首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Team Situation Awareness and Conflict: A Study of Human–Machine Teaming 团队情境意识与冲突:人机协同研究
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2021-05-19 DOI: 10.1177/15553434211017354
Nathan J. Mcneese, Mustafa Demir, Nancy J. Cooke, Manrong She
This article focuses on two fundamental human–human teamwork behaviors and seeks to understand them better in human–machine teams. Specifically, team situation awareness (TSA) and team conflict are examined in human–machine teams. There is a significant need to identify how TSA and team conflict occur during human–machine teaming, in addition to how they impact each other. In this work, we present an experiment aimed at understanding TSA and team conflict in the context of human–machine teaming in a remotely piloted aircraft system (RPAS). Three conditions were tested: (1) control: teams consisted of all humans; (2) synthetic: teams consisted of the pilot role being occupied by a computational agent based on ACT-R architecture that employed AI capabilities, with all other team roles being humans; and (3) experimenter: an experimenter playing the role of the pilot as a highly effective computational agent, with the other roles being humans. The results indicate that TSA improved over time in synthetic teams, improved and then stabilized over time in experimenter teams, and did not improve in control teams. In addition, results show that control teams had the most team conflict. Finally, in the control condition, team conflict negatively impacts TSA.
本文关注两种基本的人-人团队合作行为,并试图在人机团队中更好地理解它们。具体而言,团队情境意识(TSA)和团队冲突在人机团队中进行了检验。除了TSA和团队冲突如何相互影响外,还需要确定它们在人机团队合作过程中是如何发生的。在这项工作中,我们提出了一个实验,旨在了解远程驾驶飞机系统(RPAS)中人机协同背景下的TSA和团队冲突。测试了三种条件:(1)对照:团队由所有人组成;(2) 合成:团队由飞行员角色组成,由基于ACT-R架构的计算代理占据,该架构使用了人工智能能力,所有其他团队角色都是人类;和(3)实验者:实验者扮演飞行员的角色,作为一个高效的计算代理,其他角色是人类。结果表明,TSA在合成团队中随着时间的推移而改善,在实验者团队中随时间的推移不断改善然后稳定,而在对照团队中没有改善。此外,研究结果显示,控制团队的团队冲突最多。最后,在控制条件下,团队冲突对TSA产生负面影响。
{"title":"Team Situation Awareness and Conflict: A Study of Human–Machine Teaming","authors":"Nathan J. Mcneese, Mustafa Demir, Nancy J. Cooke, Manrong She","doi":"10.1177/15553434211017354","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15553434211017354","url":null,"abstract":"This article focuses on two fundamental human–human teamwork behaviors and seeks to understand them better in human–machine teams. Specifically, team situation awareness (TSA) and team conflict are examined in human–machine teams. There is a significant need to identify how TSA and team conflict occur during human–machine teaming, in addition to how they impact each other. In this work, we present an experiment aimed at understanding TSA and team conflict in the context of human–machine teaming in a remotely piloted aircraft system (RPAS). Three conditions were tested: (1) control: teams consisted of all humans; (2) synthetic: teams consisted of the pilot role being occupied by a computational agent based on ACT-R architecture that employed AI capabilities, with all other team roles being humans; and (3) experimenter: an experimenter playing the role of the pilot as a highly effective computational agent, with the other roles being humans. The results indicate that TSA improved over time in synthetic teams, improved and then stabilized over time in experimenter teams, and did not improve in control teams. In addition, results show that control teams had the most team conflict. Finally, in the control condition, team conflict negatively impacts TSA.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"15 1","pages":"83 - 96"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/15553434211017354","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45469288","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 19
Exploring the Impact of Coordination in Human–Agent Teams 人力-代理团队中协调的影响探讨
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2021-05-16 DOI: 10.1177/15553434211010573
Michael F. Schneider, Michael Miller, D. Jacques, Gilbert L. Peterson, Thomas C. Ford
Teaming permits cognitively complex work to be rapidly executed by multiple entities. As artificial agents (AAs) participate in increasingly complex cognitive work, they hold the promise of moving beyond tools to becoming effective members of human–agent teams. Coordination has been identified as the critical process that enables effective teams and is required to achieve the vision of tightly coupled teams of humans and AAs. This paper characterizes coordination on the axes of types, content, and cost. This characterization is grounded in the human and AA literature and is evaluated to extract design implications for human–agent teams. These design implications are the mechanisms, moderators, and models employed within human–agent teams, which illuminate potential AA design improvements to support coordination.
