首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Preface to the Special Issue on “The Role of Best Practices in Health Care Decision Making” “最佳做法在医疗保健决策中的作用”特刊序言
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418790084
L. Militello, M. Weiner
cussing the Role of Best Practices in Health Care Decision Making. The discussion began with a provocative article published in April 2016 in which Devorah Klein, David Woods, Gary Klein, and Shawna Perry raised the question, “Can we trust best practices?” (Klein, Woods, Klein, & Perry, 2016). Klein and her colleagues highlighted some of the challenges associated with using evidence-based medicine (EBM) to support clinical care and suggested naturalistic decision making (NDM) as an important perspective for addressing these challenges. Soon thereafter, Paul Falzer (2018) submitted a manuscript deepening and extending the discussion, pointing out that these challenges are well known in the health care community, and noting that effective solutions remain elusive. In this issue, Falzer’s article provides an in-depth discussion of the concept of decision making in this context and the unintended negative consequences of evidence-based recommendations. This article became the centerpiece for this special issue; commentators were asked to react to Falzer’s article. Because the topic of EBM and its impact on decision making and the quality of health care falls at the intersection of at least two important scientific disciplines (EBM and NDM), perspectives of experts who have been thinking about these issues in various contexts and from various traditions are important. We have been fortunate to obtain commentaries from a range of thought leaders representing both EBM and NDM for this special issue. As we reviewed the commentaries, it became clear that there is sometimes disagreement about what EBM really is and what it implies for health care. “Critics” tend to view EBM narrowly, whereas “proponents” have a broader and more multidimensional view of EBM. It is also worth noting that many commentators draw strong links between EBM and the “best practices regimen” (i.e., initiatives and interventions that define the quality of decision making by conformance to evidence-based practices). Other contributors note that these are distinct concepts, suggesting that while managed care, health services research, implementation research, and the best practices regimen are directly influenced by classical decision theory, EBM might be characterized as restoring decision making to its “rightful place.” In some ways, this confusion rooted in the language can be seen as encouraging: There might not be as much disagreement as it appears on the surface, if one begins to discuss concepts and approaches rather than relying on labels. Nonetheless, important points of divergence are found in the commentaries. Although many would agree that EBM was never intended to constrain clinician discretion and discount expertise, there is little agreement about how EBM should be implemented. We invite you to enjoy the following commentaries. Haynes, a member of the working group that articulated a vision for evidence-based medicine in 1992 (Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group, 1992)
讨论最佳实践在医疗保健决策中的作用。讨论始于2016年4月发表的一篇挑衅性文章,Devorah Klein、David Woods、Gary Klein和Shawna Perry在文章中提出了“我们能相信最佳实践吗?”(Klein、Woods、Klein和Perry,2016)。Klein和她的同事强调了使用循证医学(EBM)支持临床护理的一些挑战,并建议将自然主义决策(NDM)作为应对这些挑战的重要视角。此后不久,Paul Falzer(2018)提交了一份深化和扩展讨论的手稿,指出这些挑战在医疗保健界是众所周知的,并指出有效的解决方案仍然难以捉摸。在本期文章中,Falzer的文章深入讨论了在此背景下的决策概念以及循证建议的意外负面后果。这篇文章成为本期特刊的核心;评论员被要求对法尔泽的文章作出反应。由于循证医学及其对决策和医疗质量的影响是至少两个重要科学学科(循证医学和新医学)的交叉点,因此,在不同背景和不同传统中思考这些问题的专家的观点很重要。我们很幸运地获得了代表EBM和NDM的一系列思想领袖对本期特刊的评论。当我们回顾评论时,很明显,有时对循证医学的真正含义及其对医疗保健的含义存在分歧。“批评者”倾向于狭隘地看待循证医学,而“支持者”则对循证医学有着更广泛、更多维的看法。同样值得注意的是,许多评论家将循证医学与“最佳实践方案”(即通过遵守循证实践来定义决策质量的举措和干预措施)联系起来。其他贡献者指出,这些都是不同的概念,表明虽然管理护理、医疗服务研究、实施研究和最佳实践方案直接受到经典决策理论的影响,但循证医学可能被描述为将决策恢复到其“合法地位”,这种植根于语言中的困惑可以被视为令人鼓舞的:如果一个人开始讨论概念和方法,而不是依赖标签,那么可能不会有表面上那么多的分歧。尽管如此,在评注中还是发现了重要的分歧点。尽管许多人会同意循证医学从来没有打算限制临床医生的自由裁量权和折扣专业知识,但对于如何实施循证医学,人们几乎没有达成一致。我们邀请您欣赏以下评论。海恩斯是1992年阐明循证医学愿景的工作组成员(循证医学工作组,1992年),他澄清说,“遵循原则”、“最佳实践方案”和“转导模型”不是循证医学的一部分。此外,循证医学从一开始就被认为是临床医生和患者决策的众多投入之一。然而,他警告说,在没有实证验证这种方法对实际实践环境中的结果产生积极影响的情况下,不要采用NDM实践和应用。他鼓励NDM科学、知识翻译、传播和实施的倡导者之间的合作。在今天的讨论中,Schneider提醒我们更早的相关讨论,790084 EDMXXX10.1177/15553343418790084认知工程与决策杂志“最佳实践在医疗保健决策中的作用”2018前言
