首页 > 最新文献

International Review of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
“Sorry for Congo, Let’s Make Amends”: Belgians’ Ideological Worldviews Predict Attitudes Towards Apology and Reparation for its Colonial Past “对不起刚果,让我们赔罪”:比利时人的意识形态世界观预测了他们对殖民历史的道歉和赔偿的态度
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-08-20 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.486
Jasper Van Assche, Arne Roets, T. Haesevoets, Masi Noor
In light of the recent steps Belgium has made towards reconciling with its colonial history in Congo (e.g., the King’s letter of regret, and the removal of some colonial statues), we examined how Belgians differ in their attitudes towards an official apology, towards (symbolic) reparations (e.g., renaming streets), and towards raising more awareness about the colonial past (e.g., more detailed information in educational books) as a function of their ideological worldviews. We hypothesized that authoritarianism and group dominance would negatively predict these outcome variables, while particularly universal-diverse orientation and egalitarianism would predict them positively. We further hypothesized that these relations would be mediated by relevant intergroup emotions (i.e., group-based guilt, shame, and anger, and especially outgroup empathy). Path model analyses on a sample of 258 Flemish-speaking students provided support for our predictions: (1) universal-diverse orientation was the strongest predictor of all intergroup emotions, (2) empathy—and to a lesser extent group-based anger—were the strongest direct predictors of attitudes towards apologizing, reparation, and awareness, and (3) empathy was the most important mediator explaining the associations of universal-diverse and egalitarian orientations with the outcomes. We discuss the implications of our findings for the current debate regarding reconciliation between groups with a history of colonialism.
鉴于比利时最近为调和其在刚果的殖民历史所采取的步骤(例如,国王的遗憾信,以及拆除一些殖民雕像),我们研究了比利时人对官方道歉、对(象征性)赔偿(例如,重新命名街道)、,以及提高人们对殖民历史的更多认识(例如,在教育书籍中提供更详细的信息),作为他们意识形态世界观的一个功能。我们假设威权主义和群体主导会对这些结果变量产生负面预测,而特别是普遍多样性取向和平等主义会对它们产生正面预测。我们进一步假设,这些关系将由相关的群体间情绪(即基于群体的内疚、羞耻和愤怒,尤其是群体外移情)介导。对258名讲佛兰德语的学生样本进行的路径模型分析为我们的预测提供了支持:(1)普遍的多样化取向是所有群体间情绪的最强预测因子,(2)同理心——以及在较小程度上基于群体的愤怒——是对道歉、赔偿和意识态度的最强直接预测因子,同理心是解释普遍多样性和平等主义取向与结果之间关系的最重要中介。我们讨论了我们的调查结果对当前关于有殖民主义历史的群体之间和解的辩论的影响。
{"title":"“Sorry for Congo, Let’s Make Amends”: Belgians’ Ideological Worldviews Predict Attitudes Towards Apology and Reparation for its Colonial Past","authors":"Jasper Van Assche, Arne Roets, T. Haesevoets, Masi Noor","doi":"10.5334/irsp.486","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.486","url":null,"abstract":"In light of the recent steps Belgium has made towards reconciling with its colonial history in Congo (e.g., the King’s letter of regret, and the removal of some colonial statues), we examined how Belgians differ in their attitudes towards an official apology, towards (symbolic) reparations (e.g., renaming streets), and towards raising more awareness about the colonial past (e.g., more detailed information in educational books) as a function of their ideological worldviews. We hypothesized that authoritarianism and group dominance would negatively predict these outcome variables, while particularly universal-diverse orientation and egalitarianism would predict them positively. We further hypothesized that these relations would be mediated by relevant intergroup emotions (i.e., group-based guilt, shame, and anger, and especially outgroup empathy). Path model analyses on a sample of 258 Flemish-speaking students provided support for our predictions: (1) universal-diverse orientation was the strongest predictor of all intergroup emotions, (2) empathy—and to a lesser extent group-based anger—were the strongest direct predictors of attitudes towards apologizing, reparation, and awareness, and (3) empathy was the most important mediator explaining the associations of universal-diverse and egalitarian orientations with the outcomes. We discuss the implications of our findings for the current debate regarding reconciliation between groups with a history of colonialism.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48945243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Talking About Temperature and Social Thermoregulation in the Languages of the World 从世界语言谈温度与社会温度调节
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-08-12 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.410
Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm, D. Nikolaev
The last decade saw rapid growth of the body of work devoted to relations between social thermoregulation and various other domains, with a particular focus on the connection between prosociality and physical warmth. This paper reports on a first systematic cross-linguistic study of the exponents of conceptual metaphor AFFECTION IS WARMTH (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Grady, 1997), which provides the motivation for the large share of research in this area. Assumed to be universal, it enables researchers, mostly speakers of major European languages, to treat words like warm and cold as self-evident and easily translatable between languages – both in their concrete uses (to feel warm/cold) and as applied to interpersonal relationships (a cold/warm person, warm feelings, etc.). Based on a sample of 94 languages from all around the world and using methodology borrowed from typological linguistics and mixed-effects regression modelling, we show that the relevant expressions show a remarkably skewed distribution and seem to be absent or extremely marginal in the majority of language families and linguistic macro-areas. The study demonstrates once again the dramatic influence of the Anglocentric, Standard Average European, and WEIRD perspectives on many of the central concepts and conclusions in linguistics, psychology, and cognitive research and discusses how changing this perspective can impact research in social psychology in general and in social thermoregulation in particular.