团队合作允许多个实体快速执行认知复杂的工作。随着人工智能体参与越来越复杂的认知工作,它们有望超越工具,成为人类智能体团队的有效成员。协调已被确定为实现有效团队的关键过程,也是实现人类和AAs紧密耦合团队愿景所必需的。本文在类型、内容和成本的轴上描述了协调。这种表征基于人类和AA文献,并进行评估,以提取人类-代理团队的设计含义。这些设计含义是人类-代理团队中使用的机制、调节器和模型,它们阐明了支持协调的潜在AA设计改进。
{"title":"Exploring the Impact of Coordination in Human–Agent Teams","authors":"Michael F. Schneider, Michael Miller, D. Jacques, Gilbert L. Peterson, Thomas C. Ford","doi":"10.1177/15553434211010573","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15553434211010573","url":null,"abstract":"Teaming permits cognitively complex work to be rapidly executed by multiple entities. As artificial agents (AAs) participate in increasingly complex cognitive work, they hold the promise of moving beyond tools to becoming effective members of human–agent teams. Coordination has been identified as the critical process that enables effective teams and is required to achieve the vision of tightly coupled teams of humans and AAs. This paper characterizes coordination on the axes of types, content, and cost. This characterization is grounded in the human and AA literature and is evaluated to extract design implications for human–agent teams. These design implications are the mechanisms, moderators, and models employed within human–agent teams, which illuminate potential AA design improvements to support coordination.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"15 1","pages":"97 - 115"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/15553434211010573","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41399957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Levels of What?Investigating Drivers’ Understanding of Different Levels of Automation in Vehicles 什么水平?调查驾驶员对不同级别车辆自动化的理解程度
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2021-04-17 DOI: 10.1177/15553434211009024
Fjollë Novakazi, Mikael Johansson, Helena Strömberg, M. Karlsson
Extant levels of automation (LoAs) taxonomies describe variations in function allocations between the driver and the driving automation system (DAS) from a technical perspective. However, these taxonomies miss important human factors issues and when design decisions are based on them, the resulting interaction design leaves users confused. Therefore, the aim of this paper is to describe how users perceive different DASs by eliciting insights from an empirical driving study facilitating a Wizard-of-Oz approach, where 20 participants were interviewed after experiencing systems on two different LoAs under real driving conditions. The findings show that participants talked about the DAS by describing different relationships and dependencies between three different elements: the context (traffic conditions, road types), the vehicle (abilities, limitations, vehicle operations), and the driver (control, attentional demand, interaction with displays and controls, operation of vehicle), each with associated aspects that indicate what users identify as relevant when describing a vehicle with automated systems. Based on these findings, a conceptual model is proposed by which designers can differentiate LoAs from a human-centric perspective and that can aid in the development of design guidelines for driving automation.
