首页 > 最新文献

Trends in Neuroscience and Education最新文献

英文 中文
The structure of early numeracy: Evidence from multi-factorial models 早期计算能力的结构:来自多因子模型的证据
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2022.100171
Declan Devlin , Korbinian Moeller , Francesco Sella

Background

Despite the relevance of basic numerical skills to later mathematical outcomes, there remains little consensus on which skills comprise the construct of early numeracy.

Procedure

Here, we evaluate recent studies that investigated this construct using various approaches.

Main findings

Findings from these studies indicated broad consensus in considering skills such as counting, number relations, and basic arithmetic, as central aspects of early numeracy. However, both the number and contents of identified factors varied considerably across studies.

Conclusions

Although this review substantiated the critical relevance of certain basic numerical skills, it also highlights a need for future research to evaluate the structure of early numeracy in a more systematic and coordinated way to increase comparability and coherence across studies.

尽管基本的数字技能与后来的数学结果相关,但关于哪些技能构成了早期计算能力的构建,目前还没有达成共识。在这里,我们评估了最近使用各种方法研究这种结构的研究。这些研究的发现表明,人们普遍认为计数、数字关系和基本算术等技能是早期算术能力的核心方面。然而,在不同的研究中,确定因素的数量和内容都有很大的不同。尽管这篇综述证实了某些基本计算技能的关键相关性,但它也强调了未来研究需要以更系统和协调的方式评估早期计算能力的结构,以增加研究之间的可比性和一致性。
{"title":"The structure of early numeracy: Evidence from multi-factorial models","authors":"Declan Devlin ,&nbsp;Korbinian Moeller ,&nbsp;Francesco Sella","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2022.100171","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2022.100171","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Despite the relevance of basic numerical skills to later mathematical outcomes, there remains little consensus on which skills comprise the construct of early numeracy.</p></div><div><h3>Procedure</h3><p>Here, we evaluate recent studies that investigated this construct using various approaches.</p></div><div><h3>Main findings</h3><p>Findings from these studies indicated broad consensus in considering skills such as counting, number relations, and basic arithmetic, as central aspects of early numeracy. However, both the number and contents of identified factors varied considerably across studies.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Although this review substantiated the critical relevance of certain basic numerical skills, it also highlights a need for future research to evaluate the structure of early numeracy in a more systematic and coordinated way to increase comparability and coherence across studies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77191248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Examining cognitive sex differences in elite math intensive education: Preliminary evidence from a gender inequitable country 检验精英数学强化教育中的认知性别差异:来自性别不平等国家的初步证据
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2022.100172
Varsha Singh , Sonika Thakral , Kunal Singh , Rahul Garg

Background

It is unclear how cognitive control accounts for academic performance in math-intensive higher education and how it links to male over-representation in math-intensive education in gender-inequitable countries.

Purpose

To examine the link between cognitive control and math-intensive education with a focus on male overrepresentation by using cognitive performance (task and construct level) to account for academic grades, and examining sex-specificity in cognitive performance (task and construct level), and using sex-differences in cognitive performance to account for academic grades.

Results

Four hierarchical regressions were used (two using task scores and two summed scores) with predictors entered in 3 blocks (working memory, flexibility, inhibition) to explain academic performance (bootstrapped sampling at 2000 samples; N = 39; males =69%). Task-level analysis (Corsi span & mental rotation) and construct-level analysis indicate working memory as a significant predictor of grades, model-fit improved for all-male sample. Results of analysis of variance using the performance of 183 students on four cognitive tasks (N = 183; males = 81%) showed high scores of working memory task and decision-making task among male participants; female scores were higher in a task assessing planning/cognitive flexibility and in the inhibition task. Differences in the two hierarchical regressions indicated that planning/cognitive flexibility accounts for the academic performance of the male-female mixed sample; however, working memory, most importantly decision-making related to risk and uncertainty, accounts for the academic performance of the all-male sample.