{"title":"Preface to the Special Issue on “The Role of Best Practices in Health Care Decision Making”","authors":"L. Militello, M. Weiner","doi":"10.1177/1555343418790084","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418790084","url":null,"abstract":"cussing the Role of Best Practices in Health Care Decision Making. The discussion began with a provocative article published in April 2016 in which Devorah Klein, David Woods, Gary Klein, and Shawna Perry raised the question, “Can we trust best practices?” (Klein, Woods, Klein, & Perry, 2016). Klein and her colleagues highlighted some of the challenges associated with using evidence-based medicine (EBM) to support clinical care and suggested naturalistic decision making (NDM) as an important perspective for addressing these challenges. Soon thereafter, Paul Falzer (2018) submitted a manuscript deepening and extending the discussion, pointing out that these challenges are well known in the health care community, and noting that effective solutions remain elusive. In this issue, Falzer’s article provides an in-depth discussion of the concept of decision making in this context and the unintended negative consequences of evidence-based recommendations. This article became the centerpiece for this special issue; commentators were asked to react to Falzer’s article. Because the topic of EBM and its impact on decision making and the quality of health care falls at the intersection of at least two important scientific disciplines (EBM and NDM), perspectives of experts who have been thinking about these issues in various contexts and from various traditions are important. We have been fortunate to obtain commentaries from a range of thought leaders representing both EBM and NDM for this special issue. As we reviewed the commentaries, it became clear that there is sometimes disagreement about what EBM really is and what it implies for health care. “Critics” tend to view EBM narrowly, whereas “proponents” have a broader and more multidimensional view of EBM. It is also worth noting that many commentators draw strong links between EBM and the “best practices regimen” (i.e., initiatives and interventions that define the quality of decision making by conformance to evidence-based practices). Other contributors note that these are distinct concepts, suggesting that while managed care, health services research, implementation research, and the best practices regimen are directly influenced by classical decision theory, EBM might be characterized as restoring decision making to its “rightful place.” In some ways, this confusion rooted in the language can be seen as encouraging: There might not be as much disagreement as it appears on the surface, if one begins to discuss concepts and approaches rather than relying on labels. Nonetheless, important points of divergence are found in the commentaries. Although many would agree that EBM was never intended to constrain clinician discretion and discount expertise, there is little agreement about how EBM should be implemented. We invite you to enjoy the following commentaries. Haynes, a member of the working group that articulated a vision for evidence-based medicine in 1992 (Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group, 1992)","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"175 - 177"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418790084","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43646876","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Reimagining the Humble but Mighty Pen: Quality Measurement and Naturalistic Decision Making 重新想象谦卑但强大的笔:质量测量和自然决策
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418784372
E. Schneider
Much of the health system’s avoidable spending may be driven by doctors’ decision making. Past studies demonstrated potentially consequential and costly inconsistencies between the actual decisions that clinicians make in daily practice and optimal evidence-based decisions. This commentary examines the “best practices regimen” through the lens of the quality measurement movement.  Although quality measures have proliferated via public reporting and pay-for-performance programs, evidence for their impact on quality of care is scant; the cost of care has continued to rise; and the environment for clinical decisions may not have improved. Naturalistic decision making offers a compelling alternative conceptual frame for quality measurement. An alternative quality measurement system could build on insights from naturalistic decision making to optimize doctors’ and patients’ joint decisions, improve patients’ health outcomes, and perhaps slow the growth of health care spending in the future.