在过去的十年中,致力于社会体温调节与其他各个领域之间关系的研究工作迅速增长,特别关注亲社会性与体温之间的联系。本文首次系统地跨语言研究了概念隐喻“情感即温暖”(Lakoff & Johnson, 1980;Grady, 1997),这为这一领域的大量研究提供了动力。假设是普遍的,它使研究人员,主要是欧洲主要语言的使用者,将warm和cold等词视为不言而喻的,易于在语言之间翻译-无论是在其具体用途(感觉温暖/寒冷)还是应用于人际关系(冷/暖的人,温暖的感觉等)。基于来自世界各地的94种语言样本,并使用类型学语言学和混合效应回归模型的方法,我们发现相关表达在大多数语族和语言宏观领域中表现出明显的倾斜分布,似乎不存在或极其边缘。该研究再次证明了盎格鲁中心主义、欧洲标准平均主义和WEIRD观点对语言学、心理学和认知研究中的许多核心概念和结论的巨大影响,并讨论了改变这种观点如何影响社会心理学的研究,特别是社会体温调节。
{"title":"Talking About Temperature and Social Thermoregulation in the Languages of the World","authors":"Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm, D. Nikolaev","doi":"10.5334/irsp.410","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.410","url":null,"abstract":"The last decade saw rapid growth of the body of work devoted to relations between social thermoregulation and various other domains, with a particular focus on the connection between prosociality and physical warmth. This paper reports on a first systematic cross-linguistic study of the exponents of conceptual metaphor AFFECTION IS WARMTH (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Grady, 1997), which provides the motivation for the large share of research in this area. Assumed to be universal, it enables researchers, mostly speakers of major European languages, to treat words like warm and cold as self-evident and easily translatable between languages – both in their concrete uses (to feel warm/cold) and as applied to interpersonal relationships (a cold/warm person, warm feelings, etc.). Based on a sample of 94 languages from all around the world and using methodology borrowed from typological linguistics and mixed-effects regression modelling, we show that the relevant expressions show a remarkably skewed distribution and seem to be absent or extremely marginal in the majority of language families and linguistic macro-areas. The study demonstrates once again the dramatic influence of the Anglocentric, Standard Average European, and WEIRD perspectives on many of the central concepts and conclusions in linguistics, psychology, and cognitive research and discusses how changing this perspective can impact research in social psychology in general and in social thermoregulation in particular.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45198916","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Political Extremism and Perceived Anomie: New Evidence of Political Extremes’ Symmetries and Asymmetries Within French Samples 政治极端主义与感知缺失:法国样本中政治极端主义对称与不对称的新证据
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-08-10 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.573
O. Ionescu, J. Tavani, J. Collange
The present research aimed to extend the existing literature on political extremes’ symmetries and asymmetries, by examining the relationship between political extremism and perceived societal anomie (i.e., perceptions that the leadership is disregulated and that the social fabric is disintegrated) across three studies conducted within French samples. The first two studies revealed that perceived leadership disregulation increased as participants position themselves toward either political extreme and was associated with greater support for proposals regarding protectionism and economic redistribution; indicating a symmetry between political extremes. However, perceived social fabric disintegration was not associated with political extremism but was stronger for far-right individuals; thus suggesting an asymmetry between the extremes. The last study, conducted before the 2017 French Presidential elections, showed that, when imagining a future society in which the candidate they supported is elected as President, political extremes, compared to moderates, similarly reported reduced levels of leadership disregulation, but not social fabric disintegration. The present findings therefore suggest both similarities and differences between political extremes’ perception of anomie within current and future French society and extend previous work showing that political extremes share negative views of society.
