首页 > 最新文献

International Review of Pragmatics最新文献

英文 中文
Evaluating pragmatic competence 评估语用能力
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-28 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202006
A. Akbari, Monir Gholamzadeh Bazarbash, Raheleh Alinejadi
This paper presents an investigation into the impact of teaching pragmatic competence to translation students who translate from English (L2) to Persian (L1). For the experiment, the participants were requested to identify implicit discourse markers in a source text and to transfer them into the target text. This investigation used Think Aloud Protocols (TAP) to monitor students’ inferential translation processes. The results of this study pinpointed the challenging role of pragmatic competence for translation students. Translation performance in an experimental group of participants exposed to a period of pragmatic classroom instruction was compared to that of a control group which did not receive this training. Finally, the data analysis indicated that pragmatic teaching improved the translation students’ pragmatic competence in the experimental group through identifying both implicit and explicit discourse markers in the source text. This was clearly lacking in a number of students’ translations in the control group.
本文旨在探讨语用能力教学对英语(第二语言)译为波斯语(第一语言)的翻译学生的影响。在实验中,参与者被要求识别源文本中的隐含话语标记,并将其转化为目标文本。本研究采用出声思考协议(TAP)来监测学生的推理翻译过程。本研究的结果明确了语用能力对翻译学生的挑战作用。实验组的参与者接受了一段时间的语用课堂教学,并将其翻译表现与未接受这种培训的对照组进行了比较。最后,数据分析表明,语用教学通过识别源语中的隐性语篇标记和显性语篇标记,提高了实验组翻译学生的语用能力。这在对照组的一些学生的翻译中显然是缺乏的。
{"title":"Evaluating pragmatic competence","authors":"A. Akbari, Monir Gholamzadeh Bazarbash, Raheleh Alinejadi","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202006","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This paper presents an investigation into the impact of teaching pragmatic competence to translation students who translate from English (L2) to Persian (L1). For the experiment, the participants were requested to identify implicit discourse markers in a source text and to transfer them into the target text. This investigation used Think Aloud Protocols (TAP) to monitor students’ inferential translation processes. The results of this study pinpointed the challenging role of pragmatic competence for translation students. Translation performance in an experimental group of participants exposed to a period of pragmatic classroom instruction was compared to that of a control group which did not receive this training. Finally, the data analysis indicated that pragmatic teaching improved the translation students’ pragmatic competence in the experimental group through identifying both implicit and explicit discourse markers in the source text. This was clearly lacking in a number of students’ translations in the control group.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42660882","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Puzzles for pragmatics and rhetoric and advent of pragma-rhetoric 语用学与修辞学的困惑与语用修辞学的出现
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-19 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202003
Guojin Hou
No other interdisciplinary issue has inspired a greater debate than the pragmatics-rhetoric border. This paper explores the pragmatics-rhetoric boundary issues and the possibility of marrying pragmatics to rhetoric for pragma-rhetoric. It first addresses the twenty ‘puzzles’ or predicaments the past studies of pragmatics and rhetoric have met with. It is held that similarities between the two disciplines make ‘pragma-rhetoric’ possible and their differences serve as the conditions for their inter-complementarity. Then we discuss some misunderstandings about pragma-rhetoric integration. Due to the alikeness of speech acts, pragmatic acts and rhetoric acts, we forward ‘pragma-rhetorical act’ (PRA) for an umbrella term in the emerging ‘pragma-rhetoric’. Finally we formulate the academic tasks and features of the interdiscipline, with a ‘standard paradigm’ and two ‘sub-paradigms’ of pragma-rhetorical research.