Conclusion

Similar to developing countries, working memory and decision making might contribute to academic performance, potentially explaining male over-representation in math-intensive higher education. Academic grades might disproportionately rely on working memory and risky decision-making; equal emphasis and inclusive development of all components of cognitive control via academic curriculum and assessment might improve diversity in math-intensive higher education.

目前尚不清楚认知控制如何影响数学密集型高等教育的学习成绩,以及在性别不平等的国家,认知控制如何与数学密集型教育中男性比例过高联系起来。目的研究认知控制与数学强化教育之间的联系,重点关注男性比例过高,方法是使用认知表现(任务和结构水平)来解释学业成绩,并检查认知表现(任务和结构水平)的性别特异性,以及使用认知表现的性别差异来解释学业成绩。结果采用4个层次回归(2个使用任务分数,2个使用总和分数),预测因子分为3个区块(工作记忆、灵活性、抑制)来解释学习成绩(自举抽样2000个样本;n = 39;男性= 69%)。任务级分析(Corsi span &心理旋转和结构水平分析表明,工作记忆是成绩的显著预测因子,模型拟合在所有男性样本中都有所改善。183名学生在四项认知任务上的表现的方差分析结果(N = 183;男性(81%)在工作记忆任务和决策任务中得分较高;女性在评估计划/认知灵活性的任务和抑制任务中的得分更高。两个层次回归的差异表明,计划/认知灵活性对男女混合样本的学习成绩有影响;然而,工作记忆,最重要的是与风险和不确定性相关的决策,解释了所有男性样本的学习成绩。结论:与发展中国家类似,工作记忆和决策能力可能对学习成绩有影响,这可能解释了在数学密集型高等教育中男性比例过高的原因。学习成绩可能不成比例地依赖于工作记忆和冒险决策;通过学术课程和评估平等地强调和包容性地发展认知控制的所有组成部分,可能会改善数学密集型高等教育的多样性。
{"title":"Examining cognitive sex differences in elite math intensive education: Preliminary evidence from a gender inequitable country","authors":"Varsha Singh ,&nbsp;Sonika Thakral ,&nbsp;Kunal Singh ,&nbsp;Rahul Garg","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2022.100172","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2022.100172","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>It is unclear how cognitive control accounts for academic performance in math-intensive higher education and how it links to male over-representation in math-intensive education in gender-inequitable countries.</p></div><div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>To examine the link between cognitive control and math-intensive education with a focus on male overrepresentation by using cognitive performance (task and construct level) to account for academic grades, and examining sex-specificity in cognitive performance (task and construct level), and using sex-differences in cognitive performance to account for academic grades.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p><span>Four hierarchical regressions were used (two using task scores and two summed scores) with predictors entered in 3 blocks (working memory, flexibility, inhibition) to explain academic performance (bootstrapped sampling at 2000 samples; </span><em>N</em><span> = 39; males =69%). Task-level analysis (Corsi span &amp; mental rotation) and construct-level analysis indicate working memory as a significant predictor of grades, model-fit improved for all-male sample. Results of analysis of variance using the performance of 183 students on four cognitive tasks (</span><em>N</em> = 183; males = 81%) showed high scores of working memory task and decision-making task among male participants; female scores were higher in a task assessing planning/cognitive flexibility and in the inhibition task. Differences in the two hierarchical regressions indicated that planning/cognitive flexibility accounts for the academic performance of the male-female mixed sample; however, working memory, most importantly decision-making related to risk and uncertainty, accounts for the academic performance of the all-male sample.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Similar to developing countries, working memory and decision making might contribute to academic performance, potentially explaining male over-representation in math-intensive higher education. Academic grades might disproportionately rely on working memory and risky decision-making; equal emphasis and inclusive development of all components of cognitive control via academic curriculum and assessment might improve diversity in math-intensive higher education.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80491982","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Evaluating students’ engagement with an online learning environment during and after COVID-19 related school closures: A survival analysis approach 评估学生在COVID-19相关学校关闭期间和之后对在线学习环境的参与:生存分析方法
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100168
Markus Wolfgang Hermann Spitzer , Raphael Gutsfeld , Maria Wirzberger , Korbinian Moeller

Background

Due to the COVID-19 pandemic schools all over the world were closed and thereby students had to be instructed from distance. Consequently, the use of online learning environments for online distance learning increased massively. However, the perseverance of using online learning environments during and after school closures remains to be investigated.