现有的自动化级别(LoAs)分类法从技术角度描述了驾驶员和驾驶自动化系统(DAS)之间功能分配的变化。然而,这些分类法忽略了重要的人为因素问题,当设计决策基于这些因素时,最终的交互设计会让用户感到困惑。因此,本文的目的是通过从一项促进绿野仙踪方法的实证驾驶研究中得出见解来描述用户如何感知不同的das,其中20名参与者在真实驾驶条件下体验了两种不同的LoAs系统后接受了采访。研究结果表明,参与者通过描述三个不同元素之间的不同关系和依赖关系来谈论DAS:上下文(交通状况、道路类型)、车辆(能力、限制、车辆操作)和驾驶员(控制、注意力需求、与显示器和控制的交互、车辆操作),每个元素都有相关的方面,表明用户在描述具有自动化系统的车辆时认为哪些是相关的。基于这些发现,本文提出了一个概念模型,设计师可以从以人为中心的角度来区分loa,这有助于制定驱动自动化的设计指南。
{"title":"Levels of What?Investigating Drivers’ Understanding of Different Levels of Automation in Vehicles","authors":"Fjollë Novakazi, Mikael Johansson, Helena Strömberg, M. Karlsson","doi":"10.1177/15553434211009024","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15553434211009024","url":null,"abstract":"Extant levels of automation (LoAs) taxonomies describe variations in function allocations between the driver and the driving automation system (DAS) from a technical perspective. However, these taxonomies miss important human factors issues and when design decisions are based on them, the resulting interaction design leaves users confused. Therefore, the aim of this paper is to describe how users perceive different DASs by eliciting insights from an empirical driving study facilitating a Wizard-of-Oz approach, where 20 participants were interviewed after experiencing systems on two different LoAs under real driving conditions. The findings show that participants talked about the DAS by describing different relationships and dependencies between three different elements: the context (traffic conditions, road types), the vehicle (abilities, limitations, vehicle operations), and the driver (control, attentional demand, interaction with displays and controls, operation of vehicle), each with associated aspects that indicate what users identify as relevant when describing a vehicle with automated systems. Based on these findings, a conceptual model is proposed by which designers can differentiate LoAs from a human-centric perspective and that can aid in the development of design guidelines for driving automation.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"15 1","pages":"116 - 132"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/15553434211009024","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46126842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Team Communication Patterns in Critical Situations 关键情况下的团队沟通模式
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2021-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420986657
Floris van den Oever, J. Schraagen
Teams operating in time-pressured, dynamic environments frequently need to cope with critical situations varying in complexity and hazard. To cope with critical situations, teams may have to adapt their communication processes. Adaptation of team communication processes has been studied mostly at short time frames (minutes). Literature on adapting communication at longer time frames is limited (hours, relative to minutes). We used the relational event model to compare team communication in critical and noncritical situations of pediatric cardiac surgeries and Apollo 13 flight director’s voice loops. Teams showed some flattening of communication structures in critical situations. Both teams maintained institutional roles and displayed closed-loop and information-seeking communication. Communication patterns may change further with increasing criticality. The exact way teams adapt to critical situations may differ depending on team, team size and situation. Findings may inform team training procedures or team structure development.
在时间紧张、动态环境中工作的团队经常需要应对复杂程度和危险性不同的关键情况。为了应对关键情况,团队可能必须调整他们的沟通流程。团队沟通过程的适应性研究大多在短时间(分钟)内进行。关于在较长时间框架内调整沟通的文献有限(小时,相对于分钟)。我们使用关系事件模型来比较儿科心脏手术的关键和非关键情况下的团队沟通以及阿波罗13号飞行指导的语音循环。团队在危急情况下表现出一定程度的沟通结构扁平化。两个团队都保持着机构角色,并表现出闭环和信息寻求沟通。通信模式可能会随着关键性的增加而进一步改变。团队适应关键情况的确切方式可能因团队、团队规模和情况而异。调查结果可能会为团队培训程序或团队结构发展提供信息。
{"title":"Team Communication Patterns in Critical Situations","authors":"Floris van den Oever, J. Schraagen","doi":"10.1177/1555343420986657","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420986657","url":null,"abstract":"Teams operating in time-pressured, dynamic environments frequently need to cope with critical situations varying in complexity and hazard. To cope with critical situations, teams may have to adapt their communication processes. Adaptation of team communication processes has been studied mostly at short time frames (minutes). Literature on adapting communication at longer time frames is limited (hours, relative to minutes). We used the relational event model to compare team communication in critical and noncritical situations of pediatric cardiac surgeries and Apollo 13 flight director’s voice loops. Teams showed some flattening of communication structures in critical situations. Both teams maintained institutional roles and displayed closed-loop and information-seeking communication. Communication patterns may change further with increasing criticality. The exact way teams adapt to critical situations may differ depending on team, team size and situation. Findings may inform team training procedures or team structure development.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"15 1","pages":"28 - 51"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420986657","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43805067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Addressing Driver Disengagement and Proper System Use: Human Factors Recommendations for Level 2 Driving Automation Design 解决驾驶员脱离和正确使用系统的问题:2级驾驶自动化设计的人为因素建议
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2021-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420983126
Alexandra S. Mueller, I. Reagan, Jessica B. Cicchino
Level 2 driving automation has the potential to reduce crashes; however, there are known risks when using these systems, particularly as they relate to drivers becoming disengaged from driving. This paper provides data-driven recommendations for Level 2 driving automation design using the best currently available methods to encourage driver engagement and communicate where and how a system can safely be used. Our recommendations pertaining to driver engagement concern driver management systems that monitor the driver for signs of disengagement and return the driver to the loop using a multimodal escalation process with attention reminders, countermeasures for sustained noncompliance to the attention reminders, and proactive methods for keeping drivers engaged with respect to driver-system interactions and system functionality considerations. We also provide guidance on how the operational design domain (ODD), driver responsibilities, and system limitations should be communicated and how these systems must be self-limited within the ODD. In addition, we discuss the benefits and limitations of training to emphasize the importance of making these systems intuitive to all users, regardless of training, to ensure proper use. These recommendations should be applied as a whole, because selectively adhering to only some may inadvertently exacerbate the dangers of driver disengagement.
