首页 > 最新文献

Nordic Journal of Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Voicing patterns in stops among heritage speakers of Western Armenian in Lebanon and the US 黎巴嫩和美国讲西亚美尼亚语的传统人在停顿中的发音模式
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-05-24 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586521000093
Niamh E. Kelly, Lara Keshishian
Abstract Research on Western Armenian (WA) has described it as having a contrast between voiceless aspirated stops and voiced stops (Fairbanks 1948; Vaux 1998; Baronian 2017). Since there is no monolingual community of WA, all speakers are part of a minority language community, and also speak the majority language. The current study examines speakers from two heritage communities of WA: one in Lebanon, where the majority language is Arabic, and one in the US, where the majority language is English. The speakers in Lebanon were found to have a contrast between voiced and voiceless unaspirated stops, in line with Lebanese Arabic. The speakers in the US were more variable, some having the English pattern of voiceless unaspirated and voiceless aspirated stops, while others had voiceless aspirated stops, but their voiced stops were variable between voiced and voiceless unaspirated. These results indicate L2 transfer in both communities, leading to two different patterns of voicing in WA.
对西亚美尼亚语(WA)的抽象研究将其描述为无声送气塞音和有声塞音之间的对比(Fairbanks 1948;Vaux 1998;Baronian 2017)。由于西澳州不存在单语社区,所有讲者都是少数民族语言社区的一部分,也讲多数民族语言。目前的研究调查了西澳大利亚州两个遗产社区的发言人:一个在黎巴嫩,那里的主要语言是阿拉伯语,另一个在美国,那里的大多数语言是英语。黎巴嫩的说话者被发现有声和无声的未经修饰的停顿之间有对比,与黎巴嫩阿拉伯语一致。在美国,说话者的变化更大,一些人的英语模式是无声息和无声息送气停顿,而另一些人则有无声息,但他们的浊音停顿在有声息和无声息之间是可变的。这些结果表明,二语在两个社区中都有迁移,导致了西澳大利亚语中两种不同的发音模式。
{"title":"Voicing patterns in stops among heritage speakers of Western Armenian in Lebanon and the US","authors":"Niamh E. Kelly, Lara Keshishian","doi":"10.1017/S0332586521000093","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586521000093","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Research on Western Armenian (WA) has described it as having a contrast between voiceless aspirated stops and voiced stops (Fairbanks 1948; Vaux 1998; Baronian 2017). Since there is no monolingual community of WA, all speakers are part of a minority language community, and also speak the majority language. The current study examines speakers from two heritage communities of WA: one in Lebanon, where the majority language is Arabic, and one in the US, where the majority language is English. The speakers in Lebanon were found to have a contrast between voiced and voiceless unaspirated stops, in line with Lebanese Arabic. The speakers in the US were more variable, some having the English pattern of voiceless unaspirated and voiceless aspirated stops, while others had voiceless aspirated stops, but their voiced stops were variable between voiced and voiceless unaspirated. These results indicate L2 transfer in both communities, leading to two different patterns of voicing in WA.","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586521000093","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44268266","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Finnish inserted vowels: a case of phonologized excrescence 芬兰语插入元音:一个音系异常的例子
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-05-18 DOI: 10.1017/S033258652100007X
Robin Karlin
Abstract In this paper, I examine a case of vowel insertion found in Savo and Pohjanmaa dialects of Finnish that is typically called “epenthesis”, but which demonstrates characteristics of both phonetic excrescence and phonological epenthesis. Based on a phonological analysis paired with an acoustic corpus study, I argue that Finnish vowel insertion is the mixed result of phonetic excrescence and the phonologization of these vowels, and is related to second-mora lengthening, another dialectal phenomenon. I propose a gestural model of second-mora lengthening that would generate vowel insertion in its original phonetic state. The link to second-mora lengthening provides a unified account that addresses both the dialectal and phonological distribution of the phenomenon, which have not been linked in previous literature.