Method

We examined German students’ (n ≈ 300,000 students; ≈ 18 million computed problem sets) engagement in an online learning environment for mathematics by means of survival analysis.

Results

We observed that the total number of students who registered increased considerably during and after school closures compared to the previous three years. Importantly, however, the proportion of students engaged also decreased more rapidly over time.

Conclusion

The application of survival analysis provided valuable insights into students’ engagement in online learning - or conversely students’ increased dropout rates - over time. Its application to educational settings allows to address a broader range of questions on students’ engagement in online learning environments in the future.

由于新冠肺炎疫情,世界各地的学校都关闭了,因此学生们不得不远程授课。因此,使用在线学习环境进行在线远程学习的人数大幅增加。然而,在学校关闭期间和之后,使用在线学习环境的毅力仍有待调查。方法对德国学生(n≈30万)进行调查;≈1800万计算问题集)通过生存分析的方式参与在线数学学习环境。结果我们观察到,与前三年相比,在学校关闭期间和之后注册的学生总数大幅增加。然而,重要的是,随着时间的推移,学生参与的比例也下降得更快。生存分析的应用为学生在线学习的参与度提供了有价值的见解,或者反过来说,学生的辍学率随着时间的推移而增加。将其应用到教育环境中,可以解决关于未来学生参与在线学习环境的更广泛的问题。
{"title":"Evaluating students’ engagement with an online learning environment during and after COVID-19 related school closures: A survival analysis approach","authors":"Markus Wolfgang Hermann Spitzer ,&nbsp;Raphael Gutsfeld ,&nbsp;Maria Wirzberger ,&nbsp;Korbinian Moeller","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100168","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100168","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Due to the COVID-19 pandemic schools all over the world were closed and thereby students had to be instructed from distance. Consequently, the use of online learning environments for online distance learning increased massively. However, the perseverance of using online learning environments during and after school closures remains to be investigated.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>We examined German students’ (<em>n</em> ≈ 300,000 students; ≈ 18 million computed problem sets) engagement in an online learning environment for mathematics by means of survival analysis.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>We observed that the total number of students who registered increased considerably during and after school closures compared to the previous three years. Importantly, however, the proportion of students engaged also decreased more rapidly over time.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>The application of survival analysis provided valuable insights into students’ engagement in online learning - or conversely students’ increased dropout rates - over time. Its application to educational settings allows to address a broader range of questions on students’ engagement in online learning environments in the future.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8599139/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10740573","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 26
When a spoon is not a spoon: Examining the role of executive function in young children's divergent thinking 当勺子不再是勺子:执行功能在幼儿发散性思维中的作用
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100161
Julie Vaisarova , Stephanie M. Carlson

Background

Research with adults suggests that executive function (EF) might play a role in the development of divergent thinking, a key component of creativity, by helping children override canonical knowledge.

Procedure

We examined this possibility in two experiments, by manipulating the familiarity of objects used in the Alternate Uses test of divergent thinking both between-participants (Experiment 1: N = 53 4-year-olds and 50 6-year-olds) and within-participants (Experiment 2: N = 74 5-year-olds).

Findings

We found evidence that younger children generated more and/or more original ideas for novel than familiar objects. However, this effect disappeared with age and did not depend on child EF. Further, EF was inversely associated with divergent thinking, controlling for age, intelligence, and income.

Significance

These results call into question a simple executive account of children's divergent thinking and suggest that, among predominantly White, socioeconomically advantaged 4–6-year-olds, divergent idea-generation might be a primarily bottom-up process that can be hindered by top-down thinking.