2级自动驾驶有可能减少交通事故;然而,在使用这些系统时存在已知的风险,特别是当它们与驾驶员脱离驾驶有关时。本文为二级驾驶自动化设计提供了数据驱动的建议,使用当前可用的最佳方法来鼓励驾驶员参与,并就系统在何处以及如何安全使用进行沟通。我们关于驾驶员参与的建议涉及驾驶员管理系统,该系统可以监控驾驶员脱离的迹象,并使用带有注意提醒的多模式升级过程、持续不遵守注意提醒的对策、以及保持驾驶员参与驾驶员与系统交互和系统功能考虑的主动方法将驾驶员返回到循环中。我们还提供了如何沟通操作设计域(ODD)、驱动程序职责和系统限制的指导,以及这些系统必须如何在ODD中自我限制。此外,我们讨论了培训的好处和局限性,以强调使这些系统对所有用户直观的重要性,无论培训如何,以确保正确使用。这些建议应作为一个整体加以应用,因为有选择地只遵守其中一些建议可能会无意中加剧驾驶员脱离接触的危险。
{"title":"Addressing Driver Disengagement and Proper System Use: Human Factors Recommendations for Level 2 Driving Automation Design","authors":"Alexandra S. Mueller, I. Reagan, Jessica B. Cicchino","doi":"10.1177/1555343420983126","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420983126","url":null,"abstract":"Level 2 driving automation has the potential to reduce crashes; however, there are known risks when using these systems, particularly as they relate to drivers becoming disengaged from driving. This paper provides data-driven recommendations for Level 2 driving automation design using the best currently available methods to encourage driver engagement and communicate where and how a system can safely be used. Our recommendations pertaining to driver engagement concern driver management systems that monitor the driver for signs of disengagement and return the driver to the loop using a multimodal escalation process with attention reminders, countermeasures for sustained noncompliance to the attention reminders, and proactive methods for keeping drivers engaged with respect to driver-system interactions and system functionality considerations. We also provide guidance on how the operational design domain (ODD), driver responsibilities, and system limitations should be communicated and how these systems must be self-limited within the ODD. In addition, we discuss the benefits and limitations of training to emphasize the importance of making these systems intuitive to all users, regardless of training, to ensure proper use. These recommendations should be applied as a whole, because selectively adhering to only some may inadvertently exacerbate the dangers of driver disengagement.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"15 1","pages":"3 - 27"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420983126","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44151241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
Combining Cognitive Task Analysis and Participatory Design Methods to Elicit and Represent Task Flows 结合认知任务分析和参与式设计方法来引出和表示任务流
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2020-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420976014
E. Papautsky, Robert V Strouse, Cindy Dominguez
Representations of step-by-step procedures, such as task flows, are developed and used to support technology design and evaluation as well as for training purposes in complex work domains. However, task flows may not represent how people carry out work under uncertainty, time pressure, or high-risk conditions. We combined methods of cognitive task analysis and participatory design, resulting in a new approach for developing task flows. This approach accounts for both cognitive and behavioral work and explicitly represents its dynamic nature. Additional advantages of this approach include flexibility and adaptiveness to help overcome challenges of conducting research in real-world domains, including time constraints and access to subject matter experts. We demonstrate this approach in the context of developing a task flow for a submarine watch team’s use of an updated imaging system to maintain the ship’s safety by forming and maintaining a picture of the external environment. We provide a detailed description of each phase as well as a domain-neutral ready-to-use job aid.