本研究旨在扩展现有关于政治极端对称性和不对称性的文献,通过在法国样本中进行的三项研究,研究政治极端主义与感知到的社会失范(即领导层不受监管和社会结构解体的感知)之间的关系。前两项研究表明,当参与者将自己定位于政治极端时,感知到的领导失调会增加,并且与更大程度上支持有关保护主义和经济再分配的建议有关;表明政治极端之间的对称。然而,感知到的社会结构解体与政治极端主义无关,但在极右翼个体中更为强烈;这就暗示了极端之间的不对称。最后一项研究是在2017年法国总统选举前进行的,研究表明,在想象一个他们支持的候选人当选总统的未来社会时,与温和派相比,政治极端分子同样报告说,领导失调程度有所降低,但社会结构没有解体。因此,目前的研究结果表明,在当前和未来的法国社会中,政治极端分子对社会失范的看法既有相似之处,也有不同之处,并扩展了以前的工作,表明政治极端分子对社会有负面看法。
{"title":"Political Extremism and Perceived Anomie: New Evidence of Political Extremes’ Symmetries and Asymmetries Within French Samples","authors":"O. Ionescu, J. Tavani, J. Collange","doi":"10.5334/irsp.573","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.573","url":null,"abstract":"The present research aimed to extend the existing literature on political extremes’ symmetries and asymmetries, by examining the relationship between political extremism and perceived societal anomie (i.e., perceptions that the leadership is disregulated and that the social fabric is disintegrated) across three studies conducted within French samples. The first two studies revealed that perceived leadership disregulation increased as participants position themselves toward either political extreme and was associated with greater support for proposals regarding protectionism and economic redistribution; indicating a symmetry between political extremes. However, perceived social fabric disintegration was not associated with political extremism but was stronger for far-right individuals; thus suggesting an asymmetry between the extremes. The last study, conducted before the 2017 French Presidential elections, showed that, when imagining a future society in which the candidate they supported is elected as President, political extremes, compared to moderates, similarly reported reduced levels of leadership disregulation, but not social fabric disintegration. The present findings therefore suggest both similarities and differences between political extremes’ perception of anomie within current and future French society and extend previous work showing that political extremes share negative views of society.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47068671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
To Apologize or to Compensate for Colonial Injustices? The Role of Representations of the Colonial Past, Group-Based Guilt, and In-Group Identification 为殖民时期的不公正道歉还是赔偿?殖民历史再现的角色、群体负罪感和群体内认同
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-08-10 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.484
Borja Martinović, Karen Freihorst, Magdalena Bobowik
In the aftermath of an intergroup conflict, along with instrumental reparations, victims may request an apology on behalf of the perpetrators, yet such political apologies are often not given. Whereas we know a lot about the motivations of the victims, less is known about when and why the perpetrators are willing to apologize. In this study, from the perspective of the perpetrator group, we simultaneously examined and compared support for a political apology and for instrumental reparations (e.g., financial support and other forms of assistance) offered to both former colonies and colonial-origin minorities living in the country responsible for the past colonization. We considered the indirect role of positive and negative representations of the colonial past via feelings of group-based guilt. Using a community sample of the native Dutch population (N = 763), we showed that the Dutch were more supportive of instrumental reparations than of political apology. They also agreed with both the positive and negative aspects of their colonial past, but they did not experience much collective guilt. Agreement with positive representations of the Dutch colonial past was, via weaker feelings of group-based guilt, related to less support for both political apology and instrumental reparations. In contrast, negative representations of the past were, via higher guilt, related to more support for these reconciliatory outcomes. These processes were similar for higher and lower in-group identifiers. Importantly, the association between guilt and support for political apology was twice as large as the one between guilt and support for instrumental reparations, suggesting that political apology is more effective in restoring the in-group’s moral self-image.