对成人的研究表明,执行功能(EF)可能通过帮助儿童超越规范知识,在发散思维的发展中发挥作用,发散思维是创造力的关键组成部分。我们在两个实验中检验了这种可能性,通过操纵在发散思维替代用途测试中使用的物体的熟悉度,在参与者之间(实验1:53名4岁儿童和50名6岁儿童)和参与者内部(实验2:74名5岁儿童)。我们发现有证据表明,年龄较小的孩子对新奇事物产生的原创想法比熟悉的事物多。然而,这种影响随着年龄的增长而消失,与儿童EF无关。此外,EF与发散性思维呈负相关,受年龄、智力和收入的影响。这些结果对儿童发散性思维的简单行政解释提出了质疑,并表明,在以白人为主、社会经济条件优越的4 - 6岁儿童中,发散性想法的产生可能主要是一个自下而上的过程,可能会受到自上而下思维的阻碍。
{"title":"When a spoon is not a spoon: Examining the role of executive function in young children's divergent thinking","authors":"Julie Vaisarova ,&nbsp;Stephanie M. Carlson","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100161","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100161","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Research with adults suggests that executive function (EF) might play a role in the development of divergent thinking, a key component of creativity, by helping children override canonical knowledge.</p></div><div><h3>Procedure</h3><p>We examined this possibility in two experiments, by manipulating the familiarity of objects used in the Alternate Uses test of divergent thinking both between-participants (Experiment 1: <em>N</em> = 53 4-year-olds and 50 6-year-olds) and within-participants (Experiment 2: <em>N</em> = 74 5-year-olds).</p></div><div><h3>Findings</h3><p>We found evidence that younger children generated more and/or more original ideas for novel than familiar objects. However, this effect disappeared with age and did not depend on child EF. Further, EF was inversely associated with divergent thinking, controlling for age, intelligence, and income.</p></div><div><h3>Significance</h3><p>These results call into question a simple executive account of children's divergent thinking and suggest that, among predominantly White, socioeconomically advantaged 4–6-year-olds, divergent idea-generation might be a primarily bottom-up process that can be hindered by top-down thinking.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39677614","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The perception of subset quantity and items in an environment with distractors in a population with mathematical learning difficulties 数学学习困难人群在有干扰的环境中对子集数量和项目的感知
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100166
Sharon Levy, Liat Goldfarb

Objective

People often perceive a quantity of specific objects that appear as part of an overall group of items (a subset). This study investigates this type of perception among a population with mathematical leaning difficulties (MLD).

Method

Sixty-two participants (mean age: 26.82) reported the general and subset quantity of items using a subset quantity detection task or a conjunction visual search task.

Results

MLD had difficulties perceiving both the general quantity presented and the subset quantity of items. They also had difficulties preforming a conjunction visual search task, even when the task did not involve numerical processing.

Conclusions

MLD has spatial difficulties in the form of visual search and subset quantity detection. The current study suggests that MLD might experience greater difficulties in daily tasks, which might be related to those tasks (e.g., detecting the amount of forks among other items of silverware on the table).

