对3D SFM旋转方向的感知判断仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。早在1925年,亥姆霍兹就提出了由眼睛跟踪信号产生的运动感知(Cavanagh,1992)。Pomerantz(1970)认为眼动在动觉知觉的发生中起着重要作用。此外,神经科学研究支持了眼动和注意力转移的共同神经基础(Grosbras,Laird&Paus,2005)。本研究旨在通过控制外源性视觉线索来研究SFM感知过程中眼动的特征和时间过程,并确定注意力对SFM感知的影响。使用高级眼动分析,我们研究了在明确和模糊的深度线索下的注意力如何调节对深度旋转方向的感知判断。二十二个实验。他们的任务是通过用左手或右手拇指按下鼠标的左键(用于俯视图中的顺时针CW感知)或右键(用于CCW感知)来指示3D SFM的旋转方向。通过30个直径为0.2°的相干移动点以及平均速度为5°/s的不同半径的椭圆轨迹,创建了一个由运动产生的计算机模拟结构(3D旋转球体)。发光点随机分布在5°×5°的球形区域。在使用Bonneh、Adini和Polat(2015)和Hermens&Walker(2010)的方法进行的明确深度下。统计分析表明,在模糊线索下对旋转方向的感知判断比在模糊条件下更快、更自信。对于微扫视,在感知不明确的3D旋转期间的峰值速度和振幅高于在不明确的旋转期间的。扫视持续时间无显著差异。当参与者将SFM判断为顺时针(左)旋转时,当做出逆时针判断时,他们向左的微加速度明显高于向右的微加速度,反之亦然。在无歧义条件下,在150~400ms和500~970ms的时间窗口中,CW CW和CCW CCW之间存在显著差异。相反,在700~950ms的时间窗中,歧义条件(AMB CW和AMB CCW)的差异最大,这表明注意过程的时间是额外的。我们对3D旋转的模糊和毫不含糊的感知判断的时间动力学的发现表明了处理的两个阶段。首先,三维结构施工中刺激开始后150~200ms初始阶段的局部速度计算。其次,视觉处理将局部运动矢量流与旋转方向的整体感知判断结合起来。这种模棱两可的情况需要更长的时间。当旋转是明确的时,在SFM的3D旋转的感知判断过程中,注意力促进在更高级别的处理中加速。
{"title":"Temporal dynamics of eye movements and attentional modulation in perceptual judgments of structure-from-motion (SFM)","authors":"Jinhong Ding, Ya-Min Wang, Jiang Yang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00337","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00337","url":null,"abstract":"How attention plays a role in resolving ambiguous perceptual judgments is one of the age-old scientific questions. Understanding the processes of perceptual grouping, switching, processing speed, and awareness is a key step towards solving significant problems in applications such as computer vision and automatic driving involving three-dimensional space. The rotating three-dimensional (3D) structure from motion (SFM) is a well-known bistable ambiguous stimulus. Thus far, it is still an open question how attention, eye movements, and depth cues modulate perceptual judgments of rotating directions of a 3D SFM. As early as 1925, motion perception resulted from eye tracking signals was proposed by Helmholtz (Cavanagh, 1992). Pomerantz (1970) claimed that eye movement plays an important role in the occurrence of kinesthetic perception. Furthermore, neuroscience studies has supported a common neural basis for eye movement and attention transfer (Grosbras, Laird & Paus, 2005). The current study aimed to investigate the characteristics and the time course of eye movements during SFM perception by controlling exogenous visual cues and ascertain the effect of attention on SFM perception. Using advanced eyemovements analysis, we investigated how attention under both unambiguous and ambiguous depth cues modulate perceptual judgments of rotation directions in deepth. Twenty-two experiment. Their task was to indicate the rotation directions of 3D SFM by pressing the left (for clockwise CW percept from top view) or the right key of a mouse (for CCW percept) with their left or right thumb. A computer simulated structure-from-motion (a 3D rotating sphere) was created via 30 coherently moving dots with 0.2° diameter each along with an elliptical trajectory of different radii at a mean velocity of 5°/s. The luminant dots were randomly distributed in a spherical area extended 5°×5°. Under unambiguous depth conducted using methods from Bonneh, Adini & Polat (2015) and Hermens & Walker (2010). The statistical analysis revealed that perceptual judgments of rotation directions under unambiguous cues were faster and more confident than those under the ambiguous conditions. For the micro-saccade, peak velocity and amplitude were higher during perception of unambiguous 3D rotation than those during the ambiguous rotations. There was no significant difference in saccade duration. When participants judged the SFM as rotation of clockwise (left), their microsaccade rate towards left was significantly higher than that towards right and vise versa while the counter-clockwise judgment was made. Under the unambiguous condition, significant differences between CW-cw and CCW-ccw were found during time widows of 150~400 ms and 500~970 ms. In contrast, ambiguous conditions (AMB-cw and AMB-ccw) differed most during 700~950 ms, which indicated extra time of attentional processes. Our findings of temporal dynamics of the ambiguous and unambiguous perceptual judgments of 3D rotations in","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47242420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Mobile phone addiction and depression: Multiple mediating effects of social anxiety and attentional bias to negative emotional information 手机成瘾与抑郁:社交焦虑和负性情绪信息注意偏向的多重中介作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-04-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00362
Juan Hou, Yingge Zhu, X. Fang
{"title":"Mobile phone addiction and depression: Multiple mediating effects of social anxiety and attentional bias to negative emotional information","authors":"Juan Hou, Yingge Zhu, X. Fang","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00362","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00362","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":"53 1","pages":"362"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44079506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Confucian ideal personality traits (Junzi personality) and mental health: The serial mediating roles of self-control and authenticity 儒家理想人格特质(君子人格)与心理健康:自我控制与真实性的连环中介作用
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2021-04-25 DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00374
Xiaoyu Ge, Yubo Hou
classic Confucian propositions and related empirical studies, this paper proposes the hypothesis that a Junzi personality has a positive effect on mental health, in which self-control and authenticity play serial mediating roles. To examine the above hypothesis, four studies were conducted. In Study 1 ( N = 207), the self-rated questionnaires of Junzi personality, self-control, authenticity, and mental health were used. Study 2 ( N = 425) involved manipulating self-control using the scenario simulation method and looking into its effects on mental health and authenticity. In Study 3 ( N = 317), authenticity, which was manipulated using the method of recall priming, was investigated for its effects on mental health. Participants were also asked to recall real-life instances involving a decision that may or may not have involved self-control to support the external validity. In Study 4 ( N = 220), participants were subjected to four different tests with 2-month intervals for each that measured Junzi personality, self-control, and authenticity, with the final test measuring self-esteem, core self-evaluation, affect balance, and mental symptoms as overall indicators of mental health. Results showed that when measured simultaneously, Junzi personality positively predicted mental health. When measured after 6 months, Junzi personality also positively predicted self-esteem, core self-evaluation, and affect balance but negatively predicted mental symptoms. Junzi personality positively predicted the trait of self-control and decisions involving self-control, whether in situations provided in the experiment or as were recalled by the participants. Manipulated self-control positively affected both mental health and authenticity while manipulated authenticity positively affected mental health. Results also provided evidence to the serial mediating effect of self-control and authenticity in the positive effect of Junzi personality on mental health through simultaneous measurement and cross-temporal measurement. In addition, results of experimental manipulation also provided indirect evidence for this. This research sought to explore the positive relationship between Junzi personality and mental health and to help in the understanding of the internal mechanism of this relationship. The results are consistent with previous studies on the relationship between personality and mental health, self-control and mental health, and authenticity and mental health. In addition, this paper also discusses the practical value of Junzi personality and Confucianism in today’s China.