逐步程序的表示,如任务流,被开发和用于支持技术设计和评估,以及用于复杂工作领域的培训目的。然而,任务流可能并不代表人们如何在不确定性、时间压力或高风险条件下开展工作。我们将认知任务分析和参与式设计相结合,形成了一种开发任务流的新方法。这种方法既考虑了认知工作,也考虑了行为工作,并明确地代表了其动态性质。这种方法的其他优势包括灵活性和适应性,有助于克服在现实世界领域进行研究的挑战,包括时间限制和接触主题专家。我们在为潜艇观察小组开发任务流程的背景下展示了这种方法,该任务流程使用更新的成像系统,通过形成和维护外部环境的图像来维护船只的安全。我们提供了每个阶段的详细描述,以及一个领域中立的随时可用的工作援助。
{"title":"Combining Cognitive Task Analysis and Participatory Design Methods to Elicit and Represent Task Flows","authors":"E. Papautsky, Robert V Strouse, Cindy Dominguez","doi":"10.1177/1555343420976014","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420976014","url":null,"abstract":"Representations of step-by-step procedures, such as task flows, are developed and used to support technology design and evaluation as well as for training purposes in complex work domains. However, task flows may not represent how people carry out work under uncertainty, time pressure, or high-risk conditions. We combined methods of cognitive task analysis and participatory design, resulting in a new approach for developing task flows. This approach accounts for both cognitive and behavioral work and explicitly represents its dynamic nature. Additional advantages of this approach include flexibility and adaptiveness to help overcome challenges of conducting research in real-world domains, including time constraints and access to subject matter experts. We demonstrate this approach in the context of developing a task flow for a submarine watch team’s use of an updated imaging system to maintain the ship’s safety by forming and maintaining a picture of the external environment. We provide a detailed description of each phase as well as a domain-neutral ready-to-use job aid.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"14 1","pages":"288 - 301"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420976014","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45533247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Evaluation of Cognitive Skill Degradation in Flight Planning 飞行计划中认知技能退化的评估
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2020-11-09 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420962897
K. Volz, M. Dorneich
This work aimed to identify cognitive skills associated with flight planning, suggest which skills might be susceptible to skill degradation, and investigate the effects of cognitive skill degradation over time. Information automation systems offload cognitive tasks to reduce workload and error. However, the same phenomena seen with physical skill degradation in highly automated aircrafts may also occur when automating cognitive tasks. Two studies were conducted. An applied cognitive task analysis identified cognitive skills in flight planning. An empirical evaluation examined whether some of those skills were susceptible to cognitive skill degradation over time when using automation. Participants were placed into three groups. After conducting a flight planning task manually, groups differed in the next three practice trials: manual, alternating between manual and automation, or only with automation. Finally, all groups conducted the task manually again. Trials were separated by 2 weeks. The automation group showed the most performance degradation and highest workload, while the manual group showed the least performance degradation and least workload. Automation use did not provide the practice needed to mitigate cognitive skill degradation. Analysis of the impacts of information automation on cognitive performance is a first step in understanding the root causes of errors and developing mitigations.