人们通常认为一定数量的特定对象是作为整体项目组(子集)的一部分出现的。本研究调查了数学学习困难(MLD)人群的这种感知。方法62名被试(平均年龄26.82岁)分别使用子集数量检测任务和关联视觉搜索任务报告项目的一般数量和子集数量。结果被试对项目的总体数量和子集数量均存在感知困难。他们在执行连接视觉搜索任务时也有困难,即使这个任务不涉及数字处理。结论smld在视觉搜索和子集数量检测方面存在空间困难。目前的研究表明,MLD在日常任务中可能会遇到更大的困难,这可能与这些任务有关(例如,在桌子上的其他银器中检测叉子的数量)。
{"title":"The perception of subset quantity and items in an environment with distractors in a population with mathematical learning difficulties","authors":"Sharon Levy,&nbsp;Liat Goldfarb","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100166","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100166","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>People often perceive a quantity of specific objects that appear as part of an overall group of items (a subset). This study investigates this type of perception among a population with mathematical leaning difficulties (MLD).</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Sixty-two participants (mean age: 26.82) reported the general and subset quantity of items using a subset quantity detection task or a conjunction visual search task.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>MLD had difficulties perceiving both the general quantity presented and the subset quantity of items. They also had difficulties preforming a conjunction visual search task, even when the task did not involve numerical processing.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>MLD has spatial difficulties in the form of visual search and subset quantity detection. The current study suggests that MLD might experience greater difficulties in daily tasks, which might be related to those tasks (e.g., detecting the amount of forks among other items of silverware on the table).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39929938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Short breaks at school: effects of a physical activity and a mindfulness intervention on children's attention, reading comprehension, and self-esteem 学校短暂休息:体育活动和正念干预对儿童注意力、阅读理解和自尊的影响
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100160
Christian Müller , Barbara Otto , Viktoria Sawitzki , Priyanga Kanagalingam , Jens-Steffen Scherer , Sven Lindberg

Background

Although breaks are essential to restoring cognitive and psychological conditions for learning, short breaks within school lessons are not established and the specificity of effects has not often been investigated. Therefore, the effects of a physical activity (Study 1) and a mindfulness intervention (Study 2) were investigated.

Procedure

By an intervention-control group design, the effects of daily 10-min physical activity (Study 1: N = 162, 4th grade) and mindfulness breaks (Study 2: N = 79, 5th grade) were implemented within regular school lessons over a 2-week time period to research the impact on attention, reading comprehension, and self-esteem.

Results

In the physical activity intervention children's attention improved (attention-processing speed: p < .004, ηp2 = .05, attention-performance: p < .025, ηp2 = .03), and in the mindfulness intervention reading comprehension improved (p < .012, ηp2 = .08) compared to the controls. Results further indicated that self-esteem moderated the relationship between groups and attention improvement in study 1.

Conclusion

Classroom-based short physical and mindfulness breaks could support attention and reading comprehension, which are known to support overall academic success.

虽然休息对恢复学习的认知和心理状态是必不可少的,但在学校课程中没有建立短暂的休息,其效果的特异性也没有经常进行调查。因此,我们研究了体力活动(研究1)和正念干预(研究2)的效果。通过干预对照组设计,在为期两周的常规学校课程中实施每日10分钟体育活动(研究1:N = 162,四年级)和正念休息(研究2:N = 79,五年级)的效果,以研究对注意力,阅读理解和自尊的影响。结果体育活动干预后儿童注意加工速度显著提高;0.004, ηp2 = 0.05,注意力-表现:p <0.025, ηp2 = 0.03),正念干预组阅读理解能力提高(p <0.012, ηp2 = 0.08)。研究结果进一步表明,自尊调节了小组与注意改善之间的关系。结论基于课堂的短暂身体和正念休息可以提高注意力和阅读理解能力,这是众所周知的有助于整体学业成功的因素。
{"title":"Short breaks at school: effects of a physical activity and a mindfulness intervention on children's attention, reading comprehension, and self-esteem","authors":"Christian Müller ,&nbsp;Barbara Otto ,&nbsp;Viktoria Sawitzki ,&nbsp;Priyanga Kanagalingam ,&nbsp;Jens-Steffen Scherer ,&nbsp;Sven Lindberg","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100160","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100160","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Although breaks are essential to restoring cognitive and psychological conditions for learning, short breaks within school lessons are not established and the specificity of effects has not often been investigated. Therefore, the effects of a physical activity (Study 1) and a mindfulness intervention (Study 2) were investigated.</p></div><div><h3>Procedure</h3><p>By an intervention-control group design, the effects of daily 10-min physical activity (Study 1: N = 162, 4th grade) and mindfulness breaks (Study 2: N = 79, 5th grade) were implemented within regular school lessons over a 2-week time period to research the impact on attention, reading comprehension, and self-esteem.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>In the physical activity intervention children's attention improved (attention-processing speed: <em>p</em> &lt; .004, <span><math><msubsup><mi>η</mi><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow><mn>2</mn></msubsup></math></span> = .05, attention-performance: <em>p</em> &lt; .025, <span><math><msubsup><mi>η</mi><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow><mn>2</mn></msubsup></math></span> = .03), and in the mindfulness intervention reading comprehension improved (<em>p</em> &lt; .012, <span><math><msubsup><mi>η</mi><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow><mn>2</mn></msubsup></math></span> = .08) compared to the controls. Results further indicated that self-esteem moderated the relationship between groups and attention improvement in study 1.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Classroom-based short physical and mindfulness breaks could support attention and reading comprehension, which are known to support overall academic success.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.tine.2021.100160","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39677613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Primary school mathematics during the COVID-19 pandemic: No evidence of learning gaps in adaptive practicing results COVID-19大流行期间的小学数学:没有证据表明适应性实践结果存在学习差距
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100163
Martijn Meeter