在儒家经典命题和相关实证研究的基础上,本文提出君子人格对心理健康有积极影响的假说,其中自我控制和真实性起着一系列中介作用。为了验证上述假设,进行了四项研究。研究1(N=207)采用君子人格、自控力、真实性和心理健康自评量表。研究2(N=425)涉及使用情景模拟方法操纵自我控制,并研究其对心理健康和真实性的影响。在研究3(N=317)中,使用回忆启动方法操纵真实性,调查其对心理健康的影响。参与者还被要求回忆现实生活中涉及决策的例子,这些决策可能涉及也可能不涉及自我控制,以支持外部有效性。在研究4(N=220)中,参与者接受了四项不同的测试,每项测试间隔2个月,测量君子的个性、自制力和真实性,最后一项测试测量自尊、核心自我评价、情感平衡和精神症状,作为心理健康的总体指标。结果表明,同时测量时,君子人格对心理健康有正向预测作用。6个月后进行测量时,君子性格也对自尊、核心自我评价和影响平衡有正向预测,但对精神症状有负向预测。无论是在实验中提供的情况下还是在参与者回忆的情况下,君子性格都能积极预测自我控制的特点和涉及自我控制的决策。操纵自我控制对心理健康和真实性都有积极影响,而操纵真实性对心理健康有积极影响。通过同时测量和跨时间测量,结果也为君子人格对心理健康的积极影响中自我控制和真实性的系列中介作用提供了证据。此外,实验操作的结果也为这一点提供了间接的证据。本研究旨在探讨君子人格与心理健康之间的积极关系,并有助于理解这种关系的内在机制。这一结果与以往关于人格与心理健康、自我控制与心理健康以及真实性与心理健康之间关系的研究一致。此外,本文还探讨了君子人格与儒家思想在当今中国的实践价值。
{"title":"Confucian ideal personality traits (Junzi personality) and mental health: The serial mediating roles of self-control and authenticity","authors":"Xiaoyu Ge, Yubo Hou","doi":"10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00374","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00374","url":null,"abstract":"classic Confucian propositions and related empirical studies, this paper proposes the hypothesis that a Junzi personality has a positive effect on mental health, in which self-control and authenticity play serial mediating roles. To examine the above hypothesis, four studies were conducted. In Study 1 ( N = 207), the self-rated questionnaires of Junzi personality, self-control, authenticity, and mental health were used. Study 2 ( N = 425) involved manipulating self-control using the scenario simulation method and looking into its effects on mental health and authenticity. In Study 3 ( N = 317), authenticity, which was manipulated using the method of recall priming, was investigated for its effects on mental health. Participants were also asked to recall real-life instances involving a decision that may or may not have involved self-control to support the external validity. In Study 4 ( N = 220), participants were subjected to four different tests with 2-month intervals for each that measured Junzi personality, self-control, and authenticity, with the final test measuring self-esteem, core self-evaluation, affect balance, and mental symptoms as overall indicators of mental health. Results showed that when measured simultaneously, Junzi personality positively predicted mental health. When measured after 6 months, Junzi personality also positively predicted self-esteem, core self-evaluation, and affect balance but negatively predicted mental symptoms. Junzi personality positively predicted the trait of self-control and decisions involving self-control, whether in situations provided in the experiment or as were recalled by the participants. Manipulated self-control positively affected both mental health and authenticity while manipulated authenticity positively affected mental health. Results also provided evidence to the serial mediating effect of self-control and authenticity in the positive effect of Junzi personality on mental health through simultaneous measurement and cross-temporal measurement. In addition, results of experimental manipulation also provided indirect evidence for this. This research sought to explore the positive relationship between Junzi personality and mental health and to help in the understanding of the internal mechanism of this relationship. The results are consistent with previous studies on the relationship between personality and mental health, self-control and mental health, and authenticity and mental health. In addition, this paper also discusses the practical value of Junzi personality and Confucianism in today’s China.","PeriodicalId":36627,"journal":{"name":"心理学报","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42703997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
期刊
心理学报
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1