摘要在本文中,我研究了芬兰语Savo和Pohjanmaa方言中发现的一个元音插入案例,该案例通常被称为“ephenthesis”,但它同时表现出语音赘音和语音ephentheis的特征。基于语音分析和声学语料库研究,我认为芬兰语元音插入是这些元音的语音赘肉和音韵化的混合结果,并且与另一种方言现象第二音节延长有关。我提出了一个第二音节延长的手势模型,该模型将在其原始语音状态下产生元音插入。与第二个音节延长的联系提供了一个统一的解释,解决了该现象的方言和语音分布,这在以前的文献中没有联系。
{"title":"Finnish inserted vowels: a case of phonologized excrescence","authors":"Robin Karlin","doi":"10.1017/S033258652100007X","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S033258652100007X","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In this paper, I examine a case of vowel insertion found in Savo and Pohjanmaa dialects of Finnish that is typically called “epenthesis”, but which demonstrates characteristics of both phonetic excrescence and phonological epenthesis. Based on a phonological analysis paired with an acoustic corpus study, I argue that Finnish vowel insertion is the mixed result of phonetic excrescence and the phonologization of these vowels, and is related to second-mora lengthening, another dialectal phenomenon. I propose a gestural model of second-mora lengthening that would generate vowel insertion in its original phonetic state. The link to second-mora lengthening provides a unified account that addresses both the dialectal and phonological distribution of the phenomenon, which have not been linked in previous literature.","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S033258652100007X","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49436960","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
NJL volume 44 issue 1 Cover and Back matter NJL第44卷第1期封面和封底
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-05-01 DOI: 10.1017/s0332586521000044
{"title":"NJL volume 44 issue 1 Cover and Back matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s0332586521000044","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0332586521000044","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/s0332586521000044","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42456389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
NJL volume 44 issue 1 Cover and Front matter NJL第44卷第1期封面和封面
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-05-01 DOI: 10.1017/s0332586521000032
{"title":"NJL volume 44 issue 1 Cover and Front matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s0332586521000032","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0332586521000032","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/s0332586521000032","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43429931","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grammatical gender in L2 Swedish in Finnish-speaking immersion students: A comparison with non-immersion students 芬兰语浸入式学生与非浸入式学生在第二语言瑞典语中的语法性别比较
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-02-24 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586520000256
Eeva-Liisa Nyqvist, Sinikka Lahtinen
Abstract Swedish grammatical gender is challenging for Finnish-speaking learners of Swedish due to its abstract meaning, the complex nature of Swedish NPs and the low salience of the morphology used to mark gender. Our study compares the expression of gender in texts written in Swedish by Finnish-speaking 12- and 15-year-old immersion students with that of 16-year-old non-immersion students. The results show that NPs with gender agreement, i.e. those with several morphemes marking gender, are more difficult than NPs with only one marker. In all informant groups, uter is significantly easier than neuter, but uter is also overused, as approximately 75% of all Swedish nouns are uter in modern Swedish. Comparisons between different informant groups show that non-immersion students often reach a significantly higher level of accuracy than immersion students, which indicates that formal teaching has a positive effect.
瑞典语的语法性别对讲芬兰语的瑞典语学习者来说是一个挑战,因为它的意义抽象,瑞典语np的复杂性以及用于标记性别的形态学的不显著性。我们的研究比较了12岁和15岁的芬兰语浸入式学生与16岁的非浸入式学生在瑞典语文本中的性别表达。结果表明,具有多个词素标记性别的np比只有一个词素标记性别的np更难识别。在所有的信息组中,ter明显比中性更容易使用,但ter也被过度使用,因为在现代瑞典语中,大约75%的瑞典名词都是outer。不同信息者群体之间的比较表明,非浸入式学生往往达到显著高于浸入式学生的准确性水平,这表明正式教学具有积极作用。