Background

The COVID-19 pandemic induced many governments to close schools for months. Evidence so far suggests that learning has suffered as a result. Here, it is investigated whether forms of computer-assisted learning mitigated the decrements in learning observed during the lockdown.

Method

Performance of 53,656 primary school students who used adaptive practicing software for mathematics was compared to performance of similar students in the preceding year.

Results

During the lockdown progress was faster than it had been the year before, contradicting results reported so far. These enhanced gains were correlated with increased use, and remained after the lockdown ended. This was the case for all grades but more so for lower grades and for weak students, but less so for students in schools with disadvantaged populations.

Conclusions

These results suggest that adaptive practicing software may mitigate, or even reverse, the negative effects of school closures on mathematics learning.

COVID-19大流行导致许多政府关闭学校数月。迄今为止的证据表明,学习受到了影响。在这里,研究人员调查了计算机辅助学习的形式是否减轻了封锁期间观察到的学习减少。方法对53,656名使用自适应数学练习软件的小学生进行上一年度与同类学生成绩的比较。结果在封锁期间,进展比前一年更快,这与迄今报告的结果相矛盾。这些增加的收益与使用的增加相关,并在封锁结束后保持不变。所有年级的学生都是如此,但对低年级和成绩差的学生更是如此,而对弱势群体学校的学生则不那么如此。这些结果表明,适应性练习软件可以减轻甚至逆转学校关闭对数学学习的负面影响。
{"title":"Primary school mathematics during the COVID-19 pandemic: No evidence of learning gaps in adaptive practicing results","authors":"Martijn Meeter","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100163","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100163","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>The COVID-19 pandemic induced many governments to close schools for months. Evidence so far suggests that learning has suffered as a result. Here, it is investigated whether forms of computer-assisted learning mitigated the decrements in learning observed during the lockdown.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Performance of 53,656 primary school students who used adaptive practicing software for mathematics was compared to performance of similar students in the preceding year.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>During the lockdown progress was faster than it had been the year before, contradicting results reported so far. These enhanced gains were correlated with increased use, and remained after the lockdown ended. This was the case for all grades but more so for lower grades and for weak students, but less so for students in schools with disadvantaged populations.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These results suggest that adaptive practicing software may mitigate, or even reverse, the negative effects of school closures on mathematics learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8487463/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39929939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 34
The effect of video games, exergames and board games on executive functions in kindergarten and 2nd grade: An explorative longitudinal study 视频游戏、练习游戏和棋盘游戏对幼儿园和二年级儿童执行功能的影响:一项探索性的纵向研究
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100162
Venera Gashaj , Laura C. Dapp , Dragan Trninic , Claudia M. Roebers

We examined the relation between different kinds of play behavior (video games, exergames, board games) in kindergarten (T1) and components of executive function (EF; inhibition, switching, verbal and visuospatial updating) in kindergarten and second grade (T1 and T2). Ninety-seven children participated in this longitudinal study. Parents were asked to complete a questionnaire regarding children's play behavior, reporting frequency, duration, and game type. The results indicate that play behavior is associated with EF development in children; however, only exergames, electronic puzzle games, and board games predicted EF at T2. Additionally, the time spent on electronic games was negatively related to visuospatial updating at T1 but did not predict EF at T2. The results support further investigation of a potential link between board game and exergame play behavior and EF development.