在群体间冲突之后,除了工具性赔偿外,受害者还可以代表肇事者要求道歉,但这种政治道歉往往得不到。尽管我们对受害者的动机了解很多,但对肇事者何时以及为什么愿意道歉却知之甚少。在这项研究中,从犯罪者群体的角度,我们同时审查并比较了对政治道歉和工具性赔偿(如财政支持和其他形式的援助)的支持,这些赔偿是向居住在对过去殖民负有责任的国家的前殖民地和殖民地少数民族提供的。我们通过基于群体的内疚感来考虑殖民历史的积极和消极表征的间接作用。使用荷兰本土人口的社区样本(N=763),我们发现荷兰人更支持工具性赔偿,而不是政治道歉。他们也同意殖民历史的积极和消极方面,但他们没有经历太多的集体内疚。对荷兰殖民历史的积极表述的认同,由于基于群体的内疚感减弱,与对政治道歉和工具性赔偿的支持减少有关。相反,通过更高的内疚感,对过去的负面描述与对这些和解结果的更多支持有关。对于较高和较低的组内标识符,这些过程是相似的。重要的是,内疚和支持政治道歉之间的关联是内疚和支持工具性赔偿之间的关联的两倍,这表明政治道歉在恢复群体道德自我形象方面更有效。
{"title":"To Apologize or to Compensate for Colonial Injustices? The Role of Representations of the Colonial Past, Group-Based Guilt, and In-Group Identification","authors":"Borja Martinović, Karen Freihorst, Magdalena Bobowik","doi":"10.5334/irsp.484","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.484","url":null,"abstract":"In the aftermath of an intergroup conflict, along with instrumental reparations, victims may request an apology on behalf of the perpetrators, yet such political apologies are often not given. Whereas we know a lot about the motivations of the victims, less is known about when and why the perpetrators are willing to apologize. In this study, from the perspective of the perpetrator group, we simultaneously examined and compared support for a political apology and for instrumental reparations (e.g., financial support and other forms of assistance) offered to both former colonies and colonial-origin minorities living in the country responsible for the past colonization. We considered the indirect role of positive and negative representations of the colonial past via feelings of group-based guilt. Using a community sample of the native Dutch population (N = 763), we showed that the Dutch were more supportive of instrumental reparations than of political apology. They also agreed with both the positive and negative aspects of their colonial past, but they did not experience much collective guilt. Agreement with positive representations of the Dutch colonial past was, via weaker feelings of group-based guilt, related to less support for both political apology and instrumental reparations. In contrast, negative representations of the past were, via higher guilt, related to more support for these reconciliatory outcomes. These processes were similar for higher and lower in-group identifiers. Importantly, the association between guilt and support for political apology was twice as large as the one between guilt and support for instrumental reparations, suggesting that political apology is more effective in restoring the in-group’s moral self-image.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42203609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Men and Women Defending Themselves from Political Scandals: Gender Stereotypes and Proneness to Forgive Scandalous Politicians 男性和女性在政治丑闻中自我保护:性别刻板印象和原谅丑闻政客的倾向
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-08-02 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.463
Silvia Cucchi, A. Graziani, Margherita Guidetti, N. Cavazza
Many studies have highlighted the role of gender stereotypes in impressions of politicians. In general, a politician’s image benefits from behaving consistently within gender stereotypes. The present study tested whether this also applied to different image restoration tactics employed by male versus female politicians after a scandal. We assessed participants’ evaluation of a fictitious male or female politician soon after a scandal and then after his or her defence. We used a 3 × 2 experimental design to compare the effectiveness of three defensive tactics involving different degrees of assertiveness vs. submissiveness as a function of a politician’s gender. All tactics were overall effective in improving the damaged reputation of the fictitious politician but, as expected, using the excuse of mitigating circumstances (submissive) was more beneficial to female politicians while diffusing responsibility by accusing another person (assertive) was more beneficial to male politicians, both in terms of global attitude and stereotypical evaluations (communality and agency, respectively). The tactic of mortification and requests for forgiveness did not have different effects by gender. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
许多研究都强调了性别刻板印象在政治家印象中的作用。一般来说,政治家的形象得益于其一贯的性别刻板印象。本研究测试了这是否也适用于丑闻后男性和女性政治家所采用的不同形象恢复策略。我们评估了参与者在丑闻发生后不久对虚构的男性或女性政治家的评价,然后在他或她的辩护之后。我们使用了一个3 × 2的实验设计来比较三种防御策略的有效性,包括不同程度的自信和顺从,作为政治家性别的函数。所有策略在改善虚构政治家受损的声誉方面总体上都是有效的,但正如预期的那样,使用减轻情节的借口(顺从)对女性政治家更有利,而通过指责另一个人来分散责任(自信)对男性政治家更有利,无论是在全球态度方面还是在刻板印象评价方面(分别是共同体和代理)。羞辱和请求原谅的策略并没有因性别而有不同的效果。讨论了理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Men and Women Defending Themselves from Political Scandals: Gender Stereotypes and Proneness to Forgive Scandalous Politicians","authors":"Silvia Cucchi, A. Graziani, Margherita Guidetti, N. Cavazza","doi":"10.5334/irsp.463","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.463","url":null,"abstract":"Many studies have highlighted the role of gender stereotypes in impressions of politicians. In general, a politician’s image benefits from behaving consistently within gender stereotypes. The present study tested whether this also applied to different image restoration tactics employed by male versus female politicians after a scandal. We assessed participants’ evaluation of a fictitious male or female politician soon after a scandal and then after his or her defence. We used a 3 × 2 experimental design to compare the effectiveness of three defensive tactics involving different degrees of assertiveness vs. submissiveness as a function of a politician’s gender. All tactics were overall effective in improving the damaged reputation of the fictitious politician but, as expected, using the excuse of mitigating circumstances (submissive) was more beneficial to female politicians while diffusing responsibility by accusing another person (assertive) was more beneficial to male politicians, both in terms of global attitude and stereotypical evaluations (communality and agency, respectively). The tactic of mortification and requests for forgiveness did not have different effects by gender. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48110197","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Using the Self-Referencing Task to Produce Durable Change on Food Evaluations Measured via the IAT 使用自我参考任务对通过IAT测量的食品评估产生持久变化