没有其他跨学科的问题比语用修辞边界更能引发争论。本文探讨了语用修辞的边界问题以及语用修辞与修辞相结合的可能性。它首先解决了过去语用学和修辞学研究中遇到的二十个“困惑”或困境。认为这两个学科的相似性使“语用修辞”成为可能,而它们的差异又是它们相互补充的条件。然后,我们讨论了对语用修辞整合的一些误解。由于言语行为、语用行为和修辞行为的相似性,我们将“语用修辞行为”(PRA)作为新兴的“语用修辞”中的一个总括术语。最后,我们通过语用修辞研究的“标准范式”和两个“子范式”,阐述了跨学科的学术任务和特点。
{"title":"Puzzles for pragmatics and rhetoric and advent of pragma-rhetoric","authors":"Guojin Hou","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202003","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 No other interdisciplinary issue has inspired a greater debate than the pragmatics-rhetoric border. This paper explores the pragmatics-rhetoric boundary issues and the possibility of marrying pragmatics to rhetoric for pragma-rhetoric. It first addresses the twenty ‘puzzles’ or predicaments the past studies of pragmatics and rhetoric have met with. It is held that similarities between the two disciplines make ‘pragma-rhetoric’ possible and their differences serve as the conditions for their inter-complementarity. Then we discuss some misunderstandings about pragma-rhetoric integration. Due to the alikeness of speech acts, pragmatic acts and rhetoric acts, we forward ‘pragma-rhetorical act’ (PRA) for an umbrella term in the emerging ‘pragma-rhetoric’. Finally we formulate the academic tasks and features of the interdiscipline, with a ‘standard paradigm’ and two ‘sub-paradigms’ of pragma-rhetorical research.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46102679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Representation, conceptualization and positioning in Critical Discourse Analysis 批评语篇分析中的表征、概念化与定位
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-19 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202004
Piotr Cap
The present paper explores the current nexus between Cognitive Linguistics (CL) and Critical Discourse Analysis (CDA), focusing on theories of conceptual positioning, distancing and perspective-taking in discourse space. It assesses the strengths, limitations, and prospects for further operationalization of positioning as a valid methodology in CDA, and political discourse studies in particular. In the first part, I review the cognitive models of positioning that have made the most significant contribution to CDA. Discussing Deictic Space Theory and Text World Theory, among others, I argue that these models reveal further theoretical potential which has not been exploited yet. While they offer a comprehensive and plausible account of how representations and ideologically charged worldviews are established, they fail to deliver a pragmatic explanation of how addressees are made to establish a worldview, in the service of speaker’s goals. The second part of the paper outlines Proximization Theory, a discursive model of crisis and conflict construction in political discourse. I argue that, unlike the other models, it fully captures the complex geopolitical and ideological positioning in political discourse space, providing a viable handle on the dynamics of conflict between the opposing ideologies of the space.
本文探讨了当前认知语言学(CL)与批评语篇分析(CDA)之间的关系,重点研究了语篇空间中的概念定位、距离和视角选择理论。它评估了定位作为CDA中一种有效方法的优势、局限性和进一步操作的前景,尤其是政治话语研究。在第一部分中,我回顾了对CDA做出最重要贡献的定位认知模型。在讨论指示空间理论和文本世界理论等问题时,我认为这些模型揭示了尚未被开发的进一步的理论潜力。虽然它们对表征和意识形态世界观是如何建立的提供了全面而合理的描述,但它们未能对收件人如何建立世界观以服务于演讲者的目标做出务实的解释。文章的第二部分概述了政治话语中危机与冲突建构的话语模式——代理理论。我认为,与其他模型不同,它充分捕捉了政治话语空间中复杂的地缘政治和意识形态定位,为该空间对立意识形态之间的冲突动态提供了一个可行的处理方式。
{"title":"Representation, conceptualization and positioning in Critical Discourse Analysis","authors":"Piotr Cap","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202004","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The present paper explores the current nexus between Cognitive Linguistics (CL) and Critical Discourse Analysis (CDA), focusing on theories of conceptual positioning, distancing and perspective-taking in discourse space. It assesses the strengths, limitations, and prospects for further operationalization of positioning as a valid methodology in CDA, and political discourse studies in particular. In the first part, I review the cognitive models of positioning that have made the most significant contribution to CDA. Discussing Deictic Space Theory and Text World Theory, among others, I argue that these models reveal further theoretical potential which has not been exploited yet. While they offer a comprehensive and plausible account of how representations and ideologically charged worldviews are established, they fail to deliver a pragmatic explanation of how addressees are made to establish a worldview, in the service of speaker’s goals. The second part of the paper outlines Proximization Theory, a discursive model of crisis and conflict construction in political discourse. I argue that, unlike the other models, it fully captures the complex geopolitical and ideological positioning in political discourse space, providing a viable handle on the dynamics of conflict between the opposing ideologies of the space.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48387934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Relevance, effects and affect 相关性、影响和影响