我们研究了幼儿园(T1)不同类型的游戏行为(视频游戏、练习游戏、棋盘游戏)与执行功能成分(EF)之间的关系;抑制、转换、语言和视觉空间更新)在幼儿园和二年级(T1和T2)。97名儿童参与了这项纵向研究。家长被要求填写一份关于儿童游戏行为、报告频率、持续时间和游戏类型的问卷。结果表明,游戏行为与儿童英语发展相关;然而,只有游戏、电子益智游戏和桌游预测了EF为T2。此外,花在电子游戏上的时间与T1时的视觉空间更新负相关,但与T2时的EF不相关。研究结果支持进一步研究桌游和游戏行为与EF发展之间的潜在联系。
{"title":"The effect of video games, exergames and board games on executive functions in kindergarten and 2nd grade: An explorative longitudinal study","authors":"Venera Gashaj ,&nbsp;Laura C. Dapp ,&nbsp;Dragan Trninic ,&nbsp;Claudia M. Roebers","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100162","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100162","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined the relation between different kinds of play behavior (video games, exergames, board games) in kindergarten (T1) and components of executive function (EF; inhibition, switching, verbal and visuospatial updating) in kindergarten and second grade (T1 and T2). Ninety-seven children participated in this longitudinal study. Parents were asked to complete a questionnaire regarding children's play behavior, reporting frequency, duration, and game type. The results indicate that play behavior is associated with EF development in children; however, only exergames, electronic puzzle games, and board games predicted EF at T2. Additionally, the time spent on electronic games was negatively related to visuospatial updating at T1 but did not predict EF at T2. The results support further investigation of a potential link between board game and exergame play behavior and EF development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2211949321000144/pdfft?md5=16c1b492c96d13e8132cd569be7fc2fc&pid=1-s2.0-S2211949321000144-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39677615","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Executive functions and imaginative play: Exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling 执行功能和想象游戏:利用结构方程模型探索亲社会行为的关系
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165
Rebecca H. Bauer , Ansley T. Gilpin , Rachel B. Thibodeau-Nielsen

Background

Policy makers have long considered how to best educate children for success. Thus, this research explored whether imaginative play serves as a positive context for developing executive functions (EF) and prosocial behaviors necessary for academic success. Specifically, we hypothesized that EF would explain links between imaginative play and prosocial behaviors.

Method

284 preschool children and their teachers completed measures of imaginative play, EF, and prosocial behaviors. Structural Equation Modeling examined mediational links between these constructs.

Results

Imaginative play predicted both hot and cool EF, as well as prosocial behavior, even after controlling for age and vocabulary; furthermore, the relationship between imaginative play and prosocial behavior was fully mediated by hot EF.

Conclusion

Results suggest that imaginative play may serve as a positive context for developing EF and prosocial behaviors. Curricula and teachers should consider encouraging imaginative play to facilitate the development of EF and prosocial behaviors.