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-07-29 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.446
S. Mattavelli, J. Richetin, M. Perugini
In many daily life situations, eating behaviour manifests itself under conditions of automaticity. Associative learning procedures have proven reliable to change food items’ evaluations inferred from performances in indirect tasks, such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Targeting two alternative food brands, we investigated the impact of the Self-Referencing (SR) task on IAT performances measured immediately after the manipulation and one week later. Capitalizing on the structural features of the SR task, a first study (N = 145) demonstrated the durable effect of the manipulation on the IAT. An advantage in automatic responding for the snack brand paired with the self was detected right after the SR task and one week later. Instead, the SR manipulation showed no impact on self-reported evaluations. Moreover, a semantic priming task administered right after the SR task indicated response facilitation for the self-related target brand when self-stimuli were used as primes. Experiment 2 (N = 268, pre-registered) targeted two alternative food brands and replicated the lasting effect on the IAT, thus demonstrating the generalizability of the effect. Moreover, we extended the results’ validity by showing that SR can generate a durable effect on a behavioural choice task. We discussed the effectiveness of the SR in producing lasting effects on the IAT and other instances of automatic behavior and the potential implications for research in the food domain.
在许多日常生活中,饮食行为是在自律的条件下表现出来的。事实证明,联想学习程序可以可靠地改变从间接任务(如内隐联想测试)中的表现推断出的食物项目的评价。针对两个替代食品品牌,我们调查了自我参考(SR)任务对操纵后立即和一周后测量的IAT表现的影响。利用SR任务的结构特征,第一项研究(N=145)证明了操纵对IAT的持久影响。在SR任务完成后和一周后,发现零食品牌与自我配对在自动反应方面具有优势。相反,SR操作对自我报告的评估没有影响。此外,在SR任务之后立即进行的语义启动任务表明,当自我刺激被用作启动时,对自我相关目标品牌的反应有促进作用。实验2(N=268,预先注册)针对两个替代食品品牌,复制了对IAT的持久影响,从而证明了这种影响的可推广性。此外,我们通过证明SR可以对行为选择任务产生持久的影响来扩展结果的有效性。我们讨论了SR对IAT和其他自动行为产生持久影响的有效性,以及对食品领域研究的潜在影响。
{"title":"Using the Self-Referencing Task to Produce Durable Change on Food Evaluations Measured via the IAT","authors":"S. Mattavelli, J. Richetin, M. Perugini","doi":"10.5334/irsp.446","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.446","url":null,"abstract":"In many daily life situations, eating behaviour manifests itself under conditions of automaticity. Associative learning procedures have proven reliable to change food items’ evaluations inferred from performances in indirect tasks, such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Targeting two alternative food brands, we investigated the impact of the Self-Referencing (SR) task on IAT performances measured immediately after the manipulation and one week later. Capitalizing on the structural features of the SR task, a first study (N = 145) demonstrated the durable effect of the manipulation on the IAT. An advantage in automatic responding for the snack brand paired with the self was detected right after the SR task and one week later. Instead, the SR manipulation showed no impact on self-reported evaluations. Moreover, a semantic priming task administered right after the SR task indicated response facilitation for the self-related target brand when self-stimuli were used as primes. Experiment 2 (N = 268, pre-registered) targeted two alternative food brands and replicated the lasting effect on the IAT, thus demonstrating the generalizability of the effect. Moreover, we extended the results’ validity by showing that SR can generate a durable effect on a behavioural choice task. We discussed the effectiveness of the SR in producing lasting effects on the IAT and other instances of automatic behavior and the potential implications for research in the food domain.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46583263","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mitigating the Default? The Influence of Ingroup Diversity on Outgroup Trust 减轻违约?内群体多样性对外群体信任的影响
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-07-12 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.520
Kevin Winter, K. Sassenberg
Maintaining social cohesion in times of increasing diversity is a major challenge of modern societies. Mitigating defaults in group-based trust could be a solution because they are often driven by stereotypes and ingroup favoritism. Ingroup diversity could be a means to achieve such a mitigation given that it increases cognitive flexibility and cognitive flexibility changes defaults in trust – in the sense of increasing low and reducing high trust. We tested whether representing one’s ingroup as high (compared to low) in diversity mitigates defaults in group-based trust using a variety of well-established manipulations of ingroup diversity and measures of trust. None of the four well-powered studies (total N = 885), we conducted provided support for our hypothesis. However, an internal meta-analysis revealed a significant but very small effect in support of our prediction (r = 0.07, 95% CI [0.01, 0.14]). Thus, a diverse representation of the ingroup asserts a mitigating impact on group-based trust, but the size of the effect is very small. Thus, real world interventions should not rely on the current effect.