{"title":"Grammatical gender in L2 Swedish in Finnish-speaking immersion students: A comparison with non-immersion students","authors":"Eeva-Liisa Nyqvist, Sinikka Lahtinen","doi":"10.1017/S0332586520000256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000256","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Swedish grammatical gender is challenging for Finnish-speaking learners of Swedish due to its abstract meaning, the complex nature of Swedish NPs and the low salience of the morphology used to mark gender. Our study compares the expression of gender in texts written in Swedish by Finnish-speaking 12- and 15-year-old immersion students with that of 16-year-old non-immersion students. The results show that NPs with gender agreement, i.e. those with several morphemes marking gender, are more difficult than NPs with only one marker. In all informant groups, uter is significantly easier than neuter, but uter is also overused, as approximately 75% of all Swedish nouns are uter in modern Swedish. Comparisons between different informant groups show that non-immersion students often reach a significantly higher level of accuracy than immersion students, which indicates that formal teaching has a positive effect.","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586520000256","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41718615","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Easy Language research 简易语言研究
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-02-22 DOI: 10.1017/s0332586521000019
Ulla Vanhatalo, C. Lindholm, Tiina Onikki-Rantajääskö
The second issue of Volume 45 (autumn 2022) of the Nordic Journal of Linguistics will be a special issue devoted to Easy Language research. The issue will be edited by Camilla Lindholm, Tiina Onikki-Rantajääskö and Ulla Vanhatalo. The term EASY LANGUAGE (Germ. Leichte Sprache, Swe. lätt språk, Finn. selkokieli; earlier EASY-TO-READ LANGUAGE) refers to a modified variety of a natural language that has been adjusted so that it is easier to read and understand in terms of content, vocabulary and structure. Easy Language has been primarily targeted at people who have various difficulties in understanding standard forms of language, for example, due to learning disabilities or neurocognitive disorders. One can also see a connection between Easy Language and PLAIN LANGUAGE (see International Plain Language Federation, https://www.iplfederation.org/), both aiming at simplifying language. Although attempts to create understandable language have probably been around for a long time, the start of the Easy Language era in the European context can be dated back to the late 1960s, when the first easy-to-read Swedish materials were published. Significant milestones were reached in the 1990s, when the first recommendations for easy writing were published by The International Federation of Library Associations and Institutions (IFLA, https://www.ifla.org/) and Inclusion Europe. Since then, the target groups of Easy Language have been expanding, comprising people with various backgrounds and life situations. Ongoing changes in societies and legislation on accessibility are creating increasing need for Easy Language and research on the topic. Besides language studies, Easy Language is also of interest in many disciplines such as economics, health sciences, sociology and cognitive science. While Easy Languages have been used in Europe and especially in the Nordic countries for decades already, only little linguistic research has been done until the last few years. The development and use of Easy Languages have been built on experience gained from practical work done with the target groups (Wengelin 2015). More recently, especially interesting cases have been seen in Germany, where development and research are currently carried on a large scale (e.g. Bredel & Maaß 2016, Bock 2019, Maaß & Rink 2019, Hansen-Schirra & Maaß 2020). In this call for papers of NJL, we invite research papers which take a variety of linguistic approaches to Easy Language. First of all, we want to learn how Easy Nordic Journal of Linguistics (2021), page 1 of 2 doi:10.1017/S0332586521000019
《北欧语言学杂志》第45卷第二期(2022年秋季)将是一期专门讨论简易语言研究的特刊。本期杂志将由Camilla Lindholm、Tiina Onikki Rantajääskö和Ulla Vanhatalo编辑。易用语言一词(Germ.Leichte-Sprache,Swe.lätt-språk,Finn.selkokieli;早期的易用语言)是指一种经过调整的自然语言变体,在内容、词汇和结构方面更容易阅读和理解。Easy Language主要针对那些在理解标准语言形式方面有各种困难的人,例如,由于学习障碍或神经认知障碍。人们还可以看到简易语言和简明语言之间的联系(参见国际简明语言联合会,https://www.iplfederation.org/)两者都旨在简化语言。尽管创造可理解语言的尝试可能已经存在很长一段时间了,但在欧洲背景下,轻松语言时代的开始可以追溯到20世纪60年代末,当时第一本易读的瑞典语材料出版了。20世纪90年代,国际图书馆协会和机构联合会(IFLA,https://www.ifla.org/)以及包容欧洲。从那时起,Easy Language的目标群体不断扩大,包括各种背景和生活情况的人。社会和无障碍立法的不断变化正在增加对简易语言和该主题研究的需求。除了语言研究外,Easy language还对经济学、健康科学、社会学和认知科学等许多学科感兴趣。尽管Easy Languages在欧洲,尤其是北欧国家已经使用了几十年,但直到最近几年,人们对它的语言学研究很少。Easy Languages的开发和使用建立在与目标群体进行实际工作的经验基础上(Wengelin 2015)。最近,在德国出现了特别有趣的案例,目前德国正在进行大规模的开发和研究(例如Bredel&Maaß2016、Bock 2019、Maał&Rink 2019、Hansen Schirra&Maa 223; 2020)。在这次NJL的论文征集中,我们邀请了对Easy Language采用各种语言学方法的研究论文。首先,我们想了解《北欧语言学杂志》(2021),第1页,共2页doi:10.1017/S0332586521000019