这项工作旨在确定与飞行计划相关的认知技能,提出哪些技能可能容易受到技能退化的影响,并调查认知技能退化对时间的影响。信息自动化系统卸载认知任务以减少工作量和错误。然而,在高度自动化的飞机上,当自动化认知任务时,也可能出现物理技能下降的相同现象。进行了两项研究。一项应用认知任务分析确定了飞行计划中的认知技能。一项实证评估考察了在使用自动化时,随着时间的推移,其中一些技能是否容易受到认知技能退化的影响。参与者被分为三组。在手动执行飞行计划任务后,小组在接下来的三次练习试验中有所不同:手动、手动和自动化交替,或者仅使用自动化。最后,所有小组再次手动执行任务。试验间隔2周。自动化组表现出最大的性能下降和最高的工作负载,而手动组表现出最小的性能下降,最小的工作负载。自动化的使用并没有提供缓解认知技能退化所需的实践。分析信息自动化对认知表现的影响是理解错误根源和制定缓解措施的第一步。
{"title":"Evaluation of Cognitive Skill Degradation in Flight Planning","authors":"K. Volz, M. Dorneich","doi":"10.1177/1555343420962897","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420962897","url":null,"abstract":"This work aimed to identify cognitive skills associated with flight planning, suggest which skills might be susceptible to skill degradation, and investigate the effects of cognitive skill degradation over time. Information automation systems offload cognitive tasks to reduce workload and error. However, the same phenomena seen with physical skill degradation in highly automated aircrafts may also occur when automating cognitive tasks. Two studies were conducted. An applied cognitive task analysis identified cognitive skills in flight planning. An empirical evaluation examined whether some of those skills were susceptible to cognitive skill degradation over time when using automation. Participants were placed into three groups. After conducting a flight planning task manually, groups differed in the next three practice trials: manual, alternating between manual and automation, or only with automation. Finally, all groups conducted the task manually again. Trials were separated by 2 weeks. The automation group showed the most performance degradation and highest workload, while the manual group showed the least performance degradation and least workload. Automation use did not provide the practice needed to mitigate cognitive skill degradation. Analysis of the impacts of information automation on cognitive performance is a first step in understanding the root causes of errors and developing mitigations.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"14 1","pages":"263 - 287"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-11-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420962897","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47754958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Swift Trust in Ad Hoc Teams: A Cognitive Task Analysis of Intelligence Operators in Multi-Domain Command and Control Contexts 特设小组中的快速信任:多域指挥与控制情境下情报操作者的认知任务分析
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2020-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420943460
August A. Capiola, H. Baxter, M. Pfahler, Christopher S. Calhoun, P. Bobko
Trust is important for establishing successful relationships and performance outcomes. In some contexts, however, rich information such as knowledge of and experience with a teammate is not available to inform one’s trust. Yet, parties in these contexts are expected to work together toward common goals for a relatively brief and finite period of time. This research investigated the antecedents to quickly-formed trust (often referred to as swift trust) in fast-paced, time-constrained contexts. We conducted a cognitive task analysis (CTA) based on 11 structured interviews of subject-matter experts (SMEs) in Intelligence (Intel)—a heterogeneous job category comprising distributed and co-located personnel within multi-domain command and control (MDC2) environments. Eight antecedents to swift trust emerged from these interviews (i.e., ability, integrity, benevolence, communication, mission-focus, self-awareness, shared perspectives/experiences, and calm), with further analysis implying that swift trust is a relevant and emergent state in MDC2 that facilitates reliance. These findings offer implications for teams operating in high-risk distributed contexts and should be expanded through basic experimental investigations as well as applied initiatives.