长期以来,政策制定者一直在考虑如何最好地教育孩子取得成功。因此,本研究探讨了想象力游戏是否为学业成功所需的执行功能(EF)和亲社会行为的发展提供了积极的环境。具体来说,我们假设EF可以解释想象力游戏和亲社会行为之间的联系。方法284名学龄前儿童及其教师完成了想象游戏、EF和亲社会行为的测试。结构方程模型检验了这些结构之间的中介联系。结果在控制了年龄和词汇量的影响后,想象游戏对热、冷EF以及亲社会行为均有预测作用;此外,想象游戏与亲社会行为之间的关系完全由热EF介导。结论:想象游戏可能为英语学习和亲社会行为的发展提供积极的环境。课程和教师应考虑鼓励想象力游戏,以促进EF和亲社会行为的发展。
{"title":"Executive functions and imaginative play: Exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling","authors":"Rebecca H. Bauer ,&nbsp;Ansley T. Gilpin ,&nbsp;Rachel B. Thibodeau-Nielsen","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p><span>Policy makers have long considered how to best educate children for success. Thus, this research explored whether imaginative play serves as a positive context for developing executive functions (EF) and </span>prosocial behaviors necessary for academic success. Specifically, we hypothesized that EF would explain links between imaginative play and prosocial behaviors.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>284 preschool children and their teachers completed measures of imaginative play, EF, and prosocial behaviors. Structural Equation Modeling examined mediational links between these constructs.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Imaginative play predicted both hot and cool EF, as well as prosocial behavior, even after controlling for age and vocabulary; furthermore, the relationship between imaginative play and prosocial behavior was fully mediated by hot EF.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Results suggest that imaginative play may serve as a positive context for developing EF and prosocial behaviors. Curricula and teachers should consider encouraging imaginative play to facilitate the development of EF and prosocial behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39677616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Mental simulation and its influence on finger-based numerical representations 心理模拟及其对手指数字表征的影响
IF 3.3 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2021.100167
Isabella Luise Kreilinger , Korbinian Moeller , Silvia Pixner

Background

There is evidence indicating beneficial effects of mental simulation on athletic and musical performance. We evaluated whether such beneficial effects of mental simulation generalize to the cognitive domain in terms of embodied (finger-based) numerical representations.

Methods

We assessed 70 preschoolers (36 girls, mean age 5;9) on tasks assessing different basic numerical skills (e.g., counting, cardinality understanding, number composition, etc.) as well as different aspects of finger-based numerical representations. A subgroub completed a mental simulation phase prior to testing finger-based representations.

Results

Children who completed the mental simulation phase, performed better on the tasks assessing finger-based representations compared to, children who did not complete the simulation phase. This held even when controlling for performance in basic numerical skills.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence that beneficial effects of mental simulation generalize to embodied (finger-based) numerical representations. Mental simulation may be useful to integrate in the instruction of basic numerical skills.

有证据表明心理模拟对运动和音乐表现有有益的影响。我们评估了心理模拟的这种有益影响是否可以在具体的(基于手指的)数字表征方面推广到认知领域。方法对70名学龄前儿童(36名女孩,平均年龄5岁至9岁)进行了不同基本数字技能(如计数、基数理解、数字构成等)和手指数字表征的不同方面的测试。在测试基于手指的表征之前,一个亚群完成了心理模拟阶段。结果完成心理模拟阶段的儿童在评估手指表征任务上的表现优于未完成模拟阶段的儿童。即使在控制基本数字技能的表现时也是如此。结论心理模拟的有益效应可以推广到具体的(基于手指的)数值表征。心理模拟可能有助于将其整合到基本的数字技能教学中。
{"title":"Mental simulation and its influence on finger-based numerical representations","authors":"Isabella Luise Kreilinger ,&nbsp;Korbinian Moeller ,&nbsp;Silvia Pixner","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100167","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2021.100167","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>There is evidence indicating beneficial effects of mental simulation on athletic and musical performance. We evaluated whether such beneficial effects of mental simulation generalize to the cognitive domain in terms of embodied (finger-based) numerical representations.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>We assessed 70 preschoolers (36 girls, mean age 5;9) on tasks assessing different basic numerical skills (e.g., counting, cardinality understanding, number composition, etc.) as well as different aspects of finger-based numerical representations. A subgroub completed a mental simulation phase prior to testing finger-based representations.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Children who completed the mental simulation phase, performed better on the tasks assessing finger-based representations compared to, children who did not complete the simulation phase. This held even when controlling for performance in basic numerical skills.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>This study provides evidence that beneficial effects of mental simulation generalize to embodied (finger-based) numerical representations. Mental simulation may be useful to integrate in the instruction of basic numerical skills.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39929937","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Trends in Neuroscience and Education
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1