{"title":"Easy Language research","authors":"Ulla Vanhatalo, C. Lindholm, Tiina Onikki-Rantajääskö","doi":"10.1017/s0332586521000019","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0332586521000019","url":null,"abstract":"The second issue of Volume 45 (autumn 2022) of the Nordic Journal of Linguistics will be a special issue devoted to Easy Language research. The issue will be edited by Camilla Lindholm, Tiina Onikki-Rantajääskö and Ulla Vanhatalo. The term EASY LANGUAGE (Germ. Leichte Sprache, Swe. lätt språk, Finn. selkokieli; earlier EASY-TO-READ LANGUAGE) refers to a modified variety of a natural language that has been adjusted so that it is easier to read and understand in terms of content, vocabulary and structure. Easy Language has been primarily targeted at people who have various difficulties in understanding standard forms of language, for example, due to learning disabilities or neurocognitive disorders. One can also see a connection between Easy Language and PLAIN LANGUAGE (see International Plain Language Federation, https://www.iplfederation.org/), both aiming at simplifying language. Although attempts to create understandable language have probably been around for a long time, the start of the Easy Language era in the European context can be dated back to the late 1960s, when the first easy-to-read Swedish materials were published. Significant milestones were reached in the 1990s, when the first recommendations for easy writing were published by The International Federation of Library Associations and Institutions (IFLA, https://www.ifla.org/) and Inclusion Europe. Since then, the target groups of Easy Language have been expanding, comprising people with various backgrounds and life situations. Ongoing changes in societies and legislation on accessibility are creating increasing need for Easy Language and research on the topic. Besides language studies, Easy Language is also of interest in many disciplines such as economics, health sciences, sociology and cognitive science. While Easy Languages have been used in Europe and especially in the Nordic countries for decades already, only little linguistic research has been done until the last few years. The development and use of Easy Languages have been built on experience gained from practical work done with the target groups (Wengelin 2015). More recently, especially interesting cases have been seen in Germany, where development and research are currently carried on a large scale (e.g. Bredel & Maaß 2016, Bock 2019, Maaß & Rink 2019, Hansen-Schirra & Maaß 2020). In this call for papers of NJL, we invite research papers which take a variety of linguistic approaches to Easy Language. First of all, we want to learn how Easy Nordic Journal of Linguistics (2021), page 1 of 2 doi:10.1017/S0332586521000019","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2021-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/s0332586521000019","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41600441","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Marking one’s own viewpoint: The Finnish evidential verb+kseni ‘as far as I understand’ construction 表明自己的观点:芬兰语证据动词+kseni“据我所知”结构
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-12-23 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586520000244
Minna Jaakola
Abstract This article examines evidentiality in the frame of inferential adverbs in written interaction from the perspective of Finnish, a language that does not have evidentiality as a grammatical category. The analysis focuses on six adverbs, such as käsittääkseni ‘as far as I understand’ and tietääkseni ‘to my knowledge, as far as I know’. Evidentiality and epistemic modality intertwine in their semantics, as these adverbs represent a writer’s access to information, but also indicate her evaluation of its reliability. First, this article offers a description of the interactional functions of these adverbs such as marking a writer’s opinion in contrasts, expressing slight hedging in order to anticipate corrections, to allow space for other opinions, or to create irony. Second, in the framework of cognitive grammar, the analysis focuses on the meaning of the evidential verb+kseni construction and the effect of different verb stems on it. These adverbs share similar functions in texts, which is due to their flexible constructional meaning. While varying from lexeme to lexeme, specific evidential and epistemic dimensions can either be foregrounded and relevant in a situation or remain backgrounded and not activated.