卫生系统本可以避免的支出可能很大程度上是由医生的决策推动的。过去的研究表明,临床医生在日常实践中做出的实际决策与最佳循证决策之间存在潜在的后果性和代价高昂的不一致。这篇评论通过质量测量运动的视角审视了“最佳实践方案”。 尽管通过公开报告和绩效付费计划,质量措施激增,但很少有证据表明它们对护理质量的影响;护理费用持续上涨;并且用于临床决策的环境可能没有得到改善。自然主义决策为质量测量提供了一个令人信服的替代概念框架。一种替代的质量测量系统可以建立在自然主义决策的基础上,以优化医生和患者的联合决策,改善患者的健康结果,并可能减缓未来医疗保健支出的增长。
{"title":"Reimagining the Humble but Mighty Pen: Quality Measurement and Naturalistic Decision Making","authors":"E. Schneider","doi":"10.1177/1555343418784372","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418784372","url":null,"abstract":"Much of the health system’s avoidable spending may be driven by doctors’ decision making. Past studies demonstrated potentially consequential and costly inconsistencies between the actual decisions that clinicians make in daily practice and optimal evidence-based decisions. This commentary examines the “best practices regimen” through the lens of the quality measurement movement.  Although quality measures have proliferated via public reporting and pay-for-performance programs, evidence for their impact on quality of care is scant; the cost of care has continued to rise; and the environment for clinical decisions may not have improved. Naturalistic decision making offers a compelling alternative conceptual frame for quality measurement. An alternative quality measurement system could build on insights from naturalistic decision making to optimize doctors’ and patients’ joint decisions, improve patients’ health outcomes, and perhaps slow the growth of health care spending in the future.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"198 - 201"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418784372","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43797034","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
EBM: Rationalist Fever Dreams EBM:理性主义狂热的梦想
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418779677
D. E. Klein, David D. Woods, Gary Klein, S. Perry
In 2016, we examined the connection between naturalistic decision making and the trend toward best practice compliance; we used evidence-based medicine (EBM) in health care as an exemplar. Paul Falzer’s lead paper in this issue describes the historical underpinnings of how and why EBM came into vogue in health care. Falzer also highlights the epistemological rationale for EBM. Falzer’s article, like our own, questions the rationale of EBM and reflects on ways that naturalistic decision making can support expertise in the face of attempts to standardize practice and emphasize compliance. Our objectives in this commentary are first to explain the inherent limits of procedural approaches and second to examine ways to help decision makers become more adaptive.
2016年,我们研究了自然主义决策与最佳实践合规趋势之间的联系;我们以循证医学(EBM)在医疗保健中的应用为例。Paul Falzer在本期的主要论文描述了循证医学如何以及为什么在医疗保健中流行的历史基础。Falzer还强调了EBM的认识论原理。Falzer的文章和我们自己的文章一样,质疑EBM的基本原理,并反思了在试图规范实践和强调合规性的情况下,自然主义决策可以支持专业知识的方式。我们在本评注中的目的首先是解释程序性方法的固有局限性,其次是研究如何帮助决策者变得更具适应性。
{"title":"EBM: Rationalist Fever Dreams","authors":"D. E. Klein, David D. Woods, Gary Klein, S. Perry","doi":"10.1177/1555343418779677","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418779677","url":null,"abstract":"In 2016, we examined the connection between naturalistic decision making and the trend toward best practice compliance; we used evidence-based medicine (EBM) in health care as an exemplar. Paul Falzer’s lead paper in this issue describes the historical underpinnings of how and why EBM came into vogue in health care. Falzer also highlights the epistemological rationale for EBM. Falzer’s article, like our own, questions the rationale of EBM and reflects on ways that naturalistic decision making can support expertise in the face of attempts to standardize practice and emphasize compliance. Our objectives in this commentary are first to explain the inherent limits of procedural approaches and second to examine ways to help decision makers become more adaptive.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"227 - 230"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418779677","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41544019","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Three Observations 三个观察
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418775288
L. Beach
Invited commentary on “Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care,” by Paul R. Falzer for the Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making.