信任对于建立成功的关系和绩效结果非常重要。然而,在某些情况下,丰富的信息,如对队友的了解和经验,无法告知一个人的信任。然而,在这种情况下,各方预计将在相对短暂和有限的时间内为实现共同目标而共同努力。这项研究调查了在快节奏、时间限制的环境中快速形成信任(通常被称为快速信任)的前因。我们基于对情报(Intel)主题专家(SME)的11次结构化访谈进行了认知任务分析(CTA),这是一个异构的工作类别,包括多领域指挥控制(MDC2)环境中的分布式和协同工作人员。从这些访谈中得出了快速信任的八个前因(即能力、正直、仁爱、沟通、使命专注、自我意识、共同的观点/经验和冷静),进一步的分析表明,快速信任是MDC2中一种相关的、新兴的状态,有助于依赖。这些发现为在高风险分布式环境中运作的团队提供了启示,应通过基本的实验调查和应用举措加以扩展。
{"title":"Swift Trust in Ad Hoc Teams: A Cognitive Task Analysis of Intelligence Operators in Multi-Domain Command and Control Contexts","authors":"August A. Capiola, H. Baxter, M. Pfahler, Christopher S. Calhoun, P. Bobko","doi":"10.1177/1555343420943460","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420943460","url":null,"abstract":"Trust is important for establishing successful relationships and performance outcomes. In some contexts, however, rich information such as knowledge of and experience with a teammate is not available to inform one’s trust. Yet, parties in these contexts are expected to work together toward common goals for a relatively brief and finite period of time. This research investigated the antecedents to quickly-formed trust (often referred to as swift trust) in fast-paced, time-constrained contexts. We conducted a cognitive task analysis (CTA) based on 11 structured interviews of subject-matter experts (SMEs) in Intelligence (Intel)—a heterogeneous job category comprising distributed and co-located personnel within multi-domain command and control (MDC2) environments. Eight antecedents to swift trust emerged from these interviews (i.e., ability, integrity, benevolence, communication, mission-focus, self-awareness, shared perspectives/experiences, and calm), with further analysis implying that swift trust is a relevant and emergent state in MDC2 that facilitates reliance. These findings offer implications for teams operating in high-risk distributed contexts and should be expanded through basic experimental investigations as well as applied initiatives.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"14 1","pages":"218 - 241"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420943460","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45273401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Helicopter Pilots’ Tasks, Subjective Workload, and the Role of External Visual Cues During Shipboard Landing 直升机飞行员的任务、主观工作量和外部视觉线索在舰载降落中的作用
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2020-08-26 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420948720
M. Tušl, G. Rainieri, F. Fraboni, Marco de Angelis, M. Depolo, L. Pietrantoni, Andrea Pingitore
Helicopter shipboard landing is a cognitively complex task that is challenging both for pilots and their crew. Effective communication, accurate reading of the flight instruments, as well as monitoring of the external environment are crucial for a successful landing. In particular, the final phases of landing are critical as they imply high workload situations in an unstable environment with restricted space. In the present qualitative study, we interviewed ten helicopter pilots from the Italian Navy using an applied cognitive task analysis approach. We aimed to obtain a detailed description of the landing procedure, and to identify relevant factors that affect pilots’ workload, performance, and safety. Based on the content analysis of the interviews, we have identified six distinct phases of approaching and landing on a ship deck and four categories of factors that may significantly affect pilots’ performance and safety of the landing procedure. Consistent with previous studies, our findings suggest that external visual cueing is vital for a successful landing, in particular during the last phases of landing. Therefore, based on the pilots’ statements, we provide suggestions for possible improvements of external visual cues that have the potential to reduce pilots’ workload and improve the overall safety of landing operations.
直升机舰载降落是一项复杂的认知任务,对飞行员和机组人员都是一项挑战。有效的通信、飞行仪表的准确读数以及外部环境的监测对于成功着陆至关重要。特别是着陆的最后阶段至关重要,因为这意味着在空间有限的不稳定环境中需要高工作量。在目前的定性研究中,我们使用应用认知任务分析方法采访了意大利海军的10名直升机飞行员。我们的目的是获得着陆程序的详细描述,并确定影响飞行员工作量、性能和安全的相关因素。根据访谈的内容分析,我们确定了在船舶甲板上接近和降落的六个不同阶段,以及可能显著影响飞行员着陆过程的性能和安全的四类因素。与之前的研究一致,我们的研究结果表明,外部视觉线索对成功着陆至关重要,尤其是在着陆的最后阶段。因此,根据飞行员的陈述,我们提出了可能的外部视觉线索改进建议,这些建议有可能减少飞行员的工作量,提高着陆操作的整体安全性。