摘要本文从芬兰语这一不具有证据性语法范畴的语言的角度出发,考察了书面互动中推理副词框架下的证据性。分析的重点是六个副词,如käsittääkseni“据我所知”和tietääkseni“据我所知,据我所知”。证据性和认知情态在语义上交织在一起,因为这些副词代表了作者对信息的获取,但也表明了她对信息可靠性的评价。首先,本文描述了这些副词的互动功能,如在对比中标记作者的观点,表达轻微的模棱两可以预测更正,为其他观点留出空间,或创造讽刺。其次,在认知语法的框架下,重点分析证据动词+谓语结构的意义以及不同动词词干对其的影响。这些副词在语篇中具有相似的功能,这是由于它们具有灵活的结构意义。虽然不同的词位不同,但特定的证据和认知维度可以是突出的和相关的,也可以是背景的,不被激活。
{"title":"Marking one’s own viewpoint: The Finnish evidential verb+kseni ‘as far as I understand’ construction","authors":"Minna Jaakola","doi":"10.1017/S0332586520000244","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000244","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article examines evidentiality in the frame of inferential adverbs in written interaction from the perspective of Finnish, a language that does not have evidentiality as a grammatical category. The analysis focuses on six adverbs, such as käsittääkseni ‘as far as I understand’ and tietääkseni ‘to my knowledge, as far as I know’. Evidentiality and epistemic modality intertwine in their semantics, as these adverbs represent a writer’s access to information, but also indicate her evaluation of its reliability. First, this article offers a description of the interactional functions of these adverbs such as marking a writer’s opinion in contrasts, expressing slight hedging in order to anticipate corrections, to allow space for other opinions, or to create irony. Second, in the framework of cognitive grammar, the analysis focuses on the meaning of the evidential verb+kseni construction and the effect of different verb stems on it. These adverbs share similar functions in texts, which is due to their flexible constructional meaning. While varying from lexeme to lexeme, specific evidential and epistemic dimensions can either be foregrounded and relevant in a situation or remain backgrounded and not activated.","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586520000244","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41493528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Variation in adjunct islands: The case of Norwegian 附属岛屿的变化:以挪威人为例
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-12-11 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586520000207
Ingrid Bondevik, Dave Kush, Terje Lohndal
Abstract Finite adjunct clauses are often assumed to be among the strongest islands for filler–gap dependency creation cross-linguistically, but Kush, Lohndal & Sprouse (2019) found experimental evidence suggesting that finite conditional om-adjunct clauses are not islands for topicalization in Norwegian. To investigate the generality of these findings, we ran three acceptability judgment experiments testing topicalization out of three adjunct clause types: om ‘if’, når ‘when’ and fordi ‘because’ in Norwegian. Largely replicating Kush et al. (2019), we find evidence for the absence of strong island effects with topicalization from om-adjuncts in all three experiments. We find island effects for når- and fordi-adjuncts, but the size of the effects and the underlying judgment distributions that produce those effects differ greatly by island type. Our results suggest that the syntactic category ‘adjunct’ may not constitute a suitably fine-grained grouping to explain variation in island effects.
摘要有限附加从句通常被认为是跨语言产生填充-间隙依赖关系的最强岛屿之一,但Kush,Lohndal&Sprouse(2019)发现的实验证据表明,有限条件附加从句在挪威语中不是话题化的岛屿。为了研究这些发现的普遍性,我们进行了三个可接受性判断实验,测试了三种附加从句类型的主题化:挪威语中的om“if”、når“when”和fordi“because”。在很大程度上复制了Kush等人(2019),我们发现在所有三个实验中,om佐剂的局部化没有强烈的岛屿效应的证据。我们发现了når-和fordi佐剂的岛屿效应,但效应的大小和产生这些效应的基本判断分布因岛屿类型而异。我们的结果表明,句法类别“附属词”可能不构成一个适当的细粒度分组来解释岛屿效应的变化。
{"title":"Variation in adjunct islands: The case of Norwegian","authors":"Ingrid Bondevik, Dave Kush, Terje Lohndal","doi":"10.1017/S0332586520000207","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000207","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Finite adjunct clauses are often assumed to be among the strongest islands for filler–gap dependency creation cross-linguistically, but Kush, Lohndal & Sprouse (2019) found experimental evidence suggesting that finite conditional om-adjunct clauses are not islands for topicalization in Norwegian. To investigate the generality of these findings, we ran three acceptability judgment experiments testing topicalization out of three adjunct clause types: om ‘if’, når ‘when’ and fordi ‘because’ in Norwegian. Largely replicating Kush et al. (2019), we find evidence for the absence of strong island effects with topicalization from om-adjuncts in all three experiments. We find island effects for når- and fordi-adjuncts, but the size of the effects and the underlying judgment distributions that produce those effects differ greatly by island type. Our results suggest that the syntactic category ‘adjunct’ may not constitute a suitably fine-grained grouping to explain variation in island effects.","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586520000207","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41287436","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
NJL volume 43 issue 3 Cover and Back matter NJL第43卷第3期封面和封底
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-12-01 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586520000232
{"title":"NJL volume 43 issue 3 Cover and Back matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/S0332586520000232","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000232","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586520000232","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42774815","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
NJL volume 43 issue 3 Cover and Front matter NJL第43卷第3期封面和封面问题
IF 0.5 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-12-01 DOI: 10.1017/S0332586520000220
{"title":"NJL volume 43 issue 3 Cover and Front matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/S0332586520000220","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586520000220","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43203,"journal":{"name":"Nordic Journal of Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1017/S0332586520000220","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43281097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Nordic Journal of Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1