Paul R.Falzer为《认知工程与决策杂志》撰写的“自然主义决策与医疗保健实践”特邀评论。
{"title":"Three Observations","authors":"L. Beach","doi":"10.1177/1555343418775288","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418775288","url":null,"abstract":"Invited commentary on “Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care,” by Paul R. Falzer for the Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"206 - 208"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418775288","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47433274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Physician Judgment and Clinical Practice Guidelines 医师判断和临床实践指南
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418782850
R. Hamm, Zsolt J. Nagykaldi
Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care” by Paul R. Falzer in this special issue. Falzer (2018) and Klein, Woods, Klein, and Perry (2016) have called attention to the fact that external payers (insurance companies, government programs) may reward or penalize individual physicians or clinical groups depending on whether their behavior accords with the recommendation of an evidence-based clinical practice guideline (EB-CPG). (Klein et al. [2016] call this the “best practices regimen,” but to avoid confusion with a methodology focused on design of exemplary and successful approaches for accomplishing clinic tasks [Mold & Gregory, 2003], we will refer to “enforced conformance” to EB-CPG). Clinic administration may then institute a system to measure and reward individual physician performance to assure the group practice overall meets the stated standard. Falzer identifies flaws in the logic behind making reward contingent on meeting guideline-related standards and reviews the varied responses physicians have to enforced conformance. Although we concur with many aspects of the critique, we value the science and wisdom embodied in the guidelines and recognize that at times physicians may need external motivation to heed them. The focus is CPGs that in good faith address the health needs of patients and society. There are three frameworks for these. Guidelines can be based on the judgments of expert clinicians and other stakeholders (Crownover & Unwin, 2005), on studies providing justified evidence that clinical practices are likely to have beneficial effects (Alonso-Coello et al., 2016), or on broader analyses that consider the money needed to produce those beneficial effects so that society’s resources may be allocated to the most cost-effective practices (Mandelblatt, Fryback, Weinstein, Russell, & Gold, 1997; Pandya, 2018). Interestingly, the expert judgment recommendations are correlated with the cost-effectiveness analysis recommendations (Kuntz, Tsevat, Weinstein, & Goldman, 1999). CPGs from any of these frameworks can experience the problems Falzer (2018) identifies when recommendations become requirements and nonconformance with them is penalized. However, physicians cannot simply be ordered to behave in a way that maximizes rewards or optimizes outcomes. The way they manage patients is based on long-established habit (Hamm, 2009a). Hearing about, reading, or studying an EB-CPG endorsed by a respected authority does not make physicians immediately change behavior, even if they intend to. They have to learn the recommended alternative behavior, recognize when the guideline describes something different from what they usually do, and make the conscious choice to change behavior. This must be done consciously until it can become a new habit (Abernathy & Hamm, 1995). This situation is more complex when the current habitual practice involves multiple actors (Ackerman, Gonzales, Stahl, & Metlay, 2013; Gonzales, Steiner, Lum, & Barrett, 1999),
Paul R.Falzer在本期特刊中的《决策与医疗实践》。Falzer(2018)和Klein、Woods、Klein和Perry(2016)呼吁注意这样一个事实,即外部付款人(保险公司、政府项目)可能会根据个人医生或临床团体的行为是否符合循证临床实践指南(EB-CPG)的建议来奖励或惩罚他们。(Klein等人[2016]称之为“最佳实践方案”,但为了避免与专注于设计完成临床任务的示范性和成功方法的方法相混淆[Mold&Gregory,2003],我们将提及EB-CPG的“强制合规性”)。然后,诊所管理部门可以建立一个系统来衡量和奖励个别医生的表现,以确保团队实践总体符合规定的标准。Falzer发现了将奖励视为符合指南相关标准的逻辑中的缺陷,并审查了医生对强制遵守的各种反应。尽管我们同意批评的许多方面,但我们重视指导方针中体现的科学和智慧,并认识到有时医生可能需要外部动机来关注它们。重点是本着诚意满足患者和社会健康需求的CPG。有三个框架。指南可以基于临床医生专家和其他利益相关者的判断(Crownover&Unwin,2005),基于提供合理证据证明临床实践可能具有有益效果的研究(Alonso-Coello等人,2016),或者基于更广泛的分析,考虑产生这些有益效果所需的资金,以便将社会资源分配给最具成本效益的做法(Mandelblatt,Fryback,Weinstein,Russell,&Gold,1997;Pandya,2018)。有趣的是,专家判断建议与成本效益分析建议相关(Kuntz,Tsevat,Weinstein,&Goldman,1999)。当建议成为要求时,来自任何这些框架的CPG都可能遇到Falzer(2018)发现的问题,不符合这些建议的情况将受到惩罚。然而,不能简单地命令医生以最大化奖励或优化结果的方式行事。他们管理病人的方式是基于长期养成的习惯(Hamm,2009a)。听到、阅读或研究由受人尊敬的权威机构认可的EB-CPG并不能让医生立即改变行为,即使他们打算这样做。他们必须学习推荐的替代行为,识别指南何时描述了与他们通常做的不同的事情,并有意识地选择改变行为。这必须有意识地进行,直到它成为一种新习惯(Abernathy&Ham,1995)。当当前的习惯性做法涉及多个参与者时,这种情况会更加复杂(Ackerman,Gonzales,Stahl,&Metlay,2013;Gonzales、Steiner、Lum和Barrett,1999),而不仅仅是单个医生,当当前的做法效果更好时(Hamm,2009b)。因此,个体医生可能需要强烈的动机来遵守指导方针,即使他或她在理智上接受它是正确的。782850 EDMXX10.1177/1555334341878250Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingJudge and Guidelines 2018