在日益多样化的时代保持社会凝聚力是现代社会面临的一项重大挑战。减轻基于群体的信任中的默认值可能是一种解决方案,因为它们通常是由刻板印象和群体内偏爱所驱动的。群体内部多样性可能是实现这种缓解的一种手段,因为它增加了认知灵活性,而认知灵活性改变了信任的默认值——在增加低信任和减少高信任的意义上。我们测试了是否代表一个人的内部群体是高的(相对于低的)多样性,使用各种完善的内部群体多样性操作和信任措施减轻了基于群体的信任违约。我们进行的四项有力研究(总N = 885)中没有一项支持我们的假设。然而,内部荟萃分析显示支持我们预测的显著但非常小的影响(r = 0.07, 95% CI[0.01, 0.14])。因此,内部群体的多样化表示对基于群体的信任有缓解影响,但影响的大小非常小。因此,现实世界的干预不应依赖于当前的效果。
{"title":"Mitigating the Default? The Influence of Ingroup Diversity on Outgroup Trust","authors":"Kevin Winter, K. Sassenberg","doi":"10.5334/irsp.520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.520","url":null,"abstract":"Maintaining social cohesion in times of increasing diversity is a major challenge of modern societies. Mitigating defaults in group-based trust could be a solution because they are often driven by stereotypes and ingroup favoritism. Ingroup diversity could be a means to achieve such a mitigation given that it increases cognitive flexibility and cognitive flexibility changes defaults in trust – in the sense of increasing low and reducing high trust. We tested whether representing one’s ingroup as high (compared to low) in diversity mitigates defaults in group-based trust using a variety of well-established manipulations of ingroup diversity and measures of trust. None of the four well-powered studies (total N = 885), we conducted provided support for our hypothesis. However, an internal meta-analysis revealed a significant but very small effect in support of our prediction (r = 0.07, 95% CI [0.01, 0.14]). Thus, a diverse representation of the ingroup asserts a mitigating impact on group-based trust, but the size of the effect is very small. Thus, real world interventions should not rely on the current effect.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44556375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A More Competent, Warm, Feminine, and Human Leader: Perceptions and Effectiveness of Democratic Versus Authoritarian Political Leaders 一个更有能力、更温暖、更女性化、更有人情味的领导人:民主政治领导人与威权政治领导人的看法和有效性
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-07-12 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.452
Mario Sainz, Eva Moreno‐Bella, Laura C. Torres‐Vega
Nowadays, to the detriment of democratic leaders, the emergence of authoritarian leaders has drastically modified the political sphere. This project aims to shed light on this issue by analysing how the perceived effectiveness of democratic and authoritarian political leaders are shaped by the common dimensions of social perception, such as competence/warmth, masculinity/femininity, and human uniqueness/human nature. Accordingly, three experimental studies were conducted. In Study 1 (n = 1001), we revealed that democratic leaders are perceived as more competent, warm, feminine and human. In Study 2 (n = 548) and Study 3 (n = 622), we investigated whether these dimensions of perception mediated the relationship between leaders and their perceived effectiveness. The results revealed that democratic leaders are perceived as effective in cooperative scenarios due to their competence, femininity, and human nature. Alternatively, democratic leaders are preferred in ambiguous contexts due to their competence and cognitive flexibility, that is, human nature. In contrast, authoritarian leaders are perceived as effective in competitive scenarios because of their masculinity. In Study 3, we manipulated the (in)stability of socio-economic contexts. The results revealed that democratic and authoritarian leaders are perceived as more competent, warm, human and more effective in socio-economic contexts that are stable compared with those that are unstable. The implications of the results regarding the emergence of authoritarian leaders are discussed.