{"title":"Helicopter Pilots’ Tasks, Subjective Workload, and the Role of External Visual Cues During Shipboard Landing","authors":"M. Tušl, G. Rainieri, F. Fraboni, Marco de Angelis, M. Depolo, L. Pietrantoni, Andrea Pingitore","doi":"10.1177/1555343420948720","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420948720","url":null,"abstract":"Helicopter shipboard landing is a cognitively complex task that is challenging both for pilots and their crew. Effective communication, accurate reading of the flight instruments, as well as monitoring of the external environment are crucial for a successful landing. In particular, the final phases of landing are critical as they imply high workload situations in an unstable environment with restricted space. In the present qualitative study, we interviewed ten helicopter pilots from the Italian Navy using an applied cognitive task analysis approach. We aimed to obtain a detailed description of the landing procedure, and to identify relevant factors that affect pilots’ workload, performance, and safety. Based on the content analysis of the interviews, we have identified six distinct phases of approaching and landing on a ship deck and four categories of factors that may significantly affect pilots’ performance and safety of the landing procedure. Consistent with previous studies, our findings suggest that external visual cueing is vital for a successful landing, in particular during the last phases of landing. Therefore, based on the pilots’ statements, we provide suggestions for possible improvements of external visual cues that have the potential to reduce pilots’ workload and improve the overall safety of landing operations.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"14 1","pages":"242 - 257"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420948720","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48936489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Improving Analytic Reasoning via Crowdsourcing and Structured Analytic Techniques 通过众包和结构化分析技术改进分析推理
IF 2 Q3 ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL Pub Date : 2020-08-17 DOI: 10.1177/1555343420926287
T. van Gelder, Ariel Kruger, Sujai Thomman, Richard de Rozario, Elizabeth Silver, Morgan Saletta, Ashley Barnett, R. Sinnott, G. Jayaputera, M. Burgman
How might analytic reasoning in intelligence reports be substantially improved? One conjecture is that this can be achieved through a combination of crowdsourcing and structured analytic techniques (SATs). To explore this conjecture, we developed a new crowdsourcing platform supporting groups in collaborative reasoning and intelligence report drafting using a novel SAT we call “Contending Analyses.” In this paper we present findings from a large study designed to assess whether groups of professional analysts working on the platform produce better-reasoned reports than those analysts produce when using methods and tools normally used in their organizations. Secondary questions were whether professional analysts working on the platform produce better reasoning than the general public working on the platform; and how usable the platform is. Our main finding is a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 1.37) in favor of working on platform. This provides early support for the general conjecture. We discuss limitations of our study, implications for intelligence organizations, and future directions for the work as a whole.
如何实质性地改进情报报告中的分析推理?一种推测是,这可以通过众包和结构化分析技术(SAT)的结合来实现。为了探索这一猜测,我们开发了一个新的众包平台,支持团队使用一种新的SAT进行协作推理和情报报告起草,我们称之为“竞争分析”。“在这篇论文中,我们介绍了一项大型研究的结果,该研究旨在评估在平台上工作的专业分析师群体在使用其组织中通常使用的方法和工具时,是否会产生比这些分析师更合理的报告。次要问题是,在该平台上工作的专业分析师是否比在该平台工作的普通公众产生了更好的推理;以及平台的可用性。我们的主要发现是有利于在平台上工作的大效应大小(Cohen的d=1.37)。这为一般猜想提供了早期的支持。我们讨论了我们研究的局限性,对情报机构的影响,以及整个工作的未来方向。
{"title":"Improving Analytic Reasoning via Crowdsourcing and Structured Analytic Techniques","authors":"T. van Gelder, Ariel Kruger, Sujai Thomman, Richard de Rozario, Elizabeth Silver, Morgan Saletta, Ashley Barnett, R. Sinnott, G. Jayaputera, M. Burgman","doi":"10.1177/1555343420926287","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343420926287","url":null,"abstract":"How might analytic reasoning in intelligence reports be substantially improved? One conjecture is that this can be achieved through a combination of crowdsourcing and structured analytic techniques (SATs). To explore this conjecture, we developed a new crowdsourcing platform supporting groups in collaborative reasoning and intelligence report drafting using a novel SAT we call “Contending Analyses.” In this paper we present findings from a large study designed to assess whether groups of professional analysts working on the platform produce better-reasoned reports than those analysts produce when using methods and tools normally used in their organizations. Secondary questions were whether professional analysts working on the platform produce better reasoning than the general public working on the platform; and how usable the platform is. Our main finding is a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 1.37) in favor of working on platform. This provides early support for the general conjecture. We discuss limitations of our study, implications for intelligence organizations, and future directions for the work as a whole.","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"14 1","pages":"195 - 217"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343420926287","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43903704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1