{"title":"Physician Judgment and Clinical Practice Guidelines","authors":"R. Hamm, Zsolt J. Nagykaldi","doi":"10.1177/1555343418782850","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418782850","url":null,"abstract":"Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care” by Paul R. Falzer in this special issue. Falzer (2018) and Klein, Woods, Klein, and Perry (2016) have called attention to the fact that external payers (insurance companies, government programs) may reward or penalize individual physicians or clinical groups depending on whether their behavior accords with the recommendation of an evidence-based clinical practice guideline (EB-CPG). (Klein et al. [2016] call this the “best practices regimen,” but to avoid confusion with a methodology focused on design of exemplary and successful approaches for accomplishing clinic tasks [Mold & Gregory, 2003], we will refer to “enforced conformance” to EB-CPG). Clinic administration may then institute a system to measure and reward individual physician performance to assure the group practice overall meets the stated standard. Falzer identifies flaws in the logic behind making reward contingent on meeting guideline-related standards and reviews the varied responses physicians have to enforced conformance. Although we concur with many aspects of the critique, we value the science and wisdom embodied in the guidelines and recognize that at times physicians may need external motivation to heed them. The focus is CPGs that in good faith address the health needs of patients and society. There are three frameworks for these. Guidelines can be based on the judgments of expert clinicians and other stakeholders (Crownover & Unwin, 2005), on studies providing justified evidence that clinical practices are likely to have beneficial effects (Alonso-Coello et al., 2016), or on broader analyses that consider the money needed to produce those beneficial effects so that society’s resources may be allocated to the most cost-effective practices (Mandelblatt, Fryback, Weinstein, Russell, & Gold, 1997; Pandya, 2018). Interestingly, the expert judgment recommendations are correlated with the cost-effectiveness analysis recommendations (Kuntz, Tsevat, Weinstein, & Goldman, 1999). CPGs from any of these frameworks can experience the problems Falzer (2018) identifies when recommendations become requirements and nonconformance with them is penalized. However, physicians cannot simply be ordered to behave in a way that maximizes rewards or optimizes outcomes. The way they manage patients is based on long-established habit (Hamm, 2009a). Hearing about, reading, or studying an EB-CPG endorsed by a respected authority does not make physicians immediately change behavior, even if they intend to. They have to learn the recommended alternative behavior, recognize when the guideline describes something different from what they usually do, and make the conscious choice to change behavior. This must be done consciously until it can become a new habit (Abernathy & Hamm, 1995). This situation is more complex when the current habitual practice involves multiple actors (Ackerman, Gonzales, Stahl, & Metlay, 2013; Gonzales, Steiner, Lum, & Barrett, 1999),","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"209 - 214"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418782850","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45495868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Some Reflections on Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care 关于自然主义决策与卫生保健实践的几点思考
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418790715
P. Falzer
{"title":"Some Reflections on Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care","authors":"P. Falzer","doi":"10.1177/1555343418790715","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418790715","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"231 - 234"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418790715","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46178728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence-Based Medicine, Best Practices, Transductive Models, and Naturalistic Decision Making: Commentary on Paul R. Falzer, Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care 循证医学、最佳实践、传导模型和自然主义决策——保罗·法尔泽《自然主义决策与医疗实践》述评
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418789831
R. Haynes
Expert and informed decision making is an essential process in all of health care. Evidence-Based Medicine (EBM) purports to support and enhance this process by the timely infusion of high-quality, pertinent evidence from health research, tailored as closely as possible to the individual and their health problem. Doing so is not an easy task for many reasons, beginning with imperfections and incompleteness in the evidence and ending with the complexities of the dual decision making required by individuals and their care providers. EBM needs a lot of help supporting decision-making processes and welcomes further interdisciplinary collaboration. The “conformist principle,” “best practice regimens,” and “transductive models” should not be considered as barriers to such collaboration: These are not part of EBM. Rather, EBM has always seen evidence from health research as but one of many inputs to decision making by providers and patients. An overarching problem for collaboration to address is understanding the decision-making process well enough to develop effective means to bolster it, so that people are consistently offered the current best options for their problems in a way that fits their circumstances and that they can understand and judge.