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-19 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202001
L. D. Saussure, T. Wharton
In this paper, we argue that the successful integration of expressive acts of communication into an inferential theory of pragmatics faces a major challenge. Most post-Gricean pragmatic theories have worked to develop accounts of the interpretive processes at work in the communication of propositions; the challenge, therefore, is how expressive acts be integrated when their content is, as it appears to be, non-propositional. Following previous research (Wharton, 2009), we link the affective effects produced as a result of such acts to descriptive ineffability and procedurality, and argue that they activate experiential heuristics through which they find relevance. Our approach stands at least partially within the development of recent approaches to emotion as evaluative devices (appraisal theory) and we suggest that certain cognitive effects arise in communication thanks to affective effects, which then act as attention attractors and boosters for optimally relevant cognitive effects. We show that, sometimes, affect can win out over the non-affective side of cognition and also that at least some poetic artefacts may activate ‘pure affective effects’, which can be relevant in their own right, i.e. relevant without cognitive effects.
在本文中,我们认为将交际的表达行为成功地融入语用学的推理理论面临着重大挑战。大多数后Gricean语用理论都致力于发展对命题交流中的解释过程的描述;因此,挑战在于,当表达行为的内容似乎是非命题的时,如何将其整合在一起。根据之前的研究(沃顿商学院,2009年),我们将此类行为产生的情感效果与描述性的无效性和程序性联系起来,并认为它们激活了经验启发法,从而找到了相关性。我们的方法至少在一定程度上符合最近将情绪作为评估工具的方法(评估理论)的发展,我们认为,由于情感效应,某些认知效应在沟通中产生,然后情感效应作为注意力吸引器和最佳相关认知效果的助推器。我们表明,有时,情感可以战胜认知的非情感方面,而且至少一些诗歌作品可能会激活“纯粹的情感效果”,这种效果本身就具有相关性,即在没有认知效果的情况下具有相关性。
{"title":"Relevance, effects and affect","authors":"L. D. Saussure, T. Wharton","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202001","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In this paper, we argue that the successful integration of expressive acts of communication into an inferential theory of pragmatics faces a major challenge. Most post-Gricean pragmatic theories have worked to develop accounts of the interpretive processes at work in the communication of propositions; the challenge, therefore, is how expressive acts be integrated when their content is, as it appears to be, non-propositional. Following previous research (Wharton, 2009), we link the affective effects produced as a result of such acts to descriptive ineffability and procedurality, and argue that they activate experiential heuristics through which they find relevance. Our approach stands at least partially within the development of recent approaches to emotion as evaluative devices (appraisal theory) and we suggest that certain cognitive effects arise in communication thanks to affective effects, which then act as attention attractors and boosters for optimally relevant cognitive effects. We show that, sometimes, affect can win out over the non-affective side of cognition and also that at least some poetic artefacts may activate ‘pure affective effects’, which can be relevant in their own right, i.e. relevant without cognitive effects.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42572806","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Expansion-relevant final but for preference organisation 与扩展相关的期末考试,但优先组织
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-19 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202005
Kazuki Hata
This article focuses on the utilisation of but in English at turn-final placement regarding provisions for what follows next, where the token is not a display of a traditional sense of content-level contrasts. The production of final buts in this article is a point of expansion relevance and emergent as a means of intersubjectively creating another opportunity space to deal with the ongoing disaffiliation or lack of resources. My observations particularly unpack the contextual properties of final buts. First, a but-turn is designed not strictly to show a partial agreement and back down from the original statement. It can be more plausibly argued that the but-turn indicates which resource of interaction is (or is not) requested at that specific moment to accomplish the ongoing agenda. Second, a projected action of reworking can be formulated in collaborative completion with a co-participant explicating the account to the but-turn.