如今,独裁领导人的出现极大地改变了政治领域,这对民主领导人不利。该项目旨在通过分析社会认知的共同维度,如能力/热情、男性气质/女性气质以及人类的独特性/人性,如何塑造民主和威权政治领导人的有效性,来阐明这一问题。因此,进行了三项实验研究。在研究1(n=1001)中,我们发现民主领导人被认为更有能力、热情、女性化和人性化。在研究2(n=548)和研究3(n=622)中,我们调查了这些感知维度是否介导了领导者与其感知有效性之间的关系。研究结果表明,由于民主领导人的能力、女性气质和人性,他们在合作场景中被认为是有效的。或者,由于民主领导人的能力和认知灵活性,即人性,他们在模棱两可的情况下更受欢迎。相比之下,威权主义领导人在竞争场景中被认为是有效的,因为他们的男子气概。在研究3中,我们操纵了社会经济环境的稳定性。结果显示,与不稳定的领导人相比,民主和威权领导人在稳定的社会经济环境中被认为更有能力、更有热情、更有人性,也更有效。讨论了结果对独裁领导人出现的影响。
{"title":"A More Competent, Warm, Feminine, and Human Leader: Perceptions and Effectiveness of Democratic Versus Authoritarian Political Leaders","authors":"Mario Sainz, Eva Moreno‐Bella, Laura C. Torres‐Vega","doi":"10.5334/irsp.452","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.452","url":null,"abstract":"Nowadays, to the detriment of democratic leaders, the emergence of authoritarian leaders has drastically modified the political sphere. This project aims to shed light on this issue by analysing how the perceived effectiveness of democratic and authoritarian political leaders are shaped by the common dimensions of social perception, such as competence/warmth, masculinity/femininity, and human uniqueness/human nature. Accordingly, three experimental studies were conducted. In Study 1 (n = 1001), we revealed that democratic leaders are perceived as more competent, warm, feminine and human. In Study 2 (n = 548) and Study 3 (n = 622), we investigated whether these dimensions of perception mediated the relationship between leaders and their perceived effectiveness. The results revealed that democratic leaders are perceived as effective in cooperative scenarios due to their competence, femininity, and human nature. Alternatively, democratic leaders are preferred in ambiguous contexts due to their competence and cognitive flexibility, that is, human nature. In contrast, authoritarian leaders are perceived as effective in competitive scenarios because of their masculinity. In Study 3, we manipulated the (in)stability of socio-economic contexts. The results revealed that democratic and authoritarian leaders are perceived as more competent, warm, human and more effective in socio-economic contexts that are stable compared with those that are unstable. The implications of the results regarding the emergence of authoritarian leaders are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43859659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
These Are Not Just Words: A Cross-National Comparative Study of the Content of Political Apologies 这些不只是文字:政治道歉内容的跨国比较研究
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-07-08 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.503
Marieke Zoodsma, J. Schaafsma, Thia Sagherian-Dickey, Jasper Friedrich
Countries around the world increasingly try to redress past human rights violations by offering apologies. The debates surrounding many of these political apologies suggest they do not necessarily satisfy victims’ needs. Little is known, however, about the actual content of these apologies and the extent to which they include the elements that are often seen as essential to healing processes. In this exploratory study, we conducted a cross-national comparative analysis of the texts of political apologies (N = 203, offered by 50 countries) and coded whether they included a statement of sorry, apology, or regret (IFID), and an acknowledgement of wrongdoing, acceptance of responsibility, promise of non-repetition, promise of reparations, recognition of victim suffering, victim re-inclusion, victim praise, or a recognition of moral values/norms. We found that the majority of political apologies only include a selection of these elements, with some countries offering more comprehensive apologies than others. Most apologies, however, do contain an IFID, an acknowledgment of wrongdoing and a recognition of suffering, although there is variation in how this is expressed. This variation can be linked to the receiving group (i.e., within-country or not), the contentiousness of the apology in a country and – albeit weakly – the cultural context. Based on these findings, we suggest that when considering the impact of political apologies, it is crucial to consider quantity (how many apology components are included) as well as quality (how this is done).