专家和知情决策是所有卫生保健的一个基本过程。循证医学(EBM)旨在通过及时注入来自健康研究的高质量、相关证据来支持和加强这一进程,并尽可能密切地针对个人及其健康问题。要做到这一点并非易事,原因有很多,首先是证据的不完善和不完整,最后是个人及其护理提供者所要求的双重决策的复杂性。循证医学需要很多支持决策过程的帮助,并欢迎进一步的跨学科合作。“墨守成规的原则”、“最佳实践方案”和“转换模型”不应该被视为这种合作的障碍:这些不是循证医学的一部分。相反,循证医学一直把来自健康研究的证据视为提供者和患者决策的众多输入之一。协作要解决的一个首要问题是充分理解决策过程,以开发有效的手段来支持决策过程,这样人们就可以始终如一地以适合其环境的方式为其问题提供当前的最佳选择,并且他们可以理解和判断。
{"title":"Evidence-Based Medicine, Best Practices, Transductive Models, and Naturalistic Decision Making: Commentary on Paul R. Falzer, Naturalistic Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care","authors":"R. Haynes","doi":"10.1177/1555343418789831","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418789831","url":null,"abstract":"Expert and informed decision making is an essential process in all of health care. Evidence-Based Medicine (EBM) purports to support and enhance this process by the timely infusion of high-quality, pertinent evidence from health research, tailored as closely as possible to the individual and their health problem. Doing so is not an easy task for many reasons, beginning with imperfections and incompleteness in the evidence and ending with the complexities of the dual decision making required by individuals and their care providers. EBM needs a lot of help supporting decision-making processes and welcomes further interdisciplinary collaboration. The “conformist principle,” “best practice regimens,” and “transductive models” should not be considered as barriers to such collaboration: These are not part of EBM. Rather, EBM has always seen evidence from health research as but one of many inputs to decision making by providers and patients. An overarching problem for collaboration to address is understanding the decision-making process well enough to develop effective means to bolster it, so that people are consistently offered the current best options for their problems in a way that fits their circumstances and that they can understand and judge.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"194 - 197"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418789831","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48802814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
NDM and Healthcare Guidelines: More Attention to the Current Status, Complexity, and Context Is Needed NDM和医疗保健指南:需要更多地关注现状、复杂性和背景
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418778703
D. Matlock, R. Glasgow
Evidence-based medicine and guidelines cannot solve all problems in healthcare (Kemm, 2006). Indeed, it can be exceedingly frustrating for a clinician when the quality of his or her care gets measured based on adherence to guidelines that do not apply to a given patient (Boyd et al., 2005). Common examples of this are blood pressure and diabetes treatments for older adults. Lowering both blood pressure and glucose levels are important but can also be quite harmful for individual patients who are at higher risk for falls, incontinence, hypoglycemia, and cognitive impairment if either is controlled too aggressively. In this issue, Dr. Falzer (2018) contributes an article titled “Naturalistic Decision Making (NDM) and the Practice of Health Care.” He argues that the “best practices regimen”—an approach based on evidence and guidelines—has not worked due to a fundamental fallacy that they are overly simplistic and do not account for the nuances of modern medicine in the way that NDM could. He further asserts that implementation science approaches have not helped because they only serve to support and perpetuate the flawed “best practices regimen” approach. His point is well taken that some of the evidence and some (generally older) guidelines fall far short of providing guidance for the complex patient. However, the argument has important weaknesses. The reasoning seems to begin with a conclusion that is supported by an argument based on older thinking about implementation science and guidelines that support the a priori conclusion. This type of reasoning is a classic example of confirmation bias— a common risk when people are left to NDM approaches (Nickerson, 1998). One weakness of this paper is that it appears to be based on an outdated understanding of implementation science. Since the Lomas (Lomas et al., 1989) definition, the field of implementation science has evolved extensively and includes an understanding of how treatments reach the maximum number of eligible patients, how they are adapted to fit into different clinical contexts, how they are sustained, and how both changes in context and potential unintended consequences can be anticipated and avoided (Brownson, Colditz, & Proctor, 2017; Chambers, Glasgow, & Stange, 2013; Glasgow et al., 2012; Stirman et al., 2012). Since the articles referenced within the manuscript, there have been multiple advances in our understanding of both how to disseminate 778703 EDMXXX10.1177/1555343418778703Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingNdm and Healthcare Guidelines 2018