这篇文章的重点是使用但在英语中的最后位置,关于接下来的内容,其中代币不是传统意义上的内容层次对比的展示。本文中最后buts的产生是一个扩展相关性的点,并作为一种主体间创造另一个机会空间的手段而出现,以应对正在进行的脱离或资源缺乏。我的观察特别揭示了最后但是的上下文属性。首先,but转弯的设计并不是为了严格地表示部分同意,并从最初的陈述中后退。可以更合理地说,“但”表示在特定时刻需要(或不需要)哪种互动资源来完成正在进行的议程。其次,返工的计划行动可以在合作完成中制定,由共同参与者向but turn解释说明。
{"title":"Expansion-relevant final but for preference organisation","authors":"Kazuki Hata","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202005","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This article focuses on the utilisation of but in English at turn-final placement regarding provisions for what follows next, where the token is not a display of a traditional sense of content-level contrasts. The production of final buts in this article is a point of expansion relevance and emergent as a means of intersubjectively creating another opportunity space to deal with the ongoing disaffiliation or lack of resources. My observations particularly unpack the contextual properties of final buts. First, a but-turn is designed not strictly to show a partial agreement and back down from the original statement. It can be more plausibly argued that the but-turn indicates which resource of interaction is (or is not) requested at that specific moment to accomplish the ongoing agenda. Second, a projected action of reworking can be formulated in collaborative completion with a co-participant explicating the account to the but-turn.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48930996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Whatever floats your boat 随遇而安
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-19 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01202002
P. Duffley, P. Larrivée
The meaning of indefinites of the wh-ever paradigm has been the object of a number of claims regarding its ignorance and indifference readings. These are examined here in the light of a systematic analysis of 2500 occurrences of the wh-ever paradigm in the Brigham-Young-University TV-corpus. Contrary to general assumptions, ignorance and indifference readings are not separate, as they are found jointly in 27 % of occurrences. Each reading is sensitive to contextual cues: ignorance associates with animacy, and is compatible with plural nouns; indifference associates with independent use, subtrigging by modals and singular nouns. Finally, scalarity is not a dominant dimension of the wh-items’ interpretation, being associated with subtrigging by modal expressions and focus stress, and requiring an indifference reading. These items are defined as evoking an unidentified referent requiring identification belonging to an open-ended series of possibilities.
wh-ever范式的不确定性的含义一直是许多关于其无知和冷漠解读的主张的对象。本文通过对杨百翰大学电视语料库中2500次wh-ever范式的系统分析,对这些现象进行了研究。与一般假设相反,无知和漠不关心的读数并不是分开的,因为它们在27中共同发现 % 发生的次数。每一次阅读都对上下文线索敏感:无知与动物性联系在一起,与复数名词兼容;冷漠与独立使用、情态动词和单数名词的省略有关。最后,标量不是wh项目解释的主要维度,它与模态表达和焦点重音的减法有关,并且需要无差别阅读。这些项目被定义为唤起一个未识别的指称,需要识别,属于一系列开放的可能性。
{"title":"Whatever floats your boat","authors":"P. Duffley, P. Larrivée","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01202002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01202002","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The meaning of indefinites of the wh-ever paradigm has been the object of a number of claims regarding its ignorance and indifference readings. These are examined here in the light of a systematic analysis of 2500 occurrences of the wh-ever paradigm in the Brigham-Young-University TV-corpus. Contrary to general assumptions, ignorance and indifference readings are not separate, as they are found jointly in 27 % of occurrences. Each reading is sensitive to contextual cues: ignorance associates with animacy, and is compatible with plural nouns; indifference associates with independent use, subtrigging by modals and singular nouns. Finally, scalarity is not a dominant dimension of the wh-items’ interpretation, being associated with subtrigging by modal expressions and focus stress, and requiring an indifference reading. These items are defined as evoking an unidentified referent requiring identification belonging to an open-ended series of possibilities.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01202002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41937012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Lying, Uptake, Assertion, and Intent 谎言、理解、断言和意图
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-08-15 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-00802006
Angelo Turri, John Turri
A standard view in social science and philosophy is that a lie is a dishonest assertion: to lie is to assert something that you think is false in order to deceive your audience. We report four behavioral experiments designed to evaluate some aspects of this view. Participants read short scenarios and judged several features of interest, including whether an agent lied. We found evidence that ordinary lie attributions can be influenced by aspects of audience uptake, are based on judging that the agent made an assertion (assertion attributions), and, at least in some contexts, are not based on attributions of deceptive intent. The finding on assertion attributions is predicted by the standard view, but the finding on intent attributions is not. These results help to further clarify the ordinary concept of lying and shed light on the psychological processes involved in ordinary lie attributions and related judgments.
