{"title":"Morphological properties of mouthings in Hungarian Sign Language (MJNY)","authors":"Szilárd Engelhardt","doi":"10.1075/SLL.00016.ENG","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/SLL.00016.ENG","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43398,"journal":{"name":"Sign Language & Linguistics","volume":"929 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2018-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"59106848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We describe an intriguing interaction of negation and tense in Georgian Sign Language (GESL), a sign language which to date has received close to no attention by linguists. GESL verbs that employ an irregular negation strategy in the present tense (modal verbs and the verb know) require double marking in the past tense, i.e. the irregular negative form combines with the negative particle not, which is not used in the present and future tense with these verbs. The GESL data thus provide us with direct evidence for an active contribution of the feature tense in the grammar of GESL – in contrast to most other sign languages previously studied. We also offer a cross-linguistic perspective on the data by discussing instances of Negative Concord reported for various sign languages as well as tense-negation interactions attested in spoken languages.
{"title":"A negation-tense interaction in Georgian Sign Language","authors":"Tamar Makharoblidze, R. Pfau","doi":"10.1075/SLL.00013.MAK","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/SLL.00013.MAK","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 We describe an intriguing interaction of negation and tense in Georgian Sign Language (GESL), a sign language which to date has\u0000 received close to no attention by linguists. GESL verbs that employ an irregular negation strategy in the present tense (modal\u0000 verbs and the verb know) require double marking in the past tense, i.e. the irregular negative form combines with the\u0000 negative particle not, which is not used in the present and future tense with these verbs. The GESL data thus provide us with direct\u0000 evidence for an active contribution of the feature tense in the grammar of GESL – in contrast to most other sign languages\u0000 previously studied. We also offer a cross-linguistic perspective on the data by discussing instances of Negative Concord reported\u0000 for various sign languages as well as tense-negation interactions attested in spoken languages.","PeriodicalId":43398,"journal":{"name":"Sign Language & Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2018-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46991844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Lexical nonmanuals in German Sign Language (DGS)","authors":"Nina-Kristin Pendzich","doi":"10.1075/SLL.00015.PEN","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/SLL.00015.PEN","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":43398,"journal":{"name":"Sign Language & Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2018-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48073382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In this paper. we offer a preliminary investigation of some aspects of individual and group variation in sign rate and rhythm, considering the sociolinguistic factors of Age (younger and older adults), Gender, and Sign Variety (Black and Mainstream American Sign Language). Differences in sign rate and rhythmic structure among signers were found in signers’ elicited narratives. A novel approach to phrasal rhythm is introduced, called “rhythm ratio”, which considers sign duration and transition duration together and is similar in spirit to the “normalized pairwise variability index” (nPVI) in spoken languages. This measure appears to be promising as a method for identifying rhythm class in sign languages; however, due to the small number of signers in each group these results can only be suggestive.
{"title":"Variation in phrasal rhythm in sign languages","authors":"D. Brentari, Joseph C. Hill, Brianne Amador","doi":"10.1075/SLL.00010.BRE","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/SLL.00010.BRE","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In this paper. we offer a preliminary investigation of some aspects of individual and group variation in sign rate and rhythm,\u0000 considering the sociolinguistic factors of Age (younger and older adults), Gender, and Sign Variety (Black and Mainstream American\u0000 Sign Language). Differences in sign rate and rhythmic structure among signers were found in signers’ elicited narratives. A novel\u0000 approach to phrasal rhythm is introduced, called “rhythm ratio”, which considers sign duration and transition duration together\u0000 and is similar in spirit to the “normalized pairwise variability index” (nPVI) in spoken languages. This measure appears to be\u0000 promising as a method for identifying rhythm class in sign languages; however, due to the small number of signers in each group\u0000 these results can only be suggestive.","PeriodicalId":43398,"journal":{"name":"Sign Language & Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2018-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41871675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Discourse markers in French Belgian Sign Language (LSFB) and Catalan Sign Language (LSC)","authors":"Sílvia Gabarró-López","doi":"10.1075/SLL.00014.GAB","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/SLL.00014.GAB","url":null,"abstract":"Discourse markers in French Belgian Sign Language (LSFB) and Catalan Sign Language (LSC) : Buoys, PALM-UP and SAME","PeriodicalId":43398,"journal":{"name":"Sign Language & Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2018-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41940847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
German Sign Language (DGS) displays variation in the simple plural, the form of which is conditioned by classes of phonological features within the lexicon. As a consequence, the overt realization of the plural marker is restricted to a small set of nouns specified for the appropriate phonological features, while the rest are left bare (Pfau &