循证医学和指南不能解决医疗保健中的所有问题(Kemm,2006)。事实上,当临床医生根据不适用于特定患者的指南来衡量其护理质量时,这可能会让他们非常沮丧(Boyd等人,2005)。常见的例子是老年人的血压和糖尿病治疗。降低血压和血糖水平很重要,但如果控制得过于激进,对跌倒、失禁、低血糖和认知障碍风险较高的个别患者来说也会非常有害。在本期文章中,Falzer博士(2018)发表了一篇题为《自然主义决策与医疗保健实践》的文章。他认为,“最佳实践方案”——一种基于证据和指南的方法——没有奏效,因为存在一种根本的谬论,即它们过于简单化,没有像NDM那样考虑到现代医学的细微差别。他进一步断言,实施科学方法没有帮助,因为它们只会支持和延续有缺陷的“最佳实践方案”方法。他的观点得到了很好的理解,一些证据和一些(通常是旧的)指南远远不能为复杂的患者提供指导。然而,这一论点有重要的弱点。推理似乎始于一个结论,该结论得到了基于对实现科学和支持先验结论的指导方针的旧思想的论点的支持。这种类型的推理是确认偏见的一个经典例子——当人们被NDM方法所左右时,这是一种常见的风险(Nickerson,1998)。这篇论文的一个弱点是,它似乎是基于对实现科学的过时理解。自Lomas(Lomas et al.,1989)定义以来,实施科学领域已经发生了广泛的发展,包括了解治疗如何达到符合条件的患者的最大数量,如何适应不同的临床环境,如何持续,以及如何预测和避免背景变化和潜在的意外后果(Brownson,Colditz,&Proctor,2017;Chambers,Glasgow,&Stange,2013;Glasgow等人,2012年;Stirman等人,2012)。自手稿中引用的文章以来,我们对如何传播778703 EDMXX10.1177/155533418778703《认知工程与决策杂志》和《2018年医疗保健指南》的理解取得了多项进展
{"title":"NDM and Healthcare Guidelines: More Attention to the Current Status, Complexity, and Context Is Needed","authors":"D. Matlock, R. Glasgow","doi":"10.1177/1555343418778703","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418778703","url":null,"abstract":"Evidence-based medicine and guidelines cannot solve all problems in healthcare (Kemm, 2006). Indeed, it can be exceedingly frustrating for a clinician when the quality of his or her care gets measured based on adherence to guidelines that do not apply to a given patient (Boyd et al., 2005). Common examples of this are blood pressure and diabetes treatments for older adults. Lowering both blood pressure and glucose levels are important but can also be quite harmful for individual patients who are at higher risk for falls, incontinence, hypoglycemia, and cognitive impairment if either is controlled too aggressively. In this issue, Dr. Falzer (2018) contributes an article titled “Naturalistic Decision Making (NDM) and the Practice of Health Care.” He argues that the “best practices regimen”—an approach based on evidence and guidelines—has not worked due to a fundamental fallacy that they are overly simplistic and do not account for the nuances of modern medicine in the way that NDM could. He further asserts that implementation science approaches have not helped because they only serve to support and perpetuate the flawed “best practices regimen” approach. His point is well taken that some of the evidence and some (generally older) guidelines fall far short of providing guidance for the complex patient. However, the argument has important weaknesses. The reasoning seems to begin with a conclusion that is supported by an argument based on older thinking about implementation science and guidelines that support the a priori conclusion. This type of reasoning is a classic example of confirmation bias— a common risk when people are left to NDM approaches (Nickerson, 1998). One weakness of this paper is that it appears to be based on an outdated understanding of implementation science. Since the Lomas (Lomas et al., 1989) definition, the field of implementation science has evolved extensively and includes an understanding of how treatments reach the maximum number of eligible patients, how they are adapted to fit into different clinical contexts, how they are sustained, and how both changes in context and potential unintended consequences can be anticipated and avoided (Brownson, Colditz, & Proctor, 2017; Chambers, Glasgow, & Stange, 2013; Glasgow et al., 2012; Stirman et al., 2012). Since the articles referenced within the manuscript, there have been multiple advances in our understanding of both how to disseminate 778703 EDMXXX10.1177/1555343418778703Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingNdm and Healthcare Guidelines 2018","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"202 - 205"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418778703","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49605475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Controlling Versus Supporting in a Sociotechnical System: A Commentary on Falzer (2018) 社会技术系统中的控制与支持:对Falzer(2018)的评论