世界各国越来越多地试图通过道歉来纠正过去侵犯人权的行为。围绕许多政治道歉的争论表明,这些道歉不一定能满足受害者的需求。然而,人们对这些道歉的实际内容以及它们在多大程度上包含了通常被视为对愈合过程至关重要的元素知之甚少。在这项探索性研究中,我们对政治道歉文本(N=203,由50个国家提供)进行了跨国家比较分析,并编码它们是否包括道歉、道歉或遗憾声明(IFID),以及承认不当行为、接受责任、承诺不再重复、承诺赔偿、承认受害者痛苦,受害者的重新融入、受害者的赞扬或对道德价值观/规范的认可。我们发现,大多数政治道歉只包括这些要素,一些国家提供的道歉比其他国家更全面。然而,大多数道歉都包含IFID、对不法行为的承认和对痛苦的承认,尽管表达方式各不相同。这种差异可能与接受群体(即国内外)、道歉在一个国家的争议性以及文化背景有关,尽管这种差异很弱。基于这些发现,我们建议,在考虑政治道歉的影响时,至关重要的是要考虑数量(包括多少道歉成分)和质量(如何做到这一点)。
{"title":"These Are Not Just Words: A Cross-National Comparative Study of the Content of Political Apologies","authors":"Marieke Zoodsma, J. Schaafsma, Thia Sagherian-Dickey, Jasper Friedrich","doi":"10.5334/irsp.503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.503","url":null,"abstract":"Countries around the world increasingly try to redress past human rights violations by offering apologies. The debates surrounding many of these political apologies suggest they do not necessarily satisfy victims’ needs. Little is known, however, about the actual content of these apologies and the extent to which they include the elements that are often seen as essential to healing processes. In this exploratory study, we conducted a cross-national comparative analysis of the texts of political apologies (N = 203, offered by 50 countries) and coded whether they included a statement of sorry, apology, or regret (IFID), and an acknowledgement of wrongdoing, acceptance of responsibility, promise of non-repetition, promise of reparations, recognition of victim suffering, victim re-inclusion, victim praise, or a recognition of moral values/norms. We found that the majority of political apologies only include a selection of these elements, with some countries offering more comprehensive apologies than others. Most apologies, however, do contain an IFID, an acknowledgment of wrongdoing and a recognition of suffering, although there is variation in how this is expressed. This variation can be linked to the receiving group (i.e., within-country or not), the contentiousness of the apology in a country and – albeit weakly – the cultural context. Based on these findings, we suggest that when considering the impact of political apologies, it is crucial to consider quantity (how many apology components are included) as well as quality (how this is done).","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47605211","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The Real World is the Ingroup World: A Normative Explanation of Response-Scale Effects in the Subjective Report of Behaviors 真实世界是群体世界——行为主观报告中反应量表效应的规范解释
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-07-02 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.426
F. Lorenzi‐Cioldi, Vincenzo Iacoviello, C. Kulich
The ranges of response alternatives presented in frequency scales influence respondents’ behavioral estimates. This research aimed at complementing the existing cognitive interpretations of this scale effect (e.g., Schwarz, 1994). We propose a normative interpretation, according to which targets associated with generic behavioral norms, and with ingroup norms, lead participants to rely more strongly on the scale’s frequency properties. Studies 1–2 showed stronger scale effects when participants reported behaviors over an extended (vs. short) time period, specifically when they reported behaviors of themselves (vs. people in general). Studies 3–6 showed that the use of a scale’s properties in self-reports increased when participants were led to believe that the scale’s frequency alternatives mirrored typical ingroup (vs. outgroup) behaviors. Finally, Study 7 demonstrated the role of ingroup identification in the production of these scale effects. Collectively, these findings suggest that behavioral estimates based on shared norms override memory scanning when behavior is difficult to retrieve, and when relevant ingroup information is available.
在频率量表中提出的反应选择范围影响被调查者的行为估计。本研究旨在补充现有的对这种规模效应的认知解释(例如,Schwarz, 1994)。我们提出了一种规范性解释,根据这种解释,与一般行为规范和群体内规范相关的目标,导致参与者更强烈地依赖于量表的频率属性。研究1-2表明,当参与者在较长时间内(相对于较短时间)报告行为时,特别是当他们报告自己的行为时(相对于一般人),规模效应更强。研究3-6表明,当参与者被引导相信量表的频率选择反映了典型的内群体(相对于外群体)行为时,量表属性在自我报告中的使用就会增加。最后,研究7证明了内群体认同在这些规模效应的产生中所起的作用。总的来说,这些发现表明,当行为难以检索时,以及当相关的群体信息可用时,基于共享规范的行为估计优于记忆扫描。
{"title":"The Real World is the Ingroup World: A Normative Explanation of Response-Scale Effects in the Subjective Report of Behaviors","authors":"F. Lorenzi‐Cioldi, Vincenzo Iacoviello, C. Kulich","doi":"10.5334/irsp.426","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.426","url":null,"abstract":"The ranges of response alternatives presented in frequency scales influence respondents’ behavioral estimates. This research aimed at complementing the existing cognitive interpretations of this scale effect (e.g., Schwarz, 1994). We propose a normative interpretation, according to which targets associated with generic behavioral norms, and with ingroup norms, lead participants to rely more strongly on the scale’s frequency properties. Studies 1–2 showed stronger scale effects when participants reported behaviors over an extended (vs. short) time period, specifically when they reported behaviors of themselves (vs. people in general). Studies 3–6 showed that the use of a scale’s properties in self-reports increased when participants were led to believe that the scale’s frequency alternatives mirrored typical ingroup (vs. outgroup) behaviors. Finally, Study 7 demonstrated the role of ingroup identification in the production of these scale effects. Collectively, these findings suggest that behavioral estimates based on shared norms override memory scanning when behavior is difficult to retrieve, and when relevant ingroup information is available.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48057303","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
International Review of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1