社会科学和哲学的一个标准观点是,谎言是一种不诚实的断言:撒谎是为了欺骗你的听众而断言你认为是错误的东西。我们报告了四个行为实验,旨在评估这一观点的某些方面。参与者阅读简短的场景,并判断几个感兴趣的特征,包括代理人是否撒谎。我们发现有证据表明,普通的谎言归因会受到受众吸收方面的影响,这些归因是基于对行为人做出断言的判断(断言归因),而且,至少在某些情况下,不是基于欺骗意图的归因。标准观点可以预测断言归因的结果,但不能预测意图归因的结果。这些结果有助于进一步澄清谎言的普通概念,并揭示了涉及普通谎言归因和相关判断的心理过程。
{"title":"Lying, Uptake, Assertion, and Intent","authors":"Angelo Turri, John Turri","doi":"10.1163/18773109-00802006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-00802006","url":null,"abstract":"A standard view in social science and philosophy is that a lie is a dishonest assertion: to lie is to assert something that you think is false in order to deceive your audience. We report four behavioral experiments designed to evaluate some aspects of this view. Participants read short scenarios and judged several features of interest, including whether an agent lied. We found evidence that ordinary lie attributions can be influenced by aspects of audience uptake, are based on judging that the agent made an assertion (assertion attributions), and, at least in some contexts, are not based on attributions of deceptive intent. The finding on assertion attributions is predicted by the standard view, but the finding on intent attributions is not. These results help to further clarify the ordinary concept of lying and shed light on the psychological processes involved in ordinary lie attributions and related judgments.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-00802006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44854308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The combination of Discourse Markers in Persian 波斯语语篇标记语的组合
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-13 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01201102
Gholamreza Kassaei, M. Amouzadeh
This paper sets out to investigate the ways in which some of the Discourse Markers (DM s) in Persian are used by looking at a corpus of 475 million words. By adopting Fraser’s notion of DM (2009), it will analyse all possible combinations of thirty DMs categorized into three groups: contrastive, elaborative, and inferential. This categorization will be based on the types of semantic relationship they signal between the propositions of the discourse segments preceding and following them. Their deployment in the attested data demonstrates that the ordering of these DM s is by no means arbitrary. The result of our investigation also reveals that Persian contrastive DM s show a strong tendency to combine with the members of their own category, while elaborative DM s tend to combine with inferential DM s. Inferential DM s, too, have a tendency for intra-category combinations; however, such combinations are much less frequent than those of contrastive DM s. Contrastive DM s have the lowest predisposition for combining with inferential DM s. In short, by exploring the frequency of all sequences of DM s under investigation, a hierarchy of DM combinations in Persian will be proposed, which can be argued to predict certain possible configurations of DM sequences in Persian, and such empirical findings will build up some basis for future typological research as well as for the theorization of DM sequences in general.
本文通过对一个包含4.75亿词的语料库的研究,探讨了一些语篇标记(DM )在波斯语中的使用方式。通过采用弗雷泽的DM(2009)的概念,它将分析30个DM的所有可能组合,分为三组:对比,阐述和推理。这种分类将基于它们之前和之后的话语段的命题之间的语义关系的类型。它们在已证实的数据中的部署表明,这些DM s的顺序绝不是任意的。我们的调查结果还显示,波斯语对比型DM 与自己类别的成员结合的趋势很强,而精细化型DM 与推理型DM 结合的趋势很强。推论DM s也有类别内组合的倾向;然而,这种合并比对比型糖尿病 少得多。对比型糖尿病 合并推断型糖尿病 的易感性最低。简而言之,通过研究所有DM 序列的频率,将提出波斯语中DM组合的层次结构,可以预测波斯语中DM序列的某些可能配置,并且这些实证研究结果将为未来的类型学研究以及一般DM序列的理论化建立一些基础。
{"title":"The combination of Discourse Markers in Persian","authors":"Gholamreza Kassaei, M. Amouzadeh","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01201102","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01201102","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This paper sets out to investigate the ways in which some of the Discourse Markers (DM s) in Persian are used by looking at a corpus of 475 million words. By adopting Fraser’s notion of DM (2009), it will analyse all possible combinations of thirty DMs categorized into three groups: contrastive, elaborative, and inferential. This categorization will be based on the types of semantic relationship they signal between the propositions of the discourse segments preceding and following them. Their deployment in the attested data demonstrates that the ordering of these DM s is by no means arbitrary. The result of our investigation also reveals that Persian contrastive DM s show a strong tendency to combine with the members of their own category, while elaborative DM s tend to combine with inferential DM s. Inferential DM s, too, have a tendency for intra-category combinations; however, such combinations are much less frequent than those of contrastive DM s. Contrastive DM s have the lowest predisposition for combining with inferential DM s. In short, by exploring the frequency of all sequences of DM s under investigation, a hierarchy of DM combinations in Persian will be proposed, which can be argued to predict certain possible configurations of DM sequences in Persian, and such empirical findings will build up some basis for future typological research as well as for the theorization of DM sequences in general.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01201102","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45256768","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Vague language challenged 模糊的语言受到挑战
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-13 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01201104
Grace Zhang
Existing studies focus on the effectiveness of vague language (VL). This study offers a balanced account by highlighting the ineffectiveness of VL. Drawn from institutional data involving the interactions between Australian custom officers and passengers, this study finds that while VL was effective in most cases, it was challenged in 8 % of cases. The data reveals a correlation: the more severe a situation is, the more VL is challenged. Officers performed the most VL challenging when carrying out their institutional duties, and passengers challenged VL to clarify information. Half of the responses to the VL challenge produced the required precise information. Non-compliance occurred because of either no information being available (mostly by officers, non-purposive vagueness) or withholding information (mostly by passengers, purposive vagueness). VL serves both cooperative and competitive purposes depending on the needs of the speaker. It is a double-edged sword and can both facilitate and hinder the proper use of language. The implication is that the acceptance of VL is not universal, and it requires contextual suitability to avoid communication breakdowns.
现有的研究主要集中在模糊语言的有效性上。这项研究通过强调VL的无效提供了一个平衡的解释。从涉及澳大利亚海关官员和乘客之间互动的机构数据中得出,这项研究发现,虽然VL在大多数情况下是有效的,但在8. %的情况下,它受到了挑战。数据揭示了一种相关性:情况越严重,VL受到的挑战就越大。官员在执行其机构职责时表现出最大的VL挑战,而乘客则在澄清信息时挑战VL。对VL挑战的一半回答产生了所需的精确信息。不遵守规定的原因要么是没有信息(主要是官员提供的,无目的含糊),要么是隐瞒信息(主要是乘客提供的,有目的含糊)。根据说话者的需要,VL既有合作的目的,也有竞争的目的。它是一把双刃剑,既能促进语言的正确使用,也能阻碍语言的正确使用。这意味着对VL的接受并不是普遍的,它需要上下文适应性来避免沟通中断。
{"title":"Vague language challenged","authors":"Grace Zhang","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01201104","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01201104","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Existing studies focus on the effectiveness of vague language (VL). This study offers a balanced account by highlighting the ineffectiveness of VL. Drawn from institutional data involving the interactions between Australian custom officers and passengers, this study finds that while VL was effective in most cases, it was challenged in 8 % of cases. The data reveals a correlation: the more severe a situation is, the more VL is challenged. Officers performed the most VL challenging when carrying out their institutional duties, and passengers challenged VL to clarify information. Half of the responses to the VL challenge produced the required precise information. Non-compliance occurred because of either no information being available (mostly by officers, non-purposive vagueness) or withholding information (mostly by passengers, purposive vagueness). VL serves both cooperative and competitive purposes depending on the needs of the speaker. It is a double-edged sword and can both facilitate and hinder the proper use of language. The implication is that the acceptance of VL is not universal, and it requires contextual suitability to avoid communication breakdowns.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01201104","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44952064","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Manner implicatures and how to spot them 礼仪含义及其识别方法
IF 0.8 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-13 DOI: 10.1163/18773109-01201105
J. Rett
The goal of this paper is to help develop a general picture of conversational implicature (Grice, 1975) by looking beyond scalar implicature to see how the phenomenon behaves in a general sense. I focus on non-scalar Quantity implicatures and Manner implicatures. I review canonical examples of Manner implicature, as well as a more recent, productive one involving gradable adjective antonym pairs (Rett, 2015). Based on these data, I argue that Manner implicatures—and conversational implicatures generally—are distinguishable primarily by their calculability; their reinforceability; their discourse sensitivity (to the Question Under Discussion; Roberts, 1990; van Kuppevelt, 1995; Simons et al., 2011); and their embeddability (under negation, propositional attitude verbs, quantifiers, etc.). I use these data to draw conclusions about the usefulness of implicature-specific operators and about ways to compositionally represent conversational implicatures.
本文的目的是通过超越标量含义来观察这种现象在一般意义上的表现,从而帮助发展会话含义的全貌(Grice,1975)。我关注的是非标量含义和方式含义。我回顾了礼貌含义的典型例子,以及最近一个涉及可分级形容词反义词对的富有成效的例子(Rett,2015)。基于这些数据,我认为礼貌含义——以及一般的会话含义——主要通过它们的可计算性来区分;它们的强化性;他们的话语敏感性(对讨论中的问题;Roberts,1990年;van Kuppevelt,1995年;Simons等人,2011年);以及它们的可嵌入性(在否定、命题态度动词、量词等下)。我利用这些数据得出了关于特定含义运算符的有用性以及关于会话含义的合成表示方法的结论。
{"title":"Manner implicatures and how to spot them","authors":"J. Rett","doi":"10.1163/18773109-01201105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-01201105","url":null,"abstract":"The goal of this paper is to help develop a general picture of conversational implicature (Grice, 1975) by looking beyond scalar implicature to see how the phenomenon behaves in a general sense. I focus on non-scalar Quantity implicatures and Manner implicatures. I review canonical examples of Manner implicature, as well as a more recent, productive one involving gradable adjective antonym pairs (Rett, 2015). Based on these data, I argue that Manner implicatures—and conversational implicatures generally—are distinguishable primarily by their calculability; their reinforceability; their discourse sensitivity (to the Question Under Discussion; Roberts, 1990; van Kuppevelt, 1995; Simons et al., 2011); and their embeddability (under negation, propositional attitude verbs, quantifiers, etc.). I use these data to draw conclusions about the usefulness of implicature-specific operators and about ways to compositionally represent conversational implicatures.","PeriodicalId":43536,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Pragmatics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2020-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1163/18773109-01201105","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46225839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
期刊
International Review of Pragmatics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1