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418777342
Yan Xiao, P. Gorman
{"title":"Controlling Versus Supporting in a Sociotechnical System: A Commentary on Falzer (2018)","authors":"Yan Xiao, P. Gorman","doi":"10.1177/1555343418777342","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418777342","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"215 - 218"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418777342","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48337403","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Industrial Conceptualization of Health Care Versus the Naturalistic Decision-Making Paradigm: Work as Imagined Versus Work as Done 医疗保健的工业概念化与自然主义决策范式:想象的工作与完成的工作
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2018-08-16 DOI: 10.1177/1555343418774661
K. Catchpole, Myrtede C. Alfred
Quality and safety concerns in health care over the past 20 years precipitated the need to move beyond the traditional view of health care as an artisanal process toward a sociotechnical systems view of performance. The adoption of industrial approaches placed a greater emphasis on standardization of processes and outcomes, often treating humans as the “weak” part of the system rather than valuing their role in holding together complex, opaque, and unpredictable clinical systems. Although some health care tasks can be modeled linearly, others are much more complex. Efforts to reduce variation in clinical reasoning through evidence-based practices have proven problematic by failing to provide a means for context-specific adaptation or to account for the complex and adaptive nature of clinical work. We argue that the current, highly empirical approach to clinical decision making reflects clinical reasoning “as imagined,” whereas the application of the naturalistic decision-making (NDM) paradigm can help reveal clinical reasoning “as done.” This approach will have benefits for improving the conditions for diagnosis; the design of acute, time-pressured clinical work; the identification of deteriorating patients; the development of clinical decision support systems; and many more clinical tasks. Health care seems ready to accept NDM approaches.
过去20年来,医疗保健的质量和安全问题促使人们需要超越传统的医疗保健视为一个手工过程的观点,转向社会技术系统的绩效观。工业方法的采用更加强调过程和结果的标准化,通常将人类视为系统的“薄弱”部分,而不是重视他们在将复杂、不透明和不可预测的临床系统结合在一起方面的作用。尽管一些医疗保健任务可以线性建模,但其他任务则要复杂得多。事实证明,通过循证实践减少临床推理差异的努力存在问题,因为未能提供针对具体情况的适应手段,也未能解释临床工作的复杂性和适应性。我们认为,目前高度实证的临床决策方法反映了临床推理“如想象的那样”,而自然决策(NDM)范式的应用可以帮助揭示临床推理“如常”。这种方法将有利于改善诊断条件;急性、时间紧迫的临床工作的设计;识别病情恶化的患者;临床决策支持系统的开发;以及更多的临床任务。医疗保健似乎已经准备好接受NDM方法。
{"title":"Industrial Conceptualization of Health Care Versus the Naturalistic Decision-Making Paradigm: Work as Imagined Versus Work as Done","authors":"K. Catchpole, Myrtede C. Alfred","doi":"10.1177/1555343418774661","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418774661","url":null,"abstract":"Quality and safety concerns in health care over the past 20 years precipitated the need to move beyond the traditional view of health care as an artisanal process toward a sociotechnical systems view of performance. The adoption of industrial approaches placed a greater emphasis on standardization of processes and outcomes, often treating humans as the “weak” part of the system rather than valuing their role in holding together complex, opaque, and unpredictable clinical systems. Although some health care tasks can be modeled linearly, others are much more complex. Efforts to reduce variation in clinical reasoning through evidence-based practices have proven problematic by failing to provide a means for context-specific adaptation or to account for the complex and adaptive nature of clinical work. We argue that the current, highly empirical approach to clinical decision making reflects clinical reasoning “as imagined,” whereas the application of the naturalistic decision-making (NDM) paradigm can help reveal clinical reasoning “as done.” This approach will have benefits for improving the conditions for diagnosis; the design of acute, time-pressured clinical work; the identification of deteriorating patients; the development of clinical decision support systems; and many more clinical tasks. Health care seems ready to accept NDM approaches.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"222 - 226"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418774661","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42145498","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
期刊